Skip to main content
Log in

Observers’ expectations regarding the emotional reactions of others in a failure context: the role of status and perceived dominance

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

An important determinant of observer’s expectations about other’s emotional reactions is the status of the other person. Status can be derived from a variety of cues and in any given situation more than one status cue may be available. The present study showed that both information about another person’s organizational status and information about their level of social dominance based on verbal descriptions or facial appearance influenced the emotions that the person was expected to show. Two vignette studies were conducted to investigate the combined impact of these two sources of status information. When perceptions of dominance were manipulated through facial appearance as well as a verbal description, only dominance but not organizational status influenced anticipated emotional reactions to failure. When the only cue for dominance was facial appearance, both organizational status and appearance influenced anticipated emotions. In turn, anticipated emotional reactions predicted the expectation that the person would take responsibility for the failure or apologize, and these expectations influenced the degree to which observers recommended firing the person.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Most undergraduate students in Israel serve in the army prior to their university studies and some still work during their studies. Accordingly, most students have work experience within a highly hierarchical organization. This also means that most students start university not earlier than the age of 20, most much later after excursions and some years of work.

  2. We also measured the protagonist’s likelihood to experience fear, however, this variable will not be discussed in this context.

References

  • Adolphs, R. (2002). Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: Psychological and neurological mechanisms. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 1, 21–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, A. (2007). Face processing and empathy. Empathy in mental illness (pp. 360–385). New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243–267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M. (1992). On the fit of models to covariances and methodology. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 400–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J., Cohen, B. P., & Zelditch, M. (1972). Status characteristics and social interaction. American Sociological Review, 37, 241–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J., Norman, R. Z., & Zelditch, M., Jr. (1977). Status characteristics and social interaction. New York, NY: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, D. S. (1991). Accuracy in social perception: Contribution of facial and vocal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 298–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emotion. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bugental, D. B. (2000). Acquisition of the algorithms of social life: A domain-based approach. Psychological Bulletin, 26, 187–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chance, M. R. A. (1967). Attention structures as the basis of primate rank orders. Man, 2, 503–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, M., & Zebrowitz, L. A. (1995). The contributions of appearance to occupational outcomes in civilian and military settings. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 129–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, A. (1993). Sex differences in emotionality: Fact or stereotype? Feminism and Psychology, 3, 303–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of emotions. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gifford, R. (1991). Mapping nonverbal behavior on the interpersonal circle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 279–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, P. (1992). Depression: The evolution of powerlessness. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd., and New York: Guilford.

  • Gilbert, D., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 509–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halberstadt, A. G., & Saitta, M. B. (1987). Gender, nonverbal behavior, and perceived dominance: A test of the theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 257–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hareli, S., & Eisikovits, Z. (2006). The role of communicating social emotions accompanying apologies in forgiveness. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 189–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2010). What emotional reactions can tell us about the nature of others: An appraisal perspective on person perception. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 128–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hareli, S., Shomrat, N., & Biger, N. (2005). The role of emotions in employees’ explanations for failure in the workplace. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 20, 663–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harper, D. G. C. (1991). Communication. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioral ecology (pp. 374–397). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heise, D. R. (1979). Understanding events: Affect and the construction of social action. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E. (2004). Facial appearance, gender, and emotion expression. Emotion, 4, 378–388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., Adams, R., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Who may frown and who should smile? Dominance, affiliation, and the display of happiness and anger. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 515–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., Adams, R. B., Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2007). When two do the same it might not mean the same: The perception of emotional expressions shown by men and women. In U. Hess & P. Philippot (Eds.), Group dynamics and emotional expression (pp. 33–50). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., Adams, R. B., Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2009). The face is not an empty canvas: How facial expressions interact with facial appearance. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 3497–3504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hochschild, A. R. (1979). Emotion work, feeling rules, and social structure. American Journal of Sociology, 85, 551–575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G. (1991). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keating, C. F., & Doyle, J. (2002). The faces of desirable mates and dates contain mixed social status cues. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 414–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keating, C. F., Mazur, A., Segall, M. H., Cysneiros, P. G., Divale, W. T., Kilbride, J. E., et al. (1981). Culture and the perception of social dominance from facial expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 615–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, T. D. (1991). Predicting emotions from social relations. Social Psychology Quarterly, 54, 330–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, T. D., & Collins, R. (1990). Dimensions of microinteraction. American Journal of Sociology, 96, 32–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirouac, G., & Hess, U. (1999). Group membership and the decoding of nonverbal behavior. In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 182–212). Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods, 1, 130–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J., & Harper, D. G. C. (1988). The evolution of aggression: Can selection generate variability? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London, Series B, 319, 557–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazur, A. (1985). A biosocial model of status in face-to-face primate groups. Social Forces, 64, 377–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minear, M., & Park, D. C. (2004). A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 630–633.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, U., & Mazur, A. (1996). Facial dominance of West Point cadets as a predictor of later military rank. Social Forces, 74, 823–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parkinson, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1993). Making sense of emotion in stories and social life. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 295–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. L. (1987). Nonverbal behavior, dominance, and the basis of status in task groups. American Sociological Review, 52, 683–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. L. (2006). Expectation states theory and emotion. In J. E. Stets & J. H. Turner (Eds.), Handbook of sociology of emotions (pp. 347–367). New York: Springer Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Beliefs, situations, and their interactions: Towards a model of emotion reporting. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 934–960.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schlenker, B. R., & Weigold, M. F. (1992). Interpersonal processes involving impression regulation and management. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 133–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid Mast, M., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Who is the boss and who is not? Accuracy of judging status. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 145–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Senior, C., Phillips, M. L., Barnes, J., & David, A. S. (1999). An investigation into the perception of dominance from schematic faces: A study using the World-Wide Web. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 341–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith-Lovin, L., & Heise, D. R. (1988). Analyzing social interaction: Advances in affect control theory. New York: Gordon and Breach.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiedens, L. Z. (2001). Anger and advancement versus sadness and subjugation: The effect of negative emotion expressions on social status conferral. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 86–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tiedens, L. Z., Ellsworth, P. C., & Mesquita, B. (2000). Stereotypes of sentiments and status: Emotional expectations for high and low status group members. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 560–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vrij, A., & Semin, G. R. (1996). Lie experts’ beliefs about nonverbal indicators of deception. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20, 65–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 95, 548–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility: A foundation for a theory of social conduct. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiggins, J. S. (1979). A psychological taxonomy of trait descriptive terms: The interpersonal domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 395–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, P. R. (1968). Perceptual distortion of height as a function of ascribed academic status. Journal of Social Psychology, 74, 97–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shlomo Hareli.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hareli, S., Sharabi, M., Cossette, M. et al. Observers’ expectations regarding the emotional reactions of others in a failure context: the role of status and perceived dominance. Motiv Emot 35, 52–62 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9199-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9199-0

Keywords

Navigation