Skip to main content
Log in

The effect of humorous instructional materials on interest in a math task

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two studies tested the effect of humor, embedded in learning materials, on task interest. College student participants (N Study 1 = 359, N Study 2 = 172) learned a new math technique with the presence or absence of humor in the learning program and/or test instructions. Individual interest in math was measured initially and also tested as a factor. The results showed that the effect of humor in the learning program depended on individual interest in math. Humor raised task interest for those with low individual interest in math but slightly lowered task interest for those with high individual interest in math. Mediating variables of this effect were tested across both studies. Although the mediating variables showed inconsistency, humor may affect task interest through affective responses immediately following the instruction, rather than in subsequent interaction with the task.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research. Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bergin, D. A. (1999). Influences on classroom interest. Educational Psychologist, 34, 87–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berk, R. A., & Nanda, J. (2006). A randomized trial of humor effects on test anxiety and test performance. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 19, 425–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal and Curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1963). Complexity and incongruity variables as determinants of exploratory choice and evaluative ratings. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 17, 274–290.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1969). Laughter, humor and play. In G. Lindsey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (2nd ed.) (Vol. 3). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, J., Comisky, P. W., Crane, J. S., & Zillmann, D. (1980). Relationship between college teachers’ use of humor in the classroom and students’ evaluations of their teachers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 511–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, M. J., & Dunlap, W. P. (2002). Estimating inter-rater agreement using the average deviations (AD) index: A user’s guide. Organizational Research Methods, 5, 150–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cann, A., Calhoun, L. G., & Nance, J. T. (2000). Xposure to humor before and after an unpleasant stimulus: Humor as a preventative or a cure. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 13, 177–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., Pekrun, R., Haynes, T. L., Perry, R. P., & Newall, N. E. (2009). A longitudinal analysis of achievement goals: From affective antecedents to emotional effects and achievement outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 948–963.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deckers, L., & Devine, J. (1981). Humor by violating an existing expectancy. The Journal of Psychology, 108, 107–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dienstbier, R. A. (1995). The impact of humor on energy, tension, task choices, and attributions: Exploring hypotheses from toughness theory. Motivation and Emotion, 19, 255–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durik, A., & Harackiewicz, J. (2007). Different strokes for different folks: How individual interest moderates the effects of situational factors on task interest. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 597–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988). From appraisal to emotion: Differences among unpleasant feelings. Motivation and Emotion, 12, 271–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flansburg, S. (1996). Math magic. New York: Harper Perennial.

    Google Scholar 

  • Godkewitsch, M. (1972). The relationship between arousal potential and funniness of jokes. In J. H. Goldstein & P. E. McGhee (Eds.), The psychology of humor (pp. 143–158). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., Hall, N. C., & Ludtke, O. (2007). Between- and within-domain relations of students’ academic emotions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 4, 715–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, J. (1993). Humor and comedy in mass media. Medienpsychologie, 5, 246–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gorham, J., & Christopher, D. M. (1990). The relationship of teachers’ use of humor in the classroom to immediacy and student learning. Communication Education, 39, 46–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grisaffe, C., Blom, L. C., & Burke, K. L. (2003). The effects of head and assistant coaches’ uses of humor on collegiate soccer players’ evaluation of their coaches. Journal of Sport Behavior, 26, 103–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruner, C. R. (1967). Effects of humor on speaker ethos and audience information gain. Journal of Communication, 17, 228–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gruner, C. R., & Lampton, W. E. (1972). Effects of including humorous material in a persuasive sermon. Southern Speech Communication Journal, 38, 188–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., & Sansone, C. (1991). Goals and intrinsic motivation: You can get there from here. Advances in Motivation and Achievement, 7, 21–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivating the academically unmotivated: A critical issue for the 21st century. Review of Educational Research, 70, 151–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41, 111–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S., Renninger, K. A., & Krapp, A. (1992). The present state of interest research. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 433–446). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iran-Nejad, A. (1987). Cognitive and affective causes of interest and liking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 120–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, B. (2004). MediaLab (Version 2004.3.14) [Computer software and manual]. New York, NY: Empirisoft Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, R. M., & Pascoe, G. C. (1977). Humorous lectures and humorous examples: Some effects upon comprehension and retention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 61–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuppens, P., Van Mechelen, I., Smits, D. J. M., De Boeck, P., & Ceulemans, E. (2007). Individual differences in patterns of appraisal and anger experience. Cogntion and Emotion, 21, 689–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langevin, R., & Day, H. I. (1972). Physiological correlates of humor. In J. H. Goldstein & P. E. McGhee (Eds.), The psychology of humor (pp. 129–142). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., & Henderlong, J. (2000). Turning “play into work”, and “work” into “play”: 25 years of research on intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation. In C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 257–307). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Linfeld, E. G. (1977). The functions of humor in the classroom. In A. J. Chapman & H. C. Foot (Eds.), It’s a funny thing, humor. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malone, T. W., & Lepper, M. R. (1987). Making learning fun: A taxonomy of intrinsic motivations for learning. In R. E. Snow & M. J. Farr (Eds.), Aptitude, learning and instruction. Vol 3: Conative and affective process analyses (pp. 223–253). Hillsdale, NJ: Elrlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McMorris, R. F., Urbach, S. L., & Connor, M. C. (1985). Effects of incorporating humor in test items. Journal of Educational Measurement, 22, 147–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Middleton, J. A., & Spanias, P. A. (1999). Motivation for achievement in mathematics: Findings, generalizations, and criticisms of the research. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 65–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, M. (1993). Situational interest: Its multifaceted structure in the secondary school mathematics classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 424–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. New York: Cambridge Universuty Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2006). Achievement goals and discrete achievement emotions: A theoretical model and prospective test. Journal of Educational Psychology, 3, 583–597.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2009). Achievement goals and achievement emotions: Testing a model of their joint relations with academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 115–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poffenberger, T., & Norton, D. (1959). Factors in the formation of attitudes toward mathematics. Journal of Educational Research, 52, 171–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, J. P., & Andresen, L. W. (1985). Humour and teaching in higher education. Studies in Higher Education, 10, 79–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reeve, J. (1989). The interest-enjoyment distinction in intrinsic motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 13, 83–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renninger, K. A. (2000). Individual interest and its implications for understanding intrinsic motivation. In C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 373–404). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). “I don’t feel like it”: The function of interest in self-regulation. In L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions between goals and affect (pp. 203–227). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., & Smith, J. L. (2000). Interest and self-regulation: The relation between having to and wanting to. In C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 341–372). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, U. (1991). Interest, learning and motivation. Educational Psychologist, 26, 299–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, S. R. (1994). Effects of humor on sentence memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 953–967.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., & Lehman, S. (2001). Situational interest: A review of the literature and directions for future research. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 23–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzer, R., Seipp, B., & Schwarzer, C. (1989). Mathematics performance and anxiety: A meta-analysis. In R. Schwarzer, H. M. Van, C. D. der Ploeg, & Spielberger (Eds.), Advances in test anxiety research (Vol. 6, pp. 105–119). Lisse: Sweets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, T. M. (1976). Humor in teaching. Journal of Physical Education and Recreation, 7, 10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2003). Self-efficacy and interest: Experimental studies of optimal incompetence. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 62, 237–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2005). What is interesting? Exploring the appraisal structure of interest. Emotion, 5(1), 89–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. E., Ascough, J. C., Ettinger, R. F., & Nelson, D. A. (1971). Humor, anxiety, and test performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 19, 243–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 233–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, A. A., Schwartz, J. E., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., Krueger, A., & Kahneman, D. (2006). A population approach to the study of emotion: Diurnal rhythms of a working day examined with the day reconstruction method. Emotion, 6, 139–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strick, M., Holland, R. W., van Baaren, R. B., & van Knippenberg, A. (2009a). Finding comfort in a joke: Consolatory effects of humor through cognitive distraction. Emotion, 9, 574–578.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strick, M., van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., & van Knippenberg, A. (2009b). Humor in advertisements enhances product liking by mere association. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 35–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teslow, J. L. (1995). Humor me: A call for research. Educational Technology Research and Development, 43, 6–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teven, J. J., & McCroskey, J. C. (1997). The relationship of perceived teacher caring with student learning and teacher evaluation. Communication Education, 46, 167–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vance, C. M. (1987). A comparative study on the use of humor in the design of instruction. Instructional Science, 16, 79–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wanzer, M. B., & Frymier, A. B. (1999). The relationship between student perceptions of instructor humor and students’ reports of learning. Communication Education, 48, 48–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welker, W. A. (1977). Humor in education: A foundation for wholesome living. College Student Journal, 11, 252–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, G. W. (2001). Teachers’ reports of how they used humor with students’ perceived use of such humor. Education, 122, 337–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yovetich, N. A., Dale, J. A., & Hudak, M. A. (1990). Benefits of humor in reduction of threat-induced anxiety. Psychological Reports, 66, 51–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zeidner, M., & Matthews, G. (2008). Evaluation anxiety: Current theory and research. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 141–163). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zillmann, D., Williams, B. R., Bryant, J., Boynton, K. R., & Wolf, M. A. (1980). Acquisition of information from educational television programs as a function of differently paced humorous inserts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 170–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ziv, A. (1988). Teaching and learning with humor: Experiment and replication. Journal of Experimental Education, 57, 5–15.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank John Skowronski, Lisa Finkelstein, and several anonymous reviewers for their help with the development of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amanda M. Durik.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Manipulations in the program

Introduction to task

Non-humorous version

Even though calculators have been around for a long time, people were doing math long before they had help from these devices. What did people do without the ability to multiply large numbers at the press of a few buttons? Well, today, you are going to learn just how they might have done that. You’re going to learn mental math.

Humorous version

Try your best to imagine a time, long, long ago. You know, that time your parents are always referring to—when they had to walk 18 miles to work, uphill, during a blizzard just to earn 10 cents an hour. A time when life wasn’t so easy for us “crazy kids.” A time before computers, cell phones, and other electronic devices. A time with no video games (even before old-school Nintendo), no color TV, no email. A time WITHOUT the most important tool of all… insert dramatic music here… The Calculator. Gasp!

What did our ancestors do without the ability to multiply large numbers at the press of a few buttons? It is horrifying to imagine, but—believe it or not—they got by, and they multiplied their little hearts out. Today, you are going to learn just how they might have done that.

First word problem

Non-humorous version

Anna McDougall signed up for an online chat room. Since then, she has chatted for 14 hours each month for the last 12 months. How many hours has Anna spent chatting online?

Humorous version

Anita Man signed up for an online dating service. Since then, she has gone on 14 first-dates each month for the last 12 months, and is STILL single. How many men has Anita Man driven away with her charm?

Second word problem

Non-humorous version

For reasons we will not discuss here, it takes 23 professors to teach the classes in the Psychology Building. If each professor has 42 students, how many students are taking classes in the Psychology Building?

Humorous version

For reasons we will not discuss here, it takes 23 professors to screw in a lightbulb. If there are 42 lightbulbs in the psychology building, how many professors do you need to screw in them all?

Appendix 2: Manipulations in the test instructions

Non-humorous version

Using the new mental math technique that you’ve learned, you will have 3 minutes to complete as many of the problems as you can. Record your answers to each problem on the numbered answer sheet given to you by the experimenter. When the 3 minutes are up, you will be asked to stop. Begin when you are ready.

Humorous version

Now is the time that you have been waiting for! A chance to show your parents that you, too, could have walked uphill, 18 miles to work in a blizzard. A chance to show them that you could have survived without a calculator. Using the new mental math technique that you’ve learned, you will have 3 min to complete as many of the problems as you can. Record your answers to each problem on the numbered answer sheet given to you by the experimenter. When the 3 min are up, you will be asked to stop. Begin when you are ready to multiply your little heart out.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Matarazzo, K.L., Durik, A.M. & Delaney, M.L. The effect of humorous instructional materials on interest in a math task. Motiv Emot 34, 293–305 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9178-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9178-5

Keywords

Navigation