Skip to main content
Log in

Dysphoria and Mobilization of Mental Effort: Effects on Cardiovascular Reactivity

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two studies examined the influence of dysphoria on motivational intensity in a student sample. Participants worked on a memory task (Study 1) or a mental concentration task (Study 2) without fixed performance standard (“do your best”). Based on their scores on the Center for Epidemiologic Studies – Depression Scale (L. S. Radloff, 1977), dysphoric and nondysphoric students were compared with regard to their effort-related cardiovascular reactivity during task performance. As predicted on the basis of the mood-behavior-model (G. H. E. Gendolla, 2000) and motivational intensity theory (J. W. Brehm & E. A. Self, 1989), dysphoric participants showed stronger cardiovascular reactivity while working on the cognitive tasks than nondysphoric participants. In Study 1, nondysphoric participants performed better on the memory task than dysphoric participants. Theoretical implications are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Motivational intensity theory distinguishes between tasks with and without fixed performance standards, that is, with or without a certain performance level to attain. Tasks without fixed performance standards are labeled “unfixed” difficulty tasks; the difficulty level can be determined by the participants themselves who are simply asked to do their best (Brehm & Self, 1989).

  2. We also included the BDI-II (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) in the initial questionnaire session. Participants’ scores on the BDI-II were highly correlated with their CES-D scores, r(35) = .96, p<.001. Group assignment did not change when considering the BDI-II instead of the CES-D for the selection process.

  3. Degrees of freedom are adjusted because of inequality of variances.

  4. The reason for this is that repeated measures ANOVAs revealed a decline of cardiovascular values over the first measures, while the last two measures remained stable (ps>.30). This decline is a common finding and due to habituation to the experimental setting and the fact being seated for a while.

  5. Given our directed a priori hypothesis we conducted one-tailed t-tests for comparisons between dysphoric and nondysphoric groups with regard to their cardiovascular reactivity.

  6. As in Study 1, scores of the BDI-II and the CES-D were highly correlated at both assessment times, rs>.83, ps<.001.

  7. The reason for this is that repeated measures ANOVAs revealed a decline of cardiovascular values over the first measures, while the last three measures remained stable (ps>.16).

  8. The last three measures of HR baselines were stable (p>.50) but the internal consistency was insufficient (α=.44). Therefore, we used only the last of the eight measures as HR baseline. In addition, because of problems with the measurement equipment there were some missing HR data, so that HR analyses are based on the data of 19 participants.

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Alloy, L. B., & Rosoff, R. (1981). Depression and the generation of complex hypotheses in the judgment of contingency. Behvioural Research and Therapy, 19, 35–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the BDI-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W., & Self, E. A. (1989). The intensity of motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 109–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brickenkamp, R. (1981). Test d2. Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test [d2 mental concentration test] (7th edn.). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownley, K. A., Hurwitz, B. E., & Schneiderman, N. (2000). Cardiovascular psychophysiology. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd edn., pp. 224–264). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, D. B., Zembar, M. J., & Niederehe, G. (1995). Depression and memory impairment: A meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 285–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J., Pizzagalli, D., & Nitschke, J. B. (2002). The representation and regulation of emotion in depression: Perspectives from affective neuroscience. In I. H. Gotlib & C. L. Hammen (Eds.), Handbook of depression (pp. 219–244). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickson, J. M., & MacLeod, A. K. (2004). Approach and avoidance goals and plans: Their relationship to anxiety and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28, 415–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowles, D. C. (1994). A motivational theory of psychopathology. In W. D. Spaulding (Ed.), Integrative views of motivation, cognition, and emotion. Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 41, pp. 181–238). London, UK: University of Nebraska Press.

  • Frasure-Smith, N., & Lespérance, F. (2005). Depression and coronary heart disease: Complex synergism of mind, body, and environment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 39–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuhrer, R., & Rouillon, F. (1989). La version française de l’échelle CES-D (Center for epidemiologic studies – depression scale). Description et traduction de l’echelle d’autoevaluation. European Psychiatry, 4, 163–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E. (2000). On the impact of mood on behavior: an integrative theory and a review. Review of General Psychology, 4, 378–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., Abele, A. E., & Krüsken, J. (2001). The informational impact of mood on effort mobilization: A study of cardiovascular and electrodermal responses. Emotion, 1, 12–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Brinkmann, K. (2005). The role of mood states in self-regulation: Effects on action preferences and resource mobilization. European Psychologist, 10, 187–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Krüsken, J. (2002a). Mood, task demand, and effort-related cardiovascular response. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 577–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Krüsken, J. (2002b). The joint effect of informational mood impact and performance-contingent consequences on effort-related cardiovascular response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 271–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Krüsken, J. (2002c). Informational mood impact on effort-related cardiovascular response: The diagnostic value of mood counts. Emotion, 2, 251–262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Richter, M. (2005). The role of mood states in the development of cardiovascular disease: Implications of a motivational analysis of cardiovascular reactivity in active coping. In F. Columbus (Ed.), Advances in psychology research (Vol. 33, pp. 139–157). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.

  • Gilboa, E., & Gotlib, I. H. (1997). Cognitive biases and affect persistence in previously dysphoric and never-dysphoric individuals. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 517–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., Ranganath, C., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (1998). Frontal EEG alpha asymmetry, depression, and cognitive functioning. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 449–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Burling, J., & Tibbs, K. (1992). Depression, self-focused attention, and the self-serving attributional bias. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 959–965.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and action. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henriques, J. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2000). Decreased responsiveness to reward in depression. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 711–724.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T. (1998). Relation between rumination and impaired memory in dysphoric moods. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 166–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T. (2000). The cognitive-initiative account of depression-related impairments in memory. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 39, pp. 47–71). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

  • Hockey, G. R. J. (1997). Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload: A cognitive-energetical framework. Biological Psychology, 45, 73–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E., & Siegle, G. J. (2002). Contemporary methodological issues in the study of depression: Not your father’s Oldsmobile. In I. H. Gotlib & C. L. Hammen (Eds.), Handbook of depression (pp. 86–114). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Josephson, B. R., Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). Mood regulation and memory: Repairing sad moods with happy memories. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 437–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasch, K. L., Rottenberg, J., Arnow, B. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (2002). Behavioral activation and inhibition systems and the severity and course of depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 589–597.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kibler, J. L., & Ma, M. (2004). Depressive symptoms and cardiovascular reactivity to laboratory behavioral stress. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 11, 81–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Helle, P. (1986). Motivational and volitional determinants of depression: The degenerated-intention hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 247–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Light, K. C., Dolan, C. A., Davis, M. R., & Sherwood, A. (1992). Cardiovascular responses to an active coping challenge as predictors of blood pressure patterns 10 to 15 years later. Psychosomatic Medicine, 54, 217–230.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Llabre, M. M., Spitzer, S. B., Saab, P. G., Ironson, G. H., & Schneiderman, N. (1991). The reliability and specificity of delta versus residualized change as measure of cardiovascular reactivity to behavioral challenges. Psychophysiology, 28, 701–711.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting & task performance. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., Tucker, K. L., Caldwell, N. D., & Berg, K. (1999). Why ruminators are poor problem solvers: Clues from the phenomenology of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1041–1060.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Jones, D. M., & Chamberlain, A. G. (1990). Refining the measurement of mood: The UWIST mood adjective checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 259–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2001). Gender differences in depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 173–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obrist, P. A. (1981). Cardiovascular psychophysiology: A perspective. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papillo, J. F., & Shapiro, D. (1990). The cardiovascular system. In L. G. Tassinary & J. T. Cacioppo (Eds.), Principles of psychophysiology: Physical, social, and inferential elements (pp. 456–512). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinto-Meza, A., Caseras, X., Soler, J., Puigdemont, D., Pérez, V., & Torrubia, R. (2006). Behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation systems in current and recovered major depression participants. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 215, 226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration and the depressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of reactive depression. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 122–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, M., Gendolla, G. H. E., & Krüsken, J. (2006). Context-dependent mood effects on mental effort mobilization: A view from the mood-behavior-model. In A. V. Clark (Ed.), The psychology of moods (pp. 57–79). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, M. A., Kasai, K., Koji, M., Fukuda, R., Iwanami, A., Nakagome, K., Fukuda, M., & Kato, N. (2004). Executive and prefrontal dysfunction in unipolar depression: A review of neuropsychologial and imaging evidence. Neuroscience Research, 50, 1–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rugulies, R. (2002). Depression as a predictor for coronary heart disease: A review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 23, 51–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Santor, D. A., Zuroff, D. C., Ramsay, J. O., Cervantes, P., & Palacios, J. (1995). Examining scale discriminability in the BDI and the CES-D as a function of depressive severity. Psychological Assessment, 7, 131–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, W. D., & Ingram, R. E. (1998). Affective influences in depression: Conceptual issues, cognitive consequences, and multiple mechanisms. In W. F. Flack & J. D. Laird (Eds.), Emotions in psychopathology: Theory and research (pp. 200–215). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherry, S. B., Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., & Harvey, M. (2003). Perfectionism dimensions, perfectionistic attitudes, dependent attitudes, and depression in psychiatric patients and university students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50, 373–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. W., Allred, K. D., Morrison, C. A., & Carlson, S. D. (1989). Cardiovascular reactivity and interpersonal influence: Active coping in a social setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 209–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strauman, T. J. (2002). Self-regulation and depression. Self and Identity, 1, 151–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suls, J., & Bunde, J. (2005). Anger, anxiety, and depression as risk factors for cardiovascular disease: The problems and implications of overlapping affective dispositions. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 260–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tillema, J. L., Cervone, D., & Scott, W. D. (2001). Negative mood, perceived self-efficacy, and personal standards in dysphoria: The effects of contextual cues on self-defeating patterns of cognition. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 535–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomarken, A. J., & Keener, A. D. (1998). Frontal brain asymmetry and depression: A self-regulatory perspective. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 387–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vredenburg, K., Flett, G. L., & Krames, L. (1993). Analogue versus clinical depression: A critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 327–344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E., & Brown, R. G. (2002). Rumination and executive function in depression: An experimental study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 72, 400–402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A. (1996). Brehm’s theory of motivation as a model of effort and cardiovascular response. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 424–453). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A. (1998). Ability perception and cardiovascular response to behavioral challenge. In M. Kofta, G. Weary, & G. Sedek (Eds.), Personal control in action: Cognitive and motivational mechanisms (pp. 197–232). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A., & Dill, J. C. (1993). Blood pressure responses and incentive appraisals as a function of perceived ability and objective task demand. Psychophysiology, 30, 152–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2001). Effort determination of cardiovascular response: An integrative analysis with applications in social psychology. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 33, pp. 255–307). New York: Academic Press.

  • Wright, R. A., Murray, J. B., Storey, P. L., & Williams, B. J. (1997). Ability analysis of gender relevance and sex differences in cardiovascular response to behavioral challenge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 205–217.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was facilitated by a research grant form the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Ge 987/7-1) awarded to the second author. Portions of the present research were presented at the 45th Annual Meeting of the Society for Psychophysiological Research, Lisbon, Portugal, September 21--25, 2005. We are grateful to Michael Richter and some anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier version of this article and to Alexandra Russell and Annick Tinembart for their help as hired experimenters.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kerstin Brinkmann.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Brinkmann, K., Gendolla, G.H.E. Dysphoria and Mobilization of Mental Effort: Effects on Cardiovascular Reactivity. Motiv Emot 31, 71–82 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007-9054-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007-9054-0

Keywords

Navigation