Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Brand extensions via complements or substitutes: The moderating role of manufacturing transferability

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This research investigates how consumer evaluations of brand extensions that either complement or substitute the original parent brand vary depending on the level of manufacturing transferability (i.e., the extent to which the parent brand’s existing resources and skills can be used to make the extension). We propose that a complement extension is processed by consumers at a higher, more abstract level whereas a substitute extension is processed at a lower, more concrete level. Since manufacturing transferability activates concrete cognitions of the production process, an increase in manufacturing transferability tends to result in more favorable evaluations toward substitute extensions than complement extensions. Empirical tests using a multi-method approach reveal support both for the underlying theoretical mechanism and the proposed hypotheses.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aaker, D. A., & Keller, K. L. (1990). Consumer evaluations of brand extensions. Journal of Marketing, 54, 27–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bottomley, P. A., & Holden, S. J. S. (2001). Do we really know how consumers evaluate brand extensions? Empirical generalizations based on secondary analysis of eight studies. Journal of Marketing Research, 38, 494–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caramelli, N., Setti, A., & Maurizzi, D. D. (2004). Concrete and abstract concepts in school age children. Psychology of Language and Communication, 8(2), 19–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Echambadi, R., Arroniz, I., Reinartz, W., & Lee, J. (2006). Empirical generalizations from brand extension research: How sure are we? International Journal of Research in Marketing, 23(3), 253–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huffman, C., & Houston, M. J. (1993). Goal oriented experiences and the development of knowledge. Journal of Consumer Research, 20, 190–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, H., & John, D. R. (2008). Consumer responses to brand extension: construal level as a moderator of the importance of perceived fit. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18, 116–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 5–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, E. L., & Murphy, G. L. (2001). Thematic relations in adults' concepts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 130, 3–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, I. M., & Stewart, D. W. (2001). The differential impact of goal congruency on attitudes, intentions, and the transfer of brand equity. Journal of Marketing Research, 38(4), 471–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monga, A. B., & John, D. R. (2007). Cultural difference in brand extension evaluations: The influence of analytic versus holistic thinking. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(4), 529–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muller, D., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2005). When moderation is mediated and mediation is moderated. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(6), 852–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, C. W., Jun, S. Y., & Shocker, A. D. (1996). Composite branding alliances: An investigation of extension and feedback effects. Journal of Marketing Research, 33(4), 453–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shine, B. C., Park, J., & Wyer, R. S. (2007). Brand synergy effects in multiple brand extensions. Journal of Marketing Research, 44(4), 663–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 3–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walters, R. G. (1991). Assessing the impact of retail price promotions on product substitution, complementary purchase, and interstore sales displacement. Journal of Marketing, 55, 17–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Raj Echambadi.

Appendix

Appendix

From Eq. 1in the paper, the total impact of consumption-based fit can be re-arranged as:

$$ {\beta_3}(C) + {\beta_4}(S) + {\beta_7}\left( {Q*C} \right) + {\beta_8}\left( {Q*S} \right) + {\beta_9}\left( {T*C} \right) + {\beta_{{1}0}}\left( {T*S} \right) $$
(A.1)

At mean parent quality, i.e., Q = 0 (data are mean-centered), and re-arranging (A.1), we get:

$$ \left( {{\beta_3} + {\beta_9}*T} \right){\text{C}} + \left( {{\beta_4} + {\beta_{{1}0}}*T} \right)S $$
(A.2)

Let C H (C L) represent high (low) levels of complementarity, and S H (S L) represents high (low) levels of substitutability. For complement extensions (C H = 1, S L = 1), impact of consumption-based fit can be represented as follows:

$$ \left( {{\beta_3} + {\beta_9}*T} \right)\left( {{C_{\text{H}}}} \right) + \left( {{\beta_{{4} }} + {\beta_{{1}0}}*T} \right){S_{\text{L}}} $$
(A.3)

Similarly, the impact of fit when the extension is a substitute (C L = 1, S H = 1) is given by:

$$ \left( {{\beta_3} + {\beta_9}*T} \right)\left( {{C_{\text{L}}}} \right) + \left( {{\beta_4} + {\beta_{{1}0}}*T} \right){S_{\text{H}}} $$
(A.4)

The difference between the impact of substitutes and complements is obtained by subtracting (A.4) from (A.3). At high levels of transferability (T H = 1), this difference is given by:

$$ \left( {{\beta_3} + {\beta_9}*{T_{\text{H}}}} \right)\left( {{C_{\text{L}}}-{C_{\text{H}}}} \right) + \left( {{\beta_4} + {\beta_{{1}0}}*{T_{\text{H}}}} \right)\left( {{S_{\text{H}}} - {S_{\text{L}}}} \right) $$
(A.5)

Similarly, at low levels of manufacturing transferability, the difference can be expressed as:

$$ \left( {{\beta_3} + {\beta_9}*{T_{\text{L}}}} \right)\left( {{C_{\text{L}}}-{C_{\text{H}}}} \right) + \left( {{\beta_4} + {\beta_{{1}0}}*{T_{\text{L}}}} \right)\left( {{S_{\text{H}}} - {S_{\text{L}}}} \right) $$
(A.6)

Substituting the values of β 3, β 4, β 9, and β 10 from Table 1, and setting T H = 2.99 [1.5*Standard Deviation of transferability (1.99)], we can see that the latter term in (A.5) as expressed by (β 4 + β 10*T H) (S H − S L) is always positive and (β 3 + β9*T H)(C LC H) ≥ 0. Therefore, (A.5) is always ≥0. Hence, we conclude that the difference between the impact of substitute and complement products at high levels of transferability is always positive. Substituting the values of β 3, β 4, β 9, and β 10 from Table 1, setting T L = −2.99 and testing whether (A.6) = 0, we find that (β 3 + β 9*T L)(C LC H) + (β 4 + β 10*T L) (S H − S L) is not different from zero. Hence, it is clear there is no difference between the impact of substitutes and complements at low levels of transferability.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mao, H., John Mariadoss, B., Echambadi, R. et al. Brand extensions via complements or substitutes: The moderating role of manufacturing transferability. Mark Lett 23, 279–292 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-011-9153-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-011-9153-2

Keywords

Navigation