Skip to main content
Log in

How buyers forecast: Buyer–seller relationship as a boundary condition of the impact bias

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

An Erratum to this article was published on 13 April 2011

Abstract

People frequently overestimate the impact of an event when they imagine it, relative to when they actually experience it. This phenomenon, known as the impact bias, has been well established as an intrapersonal phenomenon. We extend it to the inherently interpersonal marketing setting involving buyer–seller dyads in which the two entities are bound by a relationship, and propose a moderating effect based on the strength of the buyer–seller relationship. An online panel study and three laboratory experiments provide converging evidence such that the impact bias emerges when the buyer–seller relationship is strong, but disappears when the relationship is weak.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aaker, J., Fournier, S., & Brasel, A. S. (2004). When good brands do bad. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aggarwal, P. (2004). The effects of brand relationship norms on consumer attitudes and behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 87–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. A. (1983). Imagination and expectation: the effect of imagining behavioral scripts on personal intentions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 293–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bagozzi, R. (1986). Principles of marketing management. Chicago: Science Research Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernoff, J., & Li, C. (2008). Harnessing the power of the oh-so-social web. MIT Sloan Management Review, 49, 36–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, B. P., & Knowles, E. S. (1999). A disrupt-then-reframe technique of social influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 192–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diekmann, K. A., Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Galinsky, A. D. (2003). From self-prediction to self-defeat: behavioral forecasting, self-fulfilling prophecies, and the effect of competitive expectations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 672–683.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, E. W., Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Location, location, location: the misprediction of satisfaction in housing lotteries. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1421–1432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ebert, J. E. J., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2009). Forecasting and backcasting: predicting the impact of events on the future. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 353–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fournier, S. (1998). Consumers and their brands: developing relationship theory in consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 343–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, S.(2000). Trust and Privacy Online. http://www.pewInternet.org/PPF/r/19/report_display.asp, accessed March 28, 2008.

  • Frederic, S., & Loewenstein, G. (1999). Hedonic adaptation. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Scientific perspectives on enjoyment, suffering and well being. New York: Russell Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., Driver-Linn, E., & Wilson, T. D. (2002). The trouble with Vronsky: impact bias in the forecasting of future affective states. In L. F. Barrett & P. Salovey (Eds.), The wisdom of feeling (pp. 114–143). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., & Ebert, J. E. J. (2002). Decisions and revisions: the affective forecasting of changeable outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 503–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., Gill, M. J., & Wilson, T. D. (2002). The future is now: temporal correction in affective forecasting. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88, 430–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., Lieberman, M. D., Morewedege, C. K., & Wilson, T. D. (2004). The peculiar longevity of things not so bad. Psychological Science, 15, 14–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: a source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirt, E. R., Kardes, F. R., & Markman, K. D. (2004). Activating a mental simulation mind-set through generation of alternatives: implications for debiasing in related and unrelated domains. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 374–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. K. (2006). Memory and reality. American Psychologist, 61, 760–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kardes, F. R., Fennis, B. M., Hirt, E. R., Tormala, Z. L., & Bullington, B. (2007). The role of the need for cognitive closure in the effectiveness of the disrupt-then-reframe influence technique. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 377–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawakami, K., Dunn, E., Karmali, F., & Dovidio, J. F. (2009). Mispredicting affective and behavioral responses to racism. Science, 323, 276–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koehler, D. J. (1991). Explanation, imagination, and confidence in judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 110(3), 499–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G., O’Donoghue, T., & Rabin, M. (2003). Projection bias in predicting future utility. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118, 1209–1248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacInnis, D. J., Patrick, V. M., & Whan Park, C. (2005). Looking through the crystal ball: the role of affective forecasting and misforecasting in consumer behavior. Review of Marketing Research, 2, 43–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. The American Psychologist, 56, 239–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monga, A., & Rao, A. R. (2006). Domain-based asymmetry in expectations of the future. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100, 35–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • New York Times (2009). Video prank at domino’s taints brand. Accessible at: http://www.nytimes.com/2009/04/16/business/media/16dominos.html

  • Patrick, V. M., MacInnis, D. J., & Park, C. W. (2007). Not as happy as I thought I’d be: affective misforecasting and product evaluations. Journal of Consumer Research, 33, 479–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollai, M., Hoelzl, E., & Possas, F. (2010). Consumption-relation emotions over time: fit between prediction and experience. Marketing Letters, 21, 397–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratner, R. K., Soman, D., Zauberman, G., Ariely, D., Carmon, Z., Keller, P. A., et al. (2008). How behavioral decision research can enhance consumer welfare: from freedom of choice to paternalistic intervention. Marketing Letters, 19(3–4), 383–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanna, L. J., & Schwarz, N. (2004). Integrating temporal biases: the interplay of focal thoughts and accessibility experiences. Psychological Science, 15(7), 474–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, J. C., & Ostrom, A. L. (2006). Complaining to the masses: the role of protest framing in customer-created complaint web sites. Journal of Consumer Research, 33, 220–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806–820.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting: knowing what to want. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 131–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Centerbar, D. B., Kermer, D. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). The pleasures of uncertainty: prolonging positive moods in ways people do not anticipate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 5–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Windschitl, P. D., Smith, A. R., Rose, J. P., & Krizan, Z. (2010). The desirability bias in predictions: going optimistic without leaving realism. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Making Processes, 111, 33–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ashwani Monga.

Additional information

An erratum to this article can be found at http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11002-011-9139-0.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Monga, A., (Allan) Chen, H., Tsiros, M. et al. How buyers forecast: Buyer–seller relationship as a boundary condition of the impact bias. Mark Lett 23, 31–45 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-010-9133-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-010-9133-y

Keywords

Navigation