Skip to main content
Log in

How behavioral decision research can enhance consumer welfare: From freedom of choice to paternalistic intervention

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Decision-making researchers have largely focused on showing errors and biases in consumers' decision-making processes without paying much attention to the social welfare and policy implications of these systematic behaviors. In this paper, we explore how findings and methods in behavioral decision research can be used to help consumers improve their decision making and enhance their well-being. We first review select findings in behavioral decision research to explain why consumers need help in decisions, and based on these findings, suggest various interventions that could be effective within the scope of libertarian paternalism. Ethics and effectiveness of the interventions are also discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ariely, D., & Wertenbroch, K. (2002). Procrastination, deadlines, and performance: self-control by precommitment. Psychological Science, 13(3), 219–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashraf, N., Berry, J., & Shapiro, J. M. (2007). Can higher prices stimulate product use? Evidence from a field experiment in Zambia. Working paper, Harvard Business School.

  • Ashraf, N., Karlan, D., & Yin, W. (2006). Tying Odysseus to the mast: evidence from a commitment savings product in the Philippines. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121(2), 635–672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buehler, R., & McFarland, C. (2001). Intensity bias in affective forecasting: the role of temporal focus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(11), 1480–1493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carmon, Z., Wertenbroch, K., & Zeelenberg, M. (2003). Option attachment: when deliberating makes choosing feel like losing. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(1), 15–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheema, A., & Soman, D. (2007). The effect of partitions on constraining consumption. Journal of Marketing Research, forthcoming.

  • Coelho do Vale, R., Pieters, R., & Zeelenberg, M. (2007). Sneaky small sins flying under the radar: package sizes and consumption self-regulation. Working paper, Tilburg University.

  • Crum, A. J., & Langer, E. J. (2007). Mind-set matters: exercise and the placebo effect. Psychological Science, 18(2), 165–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dhar, R. (1997). Consumer preference for a no-choice option. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(2), 215–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., & van Baaren, R. B. (2006). On making the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science, 311, 1005–1007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, C. R., Ratner, R. K., & Lieb, D. S. (2005). How subjective grouping of options influences choice and allocation: diversification bias and the phenomenon of partition dependence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(4), 538–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaba, A., Hillion, P., & Wertenbroch, K. (2005). Investor decision making in Hong Kong, Singapore, and Taiwan: a behavioral survey for Citigroup. Technical report. Singapore: INSEAD Asia Campus.

  • Gilbert, D. T., & Ebert, J. E. (2002). Decisions and revisions: the affective forecasting of changeable outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(4), 503–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: a source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 617–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions—strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54(7), 493–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Ratajczak, H. (1990). From weighing to willing: approaching a change decision through pre- or postdecisional mentation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 45(1), 41–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gourville, J., & Soman, D. (2005). Overchoice and assortment type: when and why variety backfires. Marketing Science, 24(3), 382–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J., & Prelec, D. (1991). Melioration: a theory of distributed choice. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5(3), 137–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huber, J., Payne, J. W., & Puto, C. (1982). Adding asymmetrically dominated alternatives: violations of regularity and the similarity hypothesis. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(1), 90–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is demotivating: can one desire too much of a good thing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 995–1006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones, et al. (Ed.), Attribution: Perceiving the Causes of Behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R. C. (1987). Perspectives on queues: social justice and the psychology of queueing. Operations Research, 35(6), 895–905.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: a test of temporal construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 5–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65(3), 272–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G., & Small, D. A. (2007). The scarecrow and the tin man: the vicissitudes of human sympathy and caring. Review of General Psychology, 11(2), 112–126 (Special Issue on Emotion and Decision Making).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lusardi, Annamaria, Keller, Punam A., and Adam M. Keller (2007). New ways to make people save: a social marketing approach. Overcoming the saving slump: how to improve the effectiveness of financial education and saving programs. Book manuscript, University of Chicago Press.

  • Lynch, J., & Zauberman, G. (2006). When do you want it? Time, decisions, and public policy. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 25(1), 67–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madrian, B. C., & Shea, D. F. (2001). The power of suggestion: inertia in 401(k) participation and savings behavior. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(4), 1149–1187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malkoc, S., & Zauberman, G. (2006). Deferring versus expediting consumption: the effect of outcome concreteness on sensitivity to time horizon. Journal of Marketing Research, 43(4), 618–627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malkoc, S., Zauberman, G., & Bettman, J. R. (2007). It is in the mindset the effect of processing concreteness on consumer impatience. Working paper, Carlson School of Management, University of Minnesota.

  • Malkoc, S., Zauberman, G., & Ulu, C. (2005). Consuming now or later? The interactive effect of timing and attribute alignability. Psychological Science, 16(5), 411–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106(1), 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyvis, T., Ratner, R. K., & Levav, J. (2008). Why people don't learn to accurately forecast their feelings: how misremembering their predictions blinds people to their past forecasting errors. Working Paper.

  • O'Donoghue, T., & Rabin, M. M. (1999). Doing it now or later. American Economic Review, 89(1), 103–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Read, D., & Loewenstein, G. (1995). The diversification bias: explaining the difference between prospective and real-time taste for variety. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 1(1), 34–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, M. L., Nowlis, S. M., Mandel, N., & Morales, A. C. (2007). Why do diet foods make consumers heavier? The effect of reduced calorie packages on the consumption behaviors of dieters and non-dieters. Working paper, Arizona State University.

  • Sethi-Iyengar, S., Huberman, G., & Jiang, W. (2004). How much choice is too much? Contributions to 401(k) retirement plans. In O. S. Mitchell, & S. Utkus (Eds.) Pension Design and Structure: New Lessons from Behavioral Finance, 83–95. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiv, B., Carmon, Z., & Ariely, D. (2005a). Placebo effects of marketing actions: consumers may get what they pay for. Journal of Marketing Research, 42(4), 383–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiv, B., Carmon, Z., & Ariely, D. (2005b). Ruminating about placebo effects of marketing actions. Journal of Marketing Research, 42(4), 410–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonson, I. (1989). Choice based on reasons: the case of attraction and compromise effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(2), 158–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonson, I. (1990). The effect of purchase quantity and timing on variety-seeking behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, 27(2), 150–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Small, D. A., & Loewenstein, G. (2003). Helping “THE” victim or helping “A” victim: altruism and identifiability. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 26(1), 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Small, D. A., Loewenstein, G., & Slovic, P. (2007). Sympathy and callousness: the impact of deliberate thought on donations to identifiable and statistical victims. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102(2), 143–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Small, D. A., & Simonsohn, U. (2007). Friends of victims: personal experience and prosocial behavior. Conditionally accepted at Journal of Consumer Research.

  • Small, D. A., & Verrochi, N. M. (2007). The face of need: emotion expression on charity advertisements. Working paper, Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania.

  • Soman, D. (1998). The illusion of delayed incentives: evaluating future effort-money transactions. Journal of Marketing Research, 35(4), 427–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soman, D. (2001). Effects of payment mechanism on spending behavior: the role of rehearsal and immediacy of payments. Journal of Consumer Research, 27(4), 460–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soman, D. (2007). Behavioural economics in the field: improving prudence through field experiments. Rotman School of Management, Toronto, University of Toronto.

  • Thaler, R. H., & Benartzi, S. (2004). Save more tomorrow: using behavioral economics to increase employee saving. Journal of Political Economy, 112(1), 164–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2003). Behavioral economics, public policy, and paternalism: libertarian paternalism. The American Economic Review, 93(2), 175–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waber, R., Shiv, B., Carmon, C., & Ariely, D. (2008). Commercial features of placebo and therapuetic efficacy. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association, 299(9), 1016–1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wansink, B. (1996). Can package size accelerate usage volume? Journal of Marketing, 60(3), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertenbroch, K. (1998). Consumption self-control by rationing purchase quantities of virtue and vice. Marketing Science, 17(4), 317–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertenbroch, K., Vosgerau, J., & Bruyneel, S. D. (2008). Free will, temptation, and self-control: we must believe in free will, we have no choice. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18, 27–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). Thinking too much: introspection can reduce the quality of preference and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(2), 181–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zauberman, G. (2003). The intertemporal dynamics of consumer lock-in. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(3), 405–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zauberman, G., & Lynch, J. G. (2005). Resource slack and propensity to discount delayed investments of time versus money. Journal of Experiment Psychology. General, 134(1), 23–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, M., Hoeffler, S., & Zauberman, G. (2007). Mental simulation and preference consistency over time: the role of process- versus outcome-focused thoughts. Journal of Marketing Research, 44(2), 379–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rebecca K. Ratner.

Additional information

This paper draws on discussions in a session “Helping Consumers Help Themselves through Choice” at the Invitational Choice Symposium in June 2007 co-chaired by Rebecca Ratner, Dilip Soman, and Gal Zauberman.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ratner, R.K., Soman, D., Zauberman, G. et al. How behavioral decision research can enhance consumer welfare: From freedom of choice to paternalistic intervention. Mark Lett 19, 383–397 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-008-9044-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-008-9044-3

Keywords

Navigation