Skip to main content
Log in

The Effectiveness of Opposing Expert Witnesses for Educating Jurors about Unreliable Expert Evidence

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Law and Human Behavior

Abstract

We tested whether an opposing expert is an effective method of educating jurors about scientific validity by manipulating the methodological quality of defense expert testimony and the type of opposing prosecution expert testimony (none, standard, addresses the other expert’s methodology) within the context of a written trial transcript. The presence of opposing expert testimony caused jurors to be skeptical of all expert testimony rather than sensitizing them to flaws in the other expert’s testimony. Jurors rendered more guilty verdicts when they heard opposing expert testimony than when opposing expert testimony was absent, regardless of whether the opposing testimony addressed the methodology of the original expert or the validity of the original expert’s testimony. Thus, contrary to the assumptions in the Supreme Court’s decision in Daubert, opposing expert testimony may not be an effective safeguard against junk science in the courtroom.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Despite the general tendency of expert testimony to influence juror judgments, some studies find that the effects of expert testimony may occur only under certain circumstances (Devenport and Cutler 2004; Leippe et al. 2004), such as when the expert testimony appeared after the evidence (Leippe et al. 2004), whereas other studies find effects for expert testimony only when it appears before the remaining evidence (Brekke and Borgida 1988; Schuller and Cripps 1998).

  2. We also manipulated the credibility of the defense expert, but despite successful pilot testing with a student sample, the manipulation failed in the data collected from community members. The credibility manipulation did not interact with our other independent variables to affect our dependent measures so we removed the credibility manipulation from the analyses.

  3. Collecting data using the internet versus traditional survey methods generally results in a lower response rate; however, several studies have demonstrated that results from internet research yield very similar results to laboratory-based research (Birnbaum 2004; Krantz and Dahal 2000). In this study, participants were sent e-mail invitations and reminders to participate in the study. In one study investigating such methods, over one-third of non-respondents did not respond to the invitation because they did not read their e-mail during the data collection period (Welker 2001, reported in Birnbaum 2004).

References

  • Barlow v. State, (1998). 270 Ga 54.

  • Birnbaum, M. H. (2004). Human research and data collection via the internet. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 803–832.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brekke, N., & Borgida, E. (1988). Expert psychological testimony in rape trials: A social-cognitive analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 372–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M., & Ceci, S. J. (1995). Amicus brief for the case of State of New Jersey v. Michaels presented by committee of concerned social scientists. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 1(2), 272–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cecil, J. S., & Willging, T. E. (1993). Court appointed experts: Defining the role of experts appointed under Federal Rule of Evidence 706. Washington, DC: Federal Judicial Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. (1989). Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 212–251). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J., Bennett, E. A., & Sukel, H. L. (1996). Complex scientific testimony: How do jurors make decisions? Law and Human Behavior, 20(4), 379–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J., & Neuhaus I. (2000). The hired gun effect: Use of pay, credentials, and frequency of testifying as peripheral decisions. Law and Human Behavior, 24, 149–171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, B. L., & Penrod, S. D. (1995). Mistaken identification: The eyewitness, psychology, and the law. New York: Cambridge University Press.

  • Cutler, B. L., Dexter, H. R., & Penrod, S. D. (1990a). Nonadversarial methods for sensitizing jurors to eyewitness evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 20, 1197–1207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, B. L., Penrod, S. D., & Dexter, H. R. (1990b). Juror sensitivity to eyewitness identification evidence. Law and Human Behavior, 14(2), 185–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc. (1993). 113 S. Ct. 2786.

  • Devenport, J. L., & Cutler, B. L. (2004). Impact of defense-only and opposing eyewitness experts on juror judgments. Law and Human Behavior, 28, 569–576.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeWitt, J. S., Richardson, J. T., & Warner, L. G. (1997). Novel scientific evidence and controversial cases: A social psychological examination. Law and Psychology Review, 21, 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frye v. United States, (1923). 54 App.D. C. 46, 293 F.1013.

  • General Electric Co. v. Joiner, (1997). 522 U. S. 136.

  • Gilovich, T., Vallone, R., & Tversky, A. (1985). The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 295–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, E., Downey, C., & Goodman-Delahunty, J. (1999). Juror decisions about damages in employment discrimination cases. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 17(1), 107–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffith, J. D., Libkuman, T. M., & Poole, D. A. (1998). Repressed memories: The effects of expert testimony on mock jurors’ decision making. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 16(1), 5–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groscup, J., & Penrod, S. D. (2002). Jury instructions effects on juror assessments of scientific reliability. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the American Psychology-Law Society, San Antonio, TX.

  • Hooper, L. L., Cecil, J. S., & Willging, T. E. (2001). Assessing causation in breast implant litigation: The role of science panels. Law and Contemporary Problems, 64, 139–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jasanoff, S. (1993). What judges should know about the sociology of science. Judicature, 77(2), 77–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasanoff, S. (1995). Science at the bar: Law, science, and technology in America. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kovera, M. B., Levy, R. J., Borgida, E., & Penrod, S. D. (1994). Expert testimony in child sexual abuse cases: Effects of expert evidence type and cross examination. Law and Human Behavior, 18, 653–674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kovera, M. B., & McAuliff, B. D. (2000). The effects of peer review and evidence quality on judge evaluations of psychological science: Are judges effective gatekeepers? Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(4), 574–586.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovera, M. B., McAuliff, B. D., & Hebert, K. S. (1999). Reasoning about scientific evidence: Effects of juror gender and evidence quality on juror decisions in a hostile work environment case. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(3), 362–375.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krantz, J. H., & Dalal, R. (2000). Validity of web-based psychological research. In M. H. Birnbaum (Ed.), Psychological experiments on the internet (pp. 35–60). San Diego: Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kumho Tire Co. v. Carmichael, (1999). 526 U.S. 137.

  • Leichtman, M. D., & Ceci, S. J. (1995). The effects of stereotypes and suggestions on preschoolers’ reports. Developmental Psychology, 31, 568–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leippe, M. R. (1995). The case for expert testimony about eyewitness memory. Psychology, Public Policy and Law, 1, 909–959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leippe, M. R., Eisenstadt, D., Rauch, S. M., & Seib, H. M. (2004). Timing of eyewitness expert testimony, jurors’ need for cognition, and case strength as determinants of trial verdict. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 524–541.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lehman, D. R., Lempert, R. O., & Nisbett, R. E. (1988). The effects of graduate training on reasoning: Formal discipline and thinking about everyday-life events. American Psychologist, 43(6), 431–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, T. D. (1999). The new wave in children’s suggestibility research: A critique. Cornell Law Review, 84, 1004–1087.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAuliff, B. D., & Kovera, M. B. (in press-a). Estimating the effects of misleading information on witness accuracy: Can experts tell jurors something they don’t already know? Applied Cognitive Psychology.

  • McAuliff, B. D., & Kovera, M. B. (in press-b). Juror sensitivity to methodological flaws in psychological science. Journal of Applied Social Psychology.

  • McAuliff, B. D., Kovera, M. B., & Viswesvaran, C. (1998). Methodological issues in child suggestibility research. In B. L. Bottoms & J. A. Quas (Co-Chairs), Situational and individual sources of variability in children’s suggestibility and false memories. Symposium presented at the meeting of the American Psychology-Law Society, Redondo Beach, CA.

  • McAuliff, B. D., Nicholson, E., & Ravanshenas, D. (2007). Hypothetically speaking, can expert testimony improve jurors’ understanding of developmental differences in suggestibility? In B. D. McAuliff & D. Ravanshenas (Co-Chairs), Giving away developmental psychology in court: Can expert testimony on suggestibility and child abuse improve jurors’ decisions? Symposium presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, MA.

  • McAuliff, B. D., Samis, A. Vargas, J., & Gonzalez, K. (2006). May we suggest a hypothetical? Expert testimony in witness suggestibility cases. In L. Levett (Chair), Juror decision making about expert evidence. Symposium presented at the meeting of the American Psychology-Law Society, St. Petersburg, FL.

  • Nisbett, R. E. (1993). Rules for reasoning. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavel v. Hollins, (2001). 261 F. 3d 210.

  • Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123–203). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1998) Attitude change: Multiple roles for persuasion variables. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The Handbook of social psychology (pp. 323–390). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raitz, A., Greene, E., Goodman, J., & Loftus, E. F. (1990). Determining damages: The influence of expert testimony on jurors’ decision making. Law and Human Behavior, 14(4), 385–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schuller, R. A., & Cripps, J. (1998). Expert evidence pertaining to battered women: The impact of gender of expert and timing of testimony. Law and Human Behavior, 22, 17–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • State of Ohio v. Gersin, (1996). 76 Ohio St.3d 491.

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1971). Belief in the law of small numbers. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 105–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welker, M. (2001). Email surveys: Non-response figures reflected. In U. D. Reips, & M. Bosnjak (Eds.), Dimensions of internet science (pp. 231–238). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wingate, P. H., & Thornton, G. C. III (2004). Industrial/organizational psychology and the federal judiciary: Expert witness testimony and the Daubert standards. Law and Human Behavior, 28, 97–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was conducted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the first author’s master’s degree at Florida International University, under the supervision of the second author, and was supported in part by a grant-in-aid from the American Psychology-Law Society. Portions of this research were presented at the 2004 joint meeting of the American Psychology-Law Society and the European Association of Psychology and Law, Edinburgh, Scotland. We are grateful for the helpful suggestions of committee members Aya Gruber and Janat Parker.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lora M. Levett.

About this article

Cite this article

Levett, L.M., Kovera, M.B. The Effectiveness of Opposing Expert Witnesses for Educating Jurors about Unreliable Expert Evidence. Law Hum Behav 32, 363–374 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-007-9113-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-007-9113-9

Keywords

Navigation