Mean levels and standard deviations of depressive symptoms, support, conflict and power imbalance as reported by both siblings are presented in Table 1. In general, both siblings reported relatively low levels of depressive symptoms (ranging from 1.51 to 1.71 on a 4-point scale). Furthermore, on average, siblings perceived relatively moderate levels of sibling support and low levels of conflict and power imbalance in their relationship. Over time younger sibling depressive symptoms first decreased and then increased moderately and older sibling depressive symptoms decreased more strongly. Perceived sibling support increased slightly over time, whereas perceived conflict between siblings and perceived power imbalance slightly decreased.
Table 1 Means and standard deviations of younger and older sibling depressive symptoms, perceived support, conflict and power imbalance Model Comparisons
With longitudinal models, it is important that variables are measurement-invariant. To test measurement invariance of the variables over time, alignment optimization was used (Asparouhov and Muthén 2014; Lomazzi 2018). These analyses showed that all factor loadings were invariant over six measurement waves and that only few small differences existed in factor intercepts, allowing for reliable testing of over-time associations between sibling relationship quality and adolescent depressionFootnote 1.
Next, separate models for perceived support, conflict, and power imbalance were constructed. For each model, a baseline model was formulated that included one-year stability paths, T1 correlations, correlated changes and cross-lagged effects. Moreover, all variables were regressed on the variable representing dyadic age differences between target adolescent and sibling in order to control for dyadic age differences by regressing. These baseline models for support (χ2(164) = 573.68, p< 0.001, RMSEA = 0.08, CFI = 0.91), conflict (χ2(164) = 527.99, p< 0.001, RMSEA = 0.07, CFI = 0.92) and power imbalance (χ2(164) = 581.84, p< 0.001, RMSEA = 0.08, CFI = 0.89) did not have a good fit. Adding two-year stability paths significantly improved the fit of the model for support (Δχ2(16) = 319.16, p < 0.001), the model for conflict (Δχ2(16) = 293.25, p < 0.001), and the model for power imbalance (Δχ2(16) = 371.34, p < 0.001). Furthermore, these improved models were time invariant as over-time stabilities and correlated changes (support ΔχSB2(52) = 67.51, p > 0.05; conflict (ΔχSB2(52) = 67.41, p > 0.05; power imbalance (ΔχSB2(52) = 47.95, p > 0.10) could be constrained to be equal over time without significantly worsening model fit. Cross-lagged effects were also largely time-invariant (support ΔχSB2(64) = 91.95, p < 0.05; conflict (ΔχSB2(64) = 74.85, p > 0.10; power imbalance (ΔχSB2(64) = 83.38, p > 0.05). To increase ease of interpretation, all these stabilities, correlated changes and cross-lagged effects were constrained to be equal across waves. The fit of these final models for support (χ2(264) = 412.99, p< 0.001, RMSEA = 0.04, CFI = 0.97), conflict (χ2(264) = 375.69, p< 0.001, RMSEA = 0.03, CFI = 0.98) and power imbalance (χ2(264) = 338.52, p< 0.001, RMSEA = 0.03, CFI = 0.98) was good.
Associations Between Adolescent and Sibling Depressive Symptoms and Perceived Relationship Quality
The estimates of the final models for perceived support, conflict and power imbalance are displayed in Table 2. Due to the time invariance constraints, all stabilities, correlated residuals and cross-lagged paths were equal across time. Overall, effect sizes were modest. As displayed in this table, older and younger sibling depressive symptoms, and perceived support, conflict and power imbalance showed significant and moderate stability.
Table 2 Concurrent and longitudinal associations between older sibling (O) and younger sibling (Y) depressive symptoms and sibling relationship As expected, higher levels of younger sibling depressive symptoms were significantly related to less perceived support and more perceived conflict and power imbalance. Results for older sibling depressive symptoms were similar, with one exception: Their depressive symptoms were also significantly related to less support and more conflict, but the correlation with power imbalance was not significant.
Consistent with expectations, within the first wave, higher levels of younger sibling depressive symptoms were significantly but weakly related to more older sibling depressive symptoms. Older and younger sibling’s perceptions of sibling relationship quality were significantly intercorrelated for support and conflict, but not for power imbalance.
Longitudinal Linkages Between Adolescent and Sibling Depressive Symptoms and Perceived Relationship Quality
As indicated by the correlated residuals, changes in younger and older sibling depressive symptoms were significantly related to changes in perceived support, conflict, and power imbalance. However, changes in younger sibling depressive symptoms were not linked to changes in older sibling depressive symptoms. Additionally, changes in both adolescents’ perception of support and conflict were interrelated, but not for power imbalance.
Cross-lagged effects showed that, in line with expectations, younger sibling depressive symptoms were significantly predicted by older sibling depressive symptoms one year earlier. In contrast to expectations, younger sibling depressive symptoms over time were not significantly predicted by perceived support, conflict and power imbalance. Identical results were found for the prediction of older sibling depressive symptoms, which were significantly predicted by younger sibling depressive symptoms, but not by perceived support, conflict and power imbalance.
Furthermore, the effects of depressive symptoms on perceived sibling relationship quality over time were examined. For younger siblings, more younger sibling depressive symptoms significantly predicted less perceived support one year later. No significant effect for conflict or power imbalance was found. For older siblings, their own depressive symptoms did not predict perceived sibling relationship quality over time. However, support and conflict as perceived by the older sibling were significantly predicted by younger sibling depressive symptoms: More younger sibling depressive symptoms predicted more support and less conflict as perceived by the older sibling one year later. No significant effects of younger nor older sibling depressive symptoms on perceived power imbalance were found.
Lastly, support and conflict as perceived by older siblings significantly affected support and conflict as perceived by younger siblings one year later, and vice versa. Again, no significant effects concerning sibling power imbalance were found.
Comparing Influences of Sibling Relationship Quality and Sibling Depressive Symptoms
Subsequent models tested whether sibling depressive symptoms on the one hand and relationship quality on the other hand could be constrained to be equal in predicting depressive symptoms over time. To allow for meaningful comparison, standardization of scores was applied in these models. The results showed that for the prediction of younger sibling depressive symptoms, constraining the effects of older sibling depressive symptoms and sibling relationship quality as perceived by the younger sibling led to a significantly better-fitting model for support (Δχ2 (1) = −7.86, p < 0.001), similar-fitting model for conflict (Δχ2 (1) = 2.30, p > 0.05) and significantly worse-fitting model for power imbalance (Δχ2 (1) = 16.19, p < 0.001).
For the prediction of older sibling depressive symptoms, constraining the effects of younger sibling depressive symptoms and sibling relationship quality as perceived by the older sibling led to a significantly better-fitting model for support (Δχ2 (1) = −10.01, p < 0.001), similar-fitting model for conflict (Δχ2 (1) = −1.86, p > 0.05) and significantly worse-fitting model for power imbalance (Δχ2 (1) = 14.73, p < 0.001).
So, in the prediction of adolescent depressive symptoms, the over-time effects of sibling depressive symptoms are significantly stronger than the over-time effects of power imbalance, but similar to the over-time effects of sibling support or sibling conflict.
Moderation by Sibling Relationship Quality
Next, the potential moderating effect of perceived relationship quality on the effect of older sibling depressive symptoms on younger sibling depressive symptoms was tested. However, contrary to expectations, none of the interaction terms were significant: perceived support, conflict and power imbalance did not moderate the effect of older sibling depressive symptoms on younger sibling depressive symptoms.
These moderation models were also tested with older sibling depressive symptoms as outcome variables. The results of these analyses were identical to those with younger sibling depressive symptoms as outcome variables: Perceived support, conflict, and power imbalance did not moderate the effect of younger sibling depressive symptoms on older sibling depressive symptoms.
Moderation by Dyadic Gender Composition
Next, the possible moderating effect of dyadic gender composition on the longitudinal effects of sibling depressive symptoms and perceived sibling relationship quality on adolescent depressive symptoms differed by dyadic gender composition. Multigroup analyses were conducted, testing whether these cross-lagged effects could be constrained to be equal for dyadic gender composition without significantly worsening model fit. These models were tested separately for younger and older sibling depressive symptoms as outcome variables. The results show that dyadic gender composition did not moderate cross-lagged effects in any of these models. This implies that the effects of sibling depressive symptoms and perceived relationship quality on adolescent depressive symptoms did not differ by adolescent dyadic gender composition.
Sensitivity Analysis
The robustness of these findings was checked by examining an alternative model in which the potential moderating effects were examined between sibling relationship quality according to younger sibling and according to older sibling in predicting adolescent depressive symptoms. Similar to the models concerning the moderating effects between sibling relationship quality and sibling depressive symptoms on adolescent depressive behavior, a centered interaction between relationship quality perceived by younger sibling and by older sibling was estimated in predicting adolescent depressive symptoms over time. Again, cross-lagged paths and dyadic age differences between older and younger sibling were controlled for. These models were formulated separately for younger and older sibling depressive symptoms as outcomes, and separately for perceived support, conflict and power imbalance (so six models in total).
None of the interaction terms between older and younger sibling perceived relationship quality significantly predicted adolescent depressive symptoms (for younger or older sibling). The conclusion is therefore that there are no moderating effects between perceived relationship quality of older and younger siblings in predicting adolescent depressive symptoms.