Skip to main content
Log in

Conversion and Self-Transformation in Christian Monasticism: A Cognitive Perspective

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Religion and Health Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Religious conversion involves changes in the convert’s way of thinking and behaving. This paper focuses on the unique form that this transformative process took within the Christian monastic movement in late antiquity. Treating monastic conversion as a gradual process in which the convert is an active participant, it examines the ways in which monastic converts were able to intentionally promote such a change and influence its direction. This study draws on research in cognitive psychology and neuroscience on the cognitive underpinnings of self-transformation in order to reconstruct from the literary sources of Near Eastern monasticism the strategies and training methods that late antique monks employed in order to facilitate and sustain the transformation implicated in conversion.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See James (1958 [1902]), and Nock (1961[1933]). James acknowledged the existence of conversion as a gradual process, yet religion for him was first and foremost an individual experience prior to any rational understanding of it. A generation later, Nock studied conversion in Greco-Roman antiquity using lenses prescribed by James. Nock followed James in viewing conversion as an intense, all-consuming process. He argued that Judaism and Christianity, as exclusivist and monotheistic religions, demanded much more than acceptance of a rite or the incorporation of another religion, as was the case with the Greco-Roman polytheistic religions. Consequently, he distinguished between adhesion, which he characterized participation in the traditional religions of Greece and Rome, and conversion, which involves a decision to reject old beliefs as wrong and accept new religious beliefs. For several decades, the systematic study of conversion stayed largely where James and Nock left it.

  2. For a review of recent trends in conversion research, see Rambo and Farhadian (2004, 1–24); Lamb and Bryant (1999, 1–22); Peace (2004: 8–14). For recent approaches in the study of late antique conversion, see the contributions in Marzouki and Roy (2013).

  3. See, for example, Witherup (1994). Witherup argues that: “After careful consideration I have come to believe that the NT shows a fairly uniform teaching about conversion with minimal evidence for a dramatic evolution of the concept” (p. 2). See also Rambo (1993). While acknowledging earlier researchers like William James, who focused on dramatic, singular experiences, Rambo argues that more attention should be given to the long-term psychological and sociological processes at work in conversion, and offers a heuristic model that explores the stages of the multilayered processes involved in conversion. On Christian conversion as a gradual process of transformation, see also Finn (1997).

  4. New Testament writers employ the Greek terms ἐπιστρέφειν (to turn back) and μετάνοια (to think again, to change mentality) to characterize a radical change of perspective. Both terms were later translated into Latin as conversio. On conversion and repentance in early Christianity, see Stroumsa (1999, 167–178). On the unique features of early Christian ἐπιστροφή, see also Rotman (2016: 12–37).

  5. Monastic conversion could coincide with religious conversion to Christianity (e.g., Apoph. patr. systematic collection 15.112 and 13.12), but this was not usually the case. Rather, the majority of early monastic writings that mention a conversion to the monastic life simply ignores the religious affiliation of the convert prior to his or her embarking on the monastic way. Entrance into the monastic life was often considered as “a second conversion,” following the first one of baptism. In this view, baptism does not so much establish the Christian believer in a state of grace, as it sets him or her on a journey which leads one much beyond oneself and beyond the limits of his or her own individual experience. See Veilleux (1986: 34–45). For a broader perspective on religious conversion as one type of a larger category of spiritual transformations, see Paloutzian 2015: 331–347).

  6. The main paradigm of this profound transformation was God’s transformation to humanity, as described in Paul’s letter to the Philippians (Phil 2:6–9).

  7. Plato, Republic VII 514.a to 517.a.7. A different notion, however, can be found in Politeia VII 514.a to 517.a.7. On conversion to philosophy in the Greco-Roman world, see Szalay (2013: 484–503); Nock (1961: 14–15).

  8. See Moberg (2016, esp. 67–68). Moberg shows that the sayings collected in the Apophthegmata Patrum suggest that for those who chose to become monks, this choice was more fundamental than the moment of baptism for their identity.

  9. Hadot (1995, chap. 4). Historians of Christianity have applied Hadot’s theoretical formulation of “spiritual exercises” to early monastic texts. See, for example, Sheridan (2002: 1–51) and Bitton-Ashkelony (2003: 200–221).

  10. Though Rambo and Farhadian (2004) has recently devoted a chapter to “Religious Conversion and Cognitive Neuroscience,” that chapter merely offers directions for future research and enumerates areas of research in cognitive neuroscience that may be relevant to the study of religious conversion.

  11. Nevertheless, several scholars did try to connect between these fields. For example, Paul Markham’s study of conversion attempts to relate contemporary neuroscience to John Wesley’s Methodist doctrine of conversion, see Markham (2007). For a criticism of Markham's conclusions, see Weissenbacher (2016: 347–360). See also Davis and Rambo (2005, 159–173).

  12. I follow Martin (2007, 37–55, at 51–52). See also Martin (2004: 36–44). Experimental work suggests that cultural variations are constrained in important ways by innate cognitive mechanisms. For example, the view that infants and young children indiscriminately adopt basic theories about the world around them through social learning alone has been thoroughly challenged in recent years. Novel experimental techniques have revealed that infants exhibit preferences, expectations, and knowledge that could not have been wholly acquired through social transmission. See Nisbett and Norenzayan (2002: 132–135) and Boyer (2014: 53–72).

  13. Origen, Princ. 2.9.2 (SC 252: 355–356). On Origen’s concept of satiety, see Otis (1958: 95–124).

  14. Origen, Princ. 1.4.1 (SC 252: 166) and 2.9.2 (SC 252: 354).

  15. For the influence of the Alexandrian theologians on monastic theology, see Gould (1995, 591–598) and Rubenson (1995, 55).

  16. Evagrius, KG 2.15 and 3.35 (ed. Wilhelm Frankenberg, Evagrius Ponticus, Abhandlungen der königlichen Gesellschaft der Wissenschaften zu Göttingen, Phil.-hist. Klasse, neue Folge 13.2 [Berlin: Weidmannsche Buchhandlung, 1912], 141 and 213). According to Evagrius, “Anyone who is making progress in the ascetical life (πρακτική) is reducing the passions (τὰ πάθη μειοῖ), and anyone who is making progress in contemplation (θεωρία) is reducing ignorance” (Praktikos 87 [SC 171: 678]). He also teaches that the soul operates naturally (κατὰ φύσιν) when its reasoning part (λογιστικόν) applies itself to contemplation (Praktikos 86 [SC 171: 676]). See also Athanasius, VA 20 (SC 400: 88–200). On Evagrius’ notion of contemplative prayer, see Bitton-Ashkelony (2011: 291–321).

  17. Both penitence and attentiveness were an integral part of the daily routine of late antique monks (rather than an element of the sacramental system), and worked together in order to promote and sustain the conversion process. See Bitton-Ashkelony (1999, 179–194). On attentiveness in Eastern monasticism, see Graiver (2018).

  18. Attentiveness is also closely related to the practice of watchfulness (νῆψις), defined by Hesychius the Priest as “a spiritual method (μέθοδος πνευματική) which, if sedulously practiced over a long period completely frees us with God’s help from impassioned thoughts (ἐμπαθῶν νοημάτων), impassioned words and evil actions. It leads, in so far as this is possible, to a sure knowledge of the inapprehensible God… It is, in the true sense, purity of heart” (On Watchfulness and Holiness 1 [PG 93:1480]; trans. G. E. H Palmer, Philip Sherrard, and Kallistos Ware, The Philokalia: The Complete Text, vol. 1 [London: Faber and Faber, 1983], 162).

  19. The fourth-century desert fathers employed a wide variety of formulae. Yet, between the fifth and the eighth century the Jesus Prayer emerged as a recognized spiritual method. See Ware (1986, 175–184).

  20. On these strategies, see Stewart (2011: 263–275).

  21. Diadochus, Hundred Gnostic Chapters 59 (SC 5: 119). The writings of Diadochus provide a key link between the Syrian and Egyptian traditions. John Cassian also explains that “A monk’s whole attention (omnis intentio) should constantly be fixed on one thing, and the beginnings and the roundabout turns of all his thoughts should be strenuously called back to this very thing—that is, to the recollection of God” (Coll. 24.6.1–3 [CSEL 13: 680]). Cf. Apophth. patr. N 435.

  22. John Cassian, Coll. 7.4.2 (CSEL 13: 183).

  23. On this process, see Linge (1990, 37–70, at 65).

  24. Evagrius, Praktikos 50 (SC 171: 614); cf. On Thoughts 9 (SC 438: 180); Apophth. patr. alph. Elias 4 (PG 65: 184). Evagrius specifies the benefits of this form of self-examination: “It is necessary to know these things, so that when thoughts begin to move their own particular matter (τὰς ἰδίας ὕλας), and before we are driven too far from our proper state, we may speak out to them and indicate which one is present” (Praktikos 43 [SC 171: 600]). Cf. Apophth. patr. N 670: “We must arm ourselves prior to temptations […], and in this way we will be clearly seen to be ready for them when they come upon us” (ed. Wortley, 536).

  25. John Cassian, Coll. 1.20.1 (CSEL 13: 29).

  26. Evagrius, On Thoughts 19 (SC 438: 218–219).

  27. See Hagger et al. (2009: 208–238, at 216–217) and Schneider and Fisk (1982: 160–68).

  28. See Schneider and Shiffrin (1977: 1–66).

  29. See Kihlstrom (2008, 155–180) and Baars (1993: 281–309).

  30. In one experiment, children who have stuttered for years were told to stutter deliberately for thirty seconds each time they do so involuntarily. According to the study findings, 75% stopped stuttering in a day or two. This paradoxical technique has been observed to stop a wide variety of pathological symptoms with remarkable speed. See Baars, “Why Volition Is a Foundation Problem,” 290; Cohen et al. (1990: 332–361).

  31. Baars, “Why Volition Is a Foundation Problem,” 287.

  32. Monastic writers refer to this phenomenon when they compare ascetic practice to the gradual acquisition of any profession through systematic training. According to Cassian, “The perfection of any art or discipline (artis seu disciplinae) must begin with some simple rudiments, and grow accustomed first to somewhat easy and tender beginnings, so that being nourished and trained little by little […] it may grow up and so by degrees and step by step mount up from the lowest depths to the heights: and when by these means it has […] passed the gates of the entrance of the profession (professionis), it will consequently arrive without difficulty at the […] heights of perfection” (Coll. 10.8 [CSEL 13: 294]).

  33. For example, experiential studies show that individuals can effectively regulate their emotion through their attentional deployment; see Wadlinger and Isaacowitz (2011: 75–102). See also Bommarito (2013: 93–117).

  34. For a review of the research, see Posner (1980: 3–25).

  35. See Begley (2007: 1–4). For the ways in which individuals’ experience-based neuroarchitecture is developed, see Benvenuti and Davenport (2011, 204–238, at 213–214).

  36. See Pascual-Leone et al. (2005: 377–401). However, it is important to note that merely mental training does not result in excellent piano-playing expertise in the same way that actually practicing the piano does.

  37. Studies of cortical plasticity in animals reveal that the remodeling of cortical representations by use is modulated as a function of cognitive state, and that remodeling does not occur when the stimulation is not attended to. See Merzenich and Jenkins (1993: 89–104). See also Recanzone et al. (1993: 87–103).

  38. See Davidson and Lutz (2008: 174–176) and Lutz et al. (2008: 163–169).

  39. Neuroscientist Richard Davidson, who recruited Buddhist monks to meditate inside a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) apparatus while he measured their brain activity, initiated this development. See Davidson (2003: 564–570).

  40. See Shapiro et al. (2002, 632–645). See also Moore and Malinowski (2009: 176–186).

  41. See Walsh and Shapiro (2006: 227–239); Levenson et al. (2012: 650–658).

  42. In monastic sources, this process is described as renunciation (e.g., John Climacus, Scal. 1 [PG 88: 633]). In this view, subjecting oneself to God involves renunciation of one’s own desires and selfish will. Consequently, the ascetic imperative in early monasticism involved a renunciation of self-will. On the concept of will in Gazan monasticism, see Kofsky (2006: 321–346).

  43. The Book of Paradise, Being the Histories and Sayings of the Monks and Ascetics of the Egyptian Desert According to the Recension of “Anan-Isho” of Beth Abbe, ed. and trans. E.A. Wallis Budge (London: Printed for Lady Mieux by Drugulin, 1904), 2: 1020. The anthology The Book of Paradise was compiled in the seventh century and includes a Syriac translation of The Sayings of the Desert Fathers.

  44. Psychological research on conversion has occurred primarily during two broad phases, one at the turn of the twentieth century, as psychology was establishing itself as a discipline, and one during the last third of the twentieth century and continuing to the present. On this development see Ullman (1989). On the psychology of conversion during the early period, see James (1958) and Pratt (1907). For more recent studies which apply psychological theories to the study of conversion, see Granqvist and Kirkpatrick (2004: 223–250) and Ullman (1989).

References

Primary Sources

  • Apophthegmata patrum, anonymous collection. Ed. François Nau, “Histoires des solitaires chrétiens,” Revue de l’Orient chrétien 12 (1907): 48–68, 171–181, 393–404; 13 (1908): 47–57, 266–283; 14 (1909): 357–79; 17 (1912): 204–211, 294–301; 18 (1913): 137–146. English translation: The Anonymous Sayings of the Desert Fathers: A Select Edition and Complete English Translation, trans. John Wortley (Cambridge: Cambridge Uni. Press, 2013).

  • Apophthegmata patrum, Greek alphabetical collection. Ed. Jean Baptiste Cotelier, Ecclesiae graecae monumenta 1 (Paris: Muguet, 1677); reprinted in PG 65: 72–440. English translation: The Sayings of the Desert Fathers: The Alphabetical Collection, trans. Benedicta Ward, CS 59 (Kalamazoo: Cistercian Publications, 1975).

  • Apophthegmata patrum, Syriac collection. Text and English translation: The Book of Paradise, Being the Histories and Sayings of the Monks and Ascetics of the Egyptian Desert According to the Recension of “Anan-Isho” of Beth Abbe, ed. and trans. E.A. Wallis Budge, 2 vols. (London: Printed for Lady Mieux by Drugulin, 1904).

  • Apophthegmata patrum, systematic collection. Les Apophtegmes des Pères: Collection systèmatique, ed. Jean-Claude Guy, 3 vols, SC 387, 474, 498 (Paris: Cerf, 1993–2005).

  • Athanasius. Vita Antonii. PG 26: 838–976. Athanase d’Alexandrie: Vie d’Antoine, ed. G.J.M. Bartelink, SC 400 (Paris: Cerf, 1994).

  • Cassian, John. Collationes. Ed. Michael Petschenig, CSEL 13 (Vienna: Austrian Academy of Sciences, 2004). English translation: The Conferences, trans. Boniface Ramsey (New York: Paulist Press, 1997).

  • Climacus, John. Scala Paradisi. Ed. Mattheus Raderus, in Ioannis Scholastici abbatis Montis Sina, qui vulgo Climacus appellatur opera omnia (1633); reprinted in PG 88: 624–1164. English translation: The Ladder of Divine Ascent, trans. Colm Luibheid and Norman Russell (Mahwah, NJ: Paulist Press, 1982).

  • Diadochus of Photike. Capita centum de perfectione spirituali. Ed. Édouard des Places, SC 5 (Paris: Cerf, 1966). English translation: Following the Footsteps of the Invisible: The Complete Works of Diadochus of Photikē, trans. Cliff Ermatinger, CS 239 (Collegeville, MN: Liturgical Press, 2010).

  • Evagrius Ponticus. Kephalaia gnostica. Ed. Antoine Guillaumont, PO 28.1 (Paris: Didot, 1958).

  • Evagrius Ponticus. On Thoughts (Peri Logismōn). Ed. Paul Géhin, Claire Guillaumont, and Antoine Guillaumont, SC 438 (Paris: Cerf, 1998). English translation in Evagrius of Pontus: The Greek Ascetic Corpus, ed. and trans. Robert E. Sinkewicz (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2003), 153–182.

  • Evagrius Ponticus. Praktikos. Ed. Antoine Guillaumont and Claire Giullaumont. SC 170 (Introduction), 171 (text). Paris: Cerf, 1971. English translation by Robert E. Sinkewicz, in Evagrius of Pontus: The Greek Ascetic Corpus, 95–114.

  • Hesychius the Priest. On Watchfulness and Holiness. PG 93: 1480D–1544. English translation in Philokalia: The Eastern Christian Spiritual Texts, trans. G.E.H Palmer, Philip Sherrard, and Kallistos Ware (London: Faber & Faber, 1979), 1: 162–198.

  • Origen. De principiis. Ed. Henri Crouzel and Manlio Simonetti, SC 252–253, 268–269, 312 (Paris: Cerf, 1978–1984). English translation in Fathers of the Third Century, ANF 4 (Grand Rapids: Christian Classics Ethereal Library, 1885).

Secondary Literature

  • Baars, B. J. (1993). Why volition is a foundation problem for psychology. Consciousness and Cognition,2, 281–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Begley, S. (2007). How thinking can change the brain. Science Journal,10, 1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benvenuti, A. C., & Davenport, E. L. (2011). The new archaic: A neurophenomenological approach to religious ways of knowing. In B. M. Stafford (Ed.), A field guide to a new meta-field: Bridging the humanities-neurosciences divide (pp. 204–238). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bitton-Ashkelony, B. (1999). Penitence in Late Antique Monastic Literature. In J. Assmann & G. G. Stroumsa (Eds.), Transformations of the Inner Self in Ancient Religions (pp. 179–194). Leiden: Brill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bitton-Ashkelony, B. (2003). Demons and prayers: Spiritual exercises in the monastic community of Gaza in the Fifth and Sixth Centuries. Vigiliae Christianae,57, 200–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bitton-Ashkelony, B. (2011). The limit of the mind (ΝΟΥΣ): Pure prayer according to Evagrius Ponticus and Isaac of Nineveh. Zeitschrift für antikes Christentum,15, 291–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bommarito, N. (2013). Modesty as a virtue of attention. Philosophical Review,122, 93–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (2014). Cognitive aspects of religious ontologies. Scripta Instituti Donneriani Aboensis,17, 53–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., McClelland, J. L., & Dunbar, K. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the stroop effect. Psychological Review,97, 332–361.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine,65, 564–570.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. A., & Lutz, A. (2008). Buddha’s brain: Neuroplasticity and meditation. IEEE Signal Processing Magazine,25, 174–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, P., & Rambo, L. R. (2005). Converting: Toward a cognitive theory of religious change. In K. Bulkeley (Ed.), Soul, psyche, brain: New directions in the study of religion and brain-mind science (pp. 159–173). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finn, M. (1997). From death to rebirth: Ritual and conversion in antiquity. New York: Paulist Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, G. (1995). The influence of origen on fourth-century monasticism: some further remarks. In G. Dorival & A. Le Boulluec (Eds.), Origen and the bible: Actes du colloquium origenianum sextum chantilly, 30 août–3 septembre 1993 (pp. 591–598). Leuven: Leuven University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graiver, I. (2018). Asceticism of the mind: Forms of attention and self-transformation in late antiquity. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Granqvist, P., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2004). Religious conversion and perceived childhood attachment: A meta-analysis. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion,14, 223–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hadot, P. (1995). Philosophy as a way of life: Spiritual exercises from socrates to foucault. Translated by Michael Chase. Oxford: Blackwell

  • Hagger, S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. (2009). The strength model of self-regulation failure and health-related behaviour. Health Psychology Review,3, 208–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1958 [1902]). The varieties of religious experience. New York: New American Library.

  • Kihlstrom, J. F. (2008). The automaticity Juggernaut—Or, are we automatons after all. In J. Baer, J. C. Kaufman, & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Are we free? Psychology and free will (pp. 155–180). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kofsky, A. (2006). Renunciation of will in the monastic school of gaza. Liber Annuus,56, 321–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, C., & Bryant, M. D. (Eds.). (1999). Religious conversion: Contemporary practices and controversies. New York: Cassell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levenson, R., Ekman, P., & Ricard, M. (2012). Meditation and the startle response: A case study. Emotion,12, 650–658.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Linge, D. E. (1990). Asceticism and ‘Singleness of Mind’ in the desert fathers. In A. B. Creel & V. Narayanan (Eds.), Monastic life in the Christian and Hindu Traditions (pp. 37–70). Lewiston, NY: Edwin Mellen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,12(4), 163–169.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Markham, P. (2007). Rewired: Exploring religious conversion. Eugene, OR: Pickwick.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. H. (2004). Disenchanting’ the comparative study of religion. Method and Theory in the Study of Religion,16, 36–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. H. (2007). The promise of cognitive science for the study of early christianity. In P. Luomanen, I. Pyysiäinen, & R. Uro (Eds.), Explaining Christian origins and early Judaism contributions from cognitive and social science (pp. 37–55). Leiden: Brill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marzouki, N., & Roy, O. (Eds.). (2013). Religious conversions in the mediterranean world. London: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merzenich, M. M., & Jenkins, W. M. (1993). Reorganization of cortical representations of the hand following alterations of skin inputs induced by nerve injury, skin island transfers, and experience. Journal of Hand Therapy,6, 89–104.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moberg, S. G. (2016). The apophthegmata patrum and the greek philosophical tradition. Ph.D. Dissertation, The Catholic University of America.

  • Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Consciousness and Cognition,18, 176–186.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R., & Norenzayan, A. (2002). Culture and cognition. Current Directions in Experimental Psychology,9, 132–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nock, A. D. (1961 [1933]). Conversion: The old and the new in religion from Alexander the great to Augustine of Hippo. London: Oxford University Press.

  • Otis, B. (1958). Cappadocian thought as a coherent system. Dumbarton Oaks Papers,12, 95–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paloutzian, R. F. (2015). Religious Conversion and Spiritual Transformation: A Meaning-System Analysis. In L. R. Rambo & C. E. Farhadian (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Religious Conversion (pp. 331–347). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, A., Amedi, A., Fregni, F., & Merabet, L. B. (2005). The plastic human brain cortex. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience,28, 377–401.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Peace, R. V. (2004). Conflicting understandings of Christian conversion: A missiological challenge. International Bulletin of Mission Research,28(1), 8–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,32, 3–25.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, J. B. (1907). The psychology of religious belief. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rambo, L. R. (1993). Understanding religious conversion. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rambo, L. R., & Farhadian, C. E. (Eds.). (2004). Oxford handbook of religious conversion. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Recanzone, G. H., Schreiner, C. E., & Merzenich, M. M. (1993). Plasticity in the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following discrimination training in adult owl monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience,13, 87–103.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rotman, Y. (2016). Reconstructing late antique psychology: Reversion, conversion and introversion of the soul. Journal for Late Antique Religion and Culture,10, 12–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubenson, S. (1995). The letters of St. Antony: Monasticism and the making of a Saint. Minneapolis: Fortress Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Fisk, A. D. (1982). Degree of consistent training: Improvements in search performance and automatic process development. Perception and Psychophysics,31, 160–168.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review,84, 1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, S. L., Schwartz, G. E. R., & Santerre, C. (2002). Meditation and positive psychology. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 632–645). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheridan, M. (2002). The spiritual and intellectual world of early Egyptian monasticism. Coptica,1, 1–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, C. (2011). Evagrius ponticus and the eastern monastic tradition. Modern Theology,27, 263–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroumsa, G. G. (1999). From repentance to penance in early Christianity: Tertullian’s De Paenitentia in Context. In J. Assmann & G. G. Stroumsa (Eds.), Transformations of the inner self in ancient religions (pp. 167–178). Leiden: Brill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szalay, M. (2013). Metanoia: A phenomenological analysis of philosophical conversion. Radical Orthodoxy: Theology, Philosophy, Politics,1(3), 484–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, C. (1989). The transformed self: The psychology of religious conversion. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veilleux, A. (1986). The monastic way of conversion. The American Benedictine Review,37(1), 34–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2011). Fixing our focus: Training attention to regulate emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review,15, 75–102.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, R., & Shapiro, S. L. (2006). The meeting of meditative disciplines and western psychology: A mutually enriching dialogue. American Psychologist,61, 227–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ware, K. (1986). The origins of the Jesus Prayer: Diadochus, Gaza, Sinai. In C. Jones, G. Wainwright, & E. Yarnold (Eds.), The study of spirituality (pp. 175–184). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weissenbacher, A. C. (2016). The neuroscience of Wesleyan soteriology: The dynamic of both instantaneous and gradual change. Zygon,51(2), 347–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witherup, R. (1994). Conversion in the new testament. Collegeville, MI: Liturgical Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

None.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Inbar Graiver.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Graiver, I. Conversion and Self-Transformation in Christian Monasticism: A Cognitive Perspective. J Relig Health 59, 1610–1625 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-019-00941-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-019-00941-4

Keywords

Navigation