Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Disentangling Effects of Memory Storage and Inter-articulator Coordination on Generalization in Speech Motor Sequence Learning

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Generalization in motor control is the extent to which motor learning affects movements in situations different than those in which it originally occurred. Recent data on orofacial speech movements indicates that motor sequence learning generalizes to novel syllable sequences containing phonotactically illegal, but previously practiced, consonant clusters. Practicing an entire syllable, however, results in even larger performance gains compared to practicing just its clusters. These patterns of generalization could reflect language-general changes in phonological memory storage and/or inter-articulator coordination during motor sequence learning. To disentangle these factors, we conducted two experiments in which talkers intensively practiced producing novel syllables containing illegal onset and coda clusters over two consecutive days. During the practice phases of both experiments, we observed that, through repetition, talkers gradually produced the syllables with fewer errors, indicative of learning. After learning, talkers were tested for generalization to single syllables (Experiment 1) or syllable pairs (Experiment 2) that overlapped to varying degrees with the practiced syllables. Across both experiments, we found that performance improvements from practicing syllables with illegal clusters partially generalized to novel syllables that contained those clusters, but performance was more error prone if the clusters occurred in a different syllable position (onset versus coda) as in practice, demonstrating that inter-articulator coordination is contextually sensitive. Furthermore, changing the position of a cluster was found to be more deleterious to motor performance during the production of the second syllables in syllable pairs, which required talkers to store more phonological material in memory prior to articulation, compared to single syllables. This interaction effect reveals a complex interplay between memory storage and inter-articulator coordination on generalization in speech motor sequence learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We opted not to measure reaction time (RT) in this or the subsequent experiment since this metric of motor performance is contaminated by the simple RT paradigm utilized, i.e., participants could “pre-program” their motor responses prior to the onset of the go-signal, which in turn, would “wash out” potential differences in utterance planning time across conditions (for discussion, see Klapp, 1995, 2003; Wright et al., 2009). Consistent with this hypothesis, no reliable differences in RT were previously reported by Segawa et al. (2019).

  2. Pilot testing also revealed that instructing talkers to produce lists of sequences comprised of three or four syllables was too challenging as error rates approached ceiling.

References

  • Aichert, I., & Ziegler, A. (2013). Segments and syllables in the treatment of apraxia of speech: an investigation of learning and transfer effects. Aphasiology, 27(1), 1180–1199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A., Lewis, V., & Vallar, G. (1984). Exploring the articulatory loop. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36, 233–252. https://doi.org/10.1080/146407484

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., Steriade, D., Lennertz, T., & Vaknin, V. (2007). What we know about what we have never heard: Evidence from perceptual illusions. Cognition, 104, 591–630.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2020). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.1.09, retrieved 26 January 2020 from http://www.praat.org/.

  • Bohland, J. W., Bullock, D., & Guenther, F. H. (2010). Neural representations and mechanisms for the performance of simple speech sequences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(7), 1504–1529.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Browman, C., & Goldstein, L. (1995). Gestural syllable position effects in American English. In F. Bell-Berti & L. Raphael (Eds.), Producing speech: Contemporary issues. for Katherine Safford Harris (pp. 19–34). AIP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Browman, C. P., & Goldstein, L. (1988). Some Notes on Syllable Structure in Articulatory Phonology. Phonetica, 45(2–4), 140–155. https://doi.org/10.1159/000261823

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buchwald, A., Calhoun, H., Rimikis, S., Lowe, M. S., Wellner, R., & Edwards, D. J. (2019). Using tDCS to facilitate motor learning in speech production: The role of timing. Cortex, 111, 274–285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byrd, D. (1995). C-Centers revisited. Phonetica, 52, 263–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrd, D. (1996). Influences on articulatory timing in consonant sequences. Journal of Phonetics, 24(2), 209–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrd, D., Lee, S., Riggs, D., & Adams, J. (2005). Interacting effects of syllable and phrase position on consonant articulation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118, 3860–3873.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Choe, W. K., & Redford, M. A. (2012). The distribution of speech errors in multi-word prosodic units. Laboratory Phonology, 3, 5–26. https://doi.org/10.1515/lp-2012-0002

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cholin, J., Levelt, W. J. M., & Schiller, N. O. (2006). Effects of syllable frequency in speech production. Cognition, 99, 205–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2016). The now-or-never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 1–72. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1500031X

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, G. N. (1990). The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification. In J. Kingston & M. Beckman (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology 1: Between the grammar and physics of speech (pp. 283–333). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, L. (2006). Phonology, phonetics, or frequency: Influences on the production of non-native sequences. Journal of Phonetics, 34, 104–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, L. (2010). Phonetic bases of similarities in cross-language production: Evidence from English and Catalan. Journal of Phonetics, 38(2), 272–288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2010.01.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, L. (2011). Phonetic and phonological factors in the second language production of phonemes and phonotactics. Language and Linguistics Compass, 5(3), 126–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, L., Jusczyk, P., & Smolensky, P. (2004). The initial and final states: Theoretical implications and experimental explorations of richness of the base. In R. Kager, W. Zonneveld, & J. Pater (Eds.), Fixing priorities: Constraints in phonological acquisition. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S., Juliano, C., & Govindjee, A. (1993). Structure and content in language production: A theory of frame constraints in phonological speech errors. Cognitive Science, 17, 149–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dupoux, E., Kakehi, K., Pallier, Y., Hirose, C., & Mehler, J. (1999). Epenthetic vowels in Japanese: A perceptual illusion? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(6), 1568–1578.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dupoux, E., Parlato, E., Frota, S., Hirose, Y., & Peperkamp, S. (2011). Where do illusory vowels comes from? Journal of Memory and Language, 64(3), 199–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, C. A., Treiman, R., & Gross, J. (1993). The structure of English syllables and polysyllables. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 115–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fromkin, V. A. (1973). Speech errors as linguistic evidence. Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fujimura, O. (2000). The C/D model and prosodic control of articulatory behavior. Phonetica, 57(2–4), 128–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, L., Pouplier, M., Chen, L., Saltzman, E., & Byrd, D. (2007). Dynamic action units slip in speech production errors. Cognition, 103(3), 386–412 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.05.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guenther, F.H. (2016). Neural Control of Speech. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Keating, P., Wright, R., & Zhang, J. (1999). Word-level asymmetries in consonant articulation, In UCLA Working papers in phonetics 97.

  • Guenther, F. H., Ghosh, S. S., & Tourville, J. A. (2006). Neural modeling and imaging of the cortical interactions underlying syllable production. Brain & Language, 96, 280–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haberlandt, K., Thomas, J. G., Lawrence, H., & Krohn, T. (2005). Transposition asymmetry in immediate serial recall. Memory, 13, 274–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, J. (2004). Developmental sequences in the acquisition of English L2 syllable codas. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 85–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haunz, C. (2003). Grammatical and non-grammatical factors in loanword adaptation. In M. J. Sole ́, D. Recasens, & J. Romero (Eds.), Proceedings of the 15th international congress of phonetic sciences. Universitat Autnoma de Barcelona Barcelona, Spain.

  • Henke, E., Kaisse, E. M., & Wright, R. (2012). Is the sonority sequencing principle an epiphenomenon. In S. Parker (Ed.), The sonority controversy (pp. 65–100). Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, I.-N., Tomasini, J., & Nikl, L. (1977). The primacy and recency effects in successive single-trial immediate recall. Journal of General Psychology, 97, 157–165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keating, P., Wright, R., & Zhang, J. (1999). Word-level asymmetries in consonant articulation. UCLA Working papers in phonetics 97.

  • Keating, P. (1983). Comments on the jaw and syllable structure. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 401–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kent, R. D. (2000). Research on speech motor control and its disorders: A review and prospective. Journal of Communication Disorders, 33(5), 391–427.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, M. S., Kim, J. J., & Kwon, J. S. (2001). The effect of immediate and delayed word repetition on event-related potential in a continuous recognition task. Cognitive Brain Research, 11(3), 387–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klapp, S. T. (1995). Motor response programming during simple and choice reaction time: The role of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(5), 1015–1027.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klapp, S. T. (2003). Reaction time analysis of two types of motor preparation for speech articulation: Action as a sequence of chunks. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35(2), 135–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krakauer, J. W., Hadjiosif, A. M., Xu, J., Wong, A. L., & Haith, A. M. (2019). Motor learning. Comprehensive Physiology, 9, 613–663.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. John Wiley Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences., 22, 1–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M., & Wheeldon, L. (1994). Do speakers have access to a mental syllabary? Cognition, 50, 239–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindblom, B. (1983). Economy of speech gestures. In P. MacNeilage (Ed.), The production of speech (pp. 217–246). Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Loevenbruck, H., Collins, M. J., Beckman, M. E., Krishnamurth, A. K., & Ahalt, S. C. (1999). Temporal coordination of articulatory gestures in consonant clusters and sequences of consonants. In O. Fujimura, B. D. Joseph, & B. Palek (Eds.), Proceedings of linguistics phonetics (pp. 547–573). The Karolinum Press.

  • MacKay, D. G. (1970). Spoonerisms: The structure of errors in the serial order of speech. Neuropsychologia, 8, 323–350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marin, S., & Pouplier, M. (2010). Temporal Organization of Complex Onsets and Codas in American English: Testing the Predictions of a Gestural Coupling Model. Motor Control, 14(3), 380–407. https://doi.org/10.1123/mcj.14.3.380

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masapollo, M., Segawa, J. A., Beal, D., Tourville, J., Nieto-Castañón, A., Heyne, M., Frankford, S., & Guenther, F. H. (2021). Behavioral and neural correlates of speech motor sequence learning in neurotypical and stuttering speakers: An fMRI investigation. Neurobiology of Language, 2(1), 106–137.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nairne, J. S., & Crowder, R. G. (1982). On the locus of the stimulus suffix effect. Memory & Cognition, 10, 350–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nairne, J. S., & Walters, V. L. (1983). Silent mouthing produces modality-and suffix-like effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 475–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, K., Sakai, K., & Hikosaka, O. (1998). Neuronal activity in medial frontal cortex during learning of sequential procedures. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 2671–2687.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nittrouer, S., & Miller, M. E. (1999). The development of phonemic coding strategies for serial recall. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20(4), 563–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nooteboom, S. G. (1969). The tongue slips into patterns. Leyden studies in linguistics and phonetics. Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohala, J. (1990). Alternatives to the sonority hierarchy for explaining segmental sequential constraints. In M. Ziolkowski, M. Noske & K. Deaton (Eds.), CLS 26: Volume 2: The Parasession on the syllable in phonetics and phonology (Vol. 26, pp. 319–338). Chicago Linguistic Society.

  • Parker, S. (2008). Sound level protrusions as physical correlates of sonority. Journal of Phonetics, 1, 55–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rauschecker, A. M., Pringle, A., & Watkins, K. E. (2008). Changes in neural activity associated with learning to articulate novel auditory pseudowords by covert repetition. Human Brain Mapping, 29, 1231–1242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redford, M. A. (2008). Production constraints on learning novel onset phonotactics. Cognition, 107(3), 785–816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rottschy, C., Langner, R., Dogan, I., Reetz, K., Laird, A. R., Schulz, J. B., Fox, P. T., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2012). Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate-based meta- analysis. NeuroImage, 60, 830–846.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schoor, A., Aichert, I., & Ziegler, W. (2012). A motor learning perspective on phonetic syllable kinships: how training effects transfer from learned to new syllables in severe apraxia of speech. Aphasiology, 26(7), 880–894.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segawa, J.A. (2013). Neural representations used by brain regions underlying speech production. Ph.D. Thesis, Boston University.

  • Segawa, J. A., Masapollo, M., Tong, M., Smith, D. J., & Guenther, F. H. (2019). Chunking of phonological units in speech sequencing. Brain & Language, 195, 104636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segawa, J. A., Tourville, J. A., Beal, D. S., & Guenther, F. H. (2015). The neural correlates of speech motor sequence learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(4), 819–831.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shamsi, A., Masapollo, M., Meyer, R., & Wayland, R. (2023). Motor practice stabilizes the temporal coordination of articulatory movements for non-native onset clusters. In Poster presented at the 2023 Boston speech motor control symposium, Boston, MA.

  • Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (1979). Speech errors as evidence for a serial ordering mechanism in sentence production. In W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walker (Eds.), Sentence processing (pp. 295–342). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (1983). Sublexical units and suprasegmental structure in speech production planning. In P. F. MacNeilage (Ed.), The production of speech (pp. 109–136). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (1992). The role of word structure in segmental serial ordering. Cognition, 42, 213–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, J. A., & Davidson, L. (2011). Perceptual similarity in input-output mappings: A computational/experimental study of non-native speech production. Lingua, 121(8), 1344–1358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, J. A., & Kawahara, S. (2018). Assessing surface phonological specification through simulation and classification of phonetic trajectories. Phonology, 35(3), 481–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. R., & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: Comparisons of speech and typewriting. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 117–152). Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tiede, M., Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., Johnson, B., Ghosh, S., Matthies, M., Zandiour, M., & Perkell, J. Gestural phasing in /kt/ sequences contrasting within and cross word contexts. Proceedings of the XVI International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 521–524.

  • Treiman, R., Fowler, C. A., Gross, J., Berch, D., & Weatherston, S. (1995). Syllable structure or word structure? Evidence for onset and rime units with disyllabic and trisyllabic stimuli. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(1), 132–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (2005). Increases in phonotactic probability facilitate spoken nonword repetition. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 193–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, G. (2002). A recency-based account of the list length effect in free recall. Memory and Cognition, 30, 885–892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warker, J. A., & Dell, G. S. (2006). Speech errors reflect newly learned phonotactic constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 387–398.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wickelgren, W. A. (1969). Context-sensitive coding, associative memory, and serial order in (speech) behavior. Psychological Review, 76, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, C., Davidson, L., & Martin, S. (2014). Effects of acoustic-phonetic detail on cross-language speech production. Journal of Memory and Language, 77, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, D. L., Robin, D., Rhee, J., Vaculin, A., Jacks, A., Guenther, F. H., & Fox, P. T. (2009). Using the self-select paradigm to delineate the nature of speech motor programming. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 755–765.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Barbara Holland, Farwa Faheem, Tess Fairchild, Kaylee Tran, Riccardo Falsini, Cassandra Chappell, and Nicholas Salazar, for assistance with participant recruitment, data collection, and coding. This work benefited from helpful discussions with, or comments from, Susan Nittrouer, Jason Bohland, Jason Tourville, Stefanie Shattuck-Hufnagel, Gary Dell, Jennifer Segawa, two anonymous reviewers, and audience members at the 2021 Boston Speech Motor Control Symposium and at the 178th Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America.

Funding

The work was supported by grants from the American Speech-Language-Hearing Foundation (New Investigators Research Grant; M. Masapollo, PI) and the National Institutes of Health (R01 DC007683; F.H. Guenther, PI). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthew Masapollo.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research reported in this article was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Masapollo, M., Zezas, E., Shamsi, A. et al. Disentangling Effects of Memory Storage and Inter-articulator Coordination on Generalization in Speech Motor Sequence Learning. J Psycholinguist Res 52, 2181–2210 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-023-09998-5

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-023-09998-5

Keywords

Navigation