Skip to main content
Log in

Inter-character Orthographic Similarity Effects on the Recognition of Chinese Coordinative Compound Words

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Chinese coordinative compound words are common and unique in inter-character semantic and orthographic relationships. This study explored the inter-character orthographic similarity effects on the recognition of transparent two-morpheme coordinative compound words. Seventy-two native Chinese readers participated in a lexical decision task. The findings demonstrated robust inhibitory inter-character orthographic similarity effects, intra-word character reversal effects, and inter-character semantic similarity effects. These results were compared to those of previous studies on coordinative compound word recognition and on the orthographic similarity phenomenon at both character and word levels. The findings were explained with the multi-level representational model of morphological processing of Chinese compound words (Zhou and Marslen-Wilson in Psychologia 43:47–66, 2000). The model was further extended by adding the activation of morpho-orthographic relationships and the mapping of morphemic orthographic information onto the semantic information of both morphemes and whole words.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In this study, inter-character and intra-word are used interchangeably with the same concept except for slight difference depending on immediate contexts.

  2. Total percentage is less than 100% because the scholar could not identify the structure of some compound words (Chen and Duanmu 2016).

  3. Mok (2009) defined the degree of semantic transparency in terms of the relationship between the compound word and its constituent morphemes. Because the notions of “transparent” and “opaque” “vary along a single semantic continuum” rather than dichotomous (Mok 2009, p. 1042), we deliberately focused on those coordinative compound words whose “meanings are related to the combined meaning of both constituents (i.e., fully transparent)” (p. 1042) (see more in “The Current Study” section).

  4. A reviewer pointed out the importance of the base rates of orthographic similarity and dissimilarity when two morphemes have the same semantics, considering that native speakers may have an implicit knowledge of the likelihoods and that this might affect the way that similarity information informs word recognition. Unfortunately, we have not found any literature or statistics identifying base rates that determine orthographic similarities or dissimilarities in Chinese coordinative compound words.

  5. It is to note that for non-coordinative compounds, there was no situation where the two characters share orthographic similarity.

  6. Because orthographic similarity was our primary interest point in this study, the nonwords were created in O+ and O− conditions as well. It would be insightful to replicate this inter-character semantic similarity effect with other types of nonwords in lexical decision task in the future. We appreciate reviewer’s insight on this point.

  7. It is also possible that at a post-lexical processing stage after initial visual activation, there might be an excitatory connection between O and S. We appreciate reviewer’s insight on this point. This should be addressed in a follow-up study.

References

  • Bi, H. Y., Hu, W., & Weng, X. C. (2006). Orthographic neighborhood effects in the pronunciation of Chinese words. Acta Psychologica Sinica,38, 7–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, L.-Y., & Perfetti, C. A. (2018). Visual factors in writing system variation: Measurement and implications for reading. In H. K. Pae (Ed.), Writing systems, reading processes, and cross-linguistic influences: Reflections from the Chinese, Japanese and Korean languages. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, C., & Duanmu, S. (2016). 双音节复合词内部语素的词类标注和统计分析 [Word class notation and statistical analyses on internal morphemes of Chinese disyllabic compound words]. 汉语学习 [Chinese language learning]. Issue 1.

  • Chung, K. K. H., Tong, X., Liu, P. D., McBride-Chang, C., & Meng, X. (2010). The processing of morphological structure information in Chinese coordinative compounds: An event-related potential study. Brain Research,1352, 157–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, L. B., & Siok, W. W. T. (1999). Semantic radicals contribute to the visual identification of Chinese characters. Journal of Memory and Language,40(4), 559–576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gu, J., Li, X., & Liversedge, S. P. (2015). Character order processing in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,41(1), 127–137.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, H. W., Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Lee, C. L., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2006). Orthographic neighborhood effects in reading Chinese two-character words. NeuroReport,17(10), 1061–1065.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, H. W., Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2011). Sublexical ambiguity effect in reading Chinese disyllabic compounds. Brain and Language,117(2), 77–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inquisit 5 [Computer software]. (2016). Retrieved August 5, 2019 from https://www.millisecond.com.

  • Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist,41, 161–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, M.-F., Gao, X.-Y., Chou, T.-L., & Wu, J.-T. (2017). Neighborhood frequency effect in Chinese word recognition: Evidence from naming and lexical decision. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research,46(1), 227–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, M. F., Lin, W. C., Chou, T. L., Yang, F. L., & Wu, J. T. (2015). The role of orthographic neighborhood size effects in Chinese word recognition. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research,44(3), 219–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, Q. L., Bi, H. Y., & Zhang, J. X. (2010). Neural correlates of the orthographic neighborhood size effect in Chinese. European Journal of Neuroscience,32(5), 866–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, P. D., & McBride-Chang, C. (2010). Morphological processing of Chinese compounds from a grammatical view. Applied Psycholinguistics,31, 605–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, Y.-C., Chen, H.-Y., Lai, Y. C., & Wu, D. H. (2015). Phonological similarity and orthographic similarity affect probed serial recall of Chinese characters. Memory and Cognition,43(3), 538–554.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lupker, S. J. (2005). Visual word recognition: Theories and findings. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 39–60). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., & DE Rumelhart, (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review,88(5), 375–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mok, L. W. (2009). Word-superiority effect as a function of semantic transparency of Chinese bimorphemic compound words. Language and Cognitive Processes,24(7–8), 1039–1081.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Packard, J. L. (2000). The morphology of Chinese: A linguistic and cognitive approach. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review,96(4), 523–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M. (1994). Interactive-activation as a framework for understanding morphological processing. Language and Cognitive Processes,9(3), 271–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., & Forster, K. J. (1976). Lexical storage and retrieval of polymorphemic and polysyllabic words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,15(6), 607–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., Zhu, X., & Peng, D. (1999). Positional specificity of radicals in Chinese character recognition. Journal of Memory and Language,40(4), 498–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, L. H., & Perfetti, C. A. (1997). Visual Chinese character recognition: Does phonological information mediate access to meaning? Journal of Memory and Language,37(1), 41–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, W. (2001). Lexical access to Chinese coordinative words. Acta Psychologica Sinica,33(2), 117–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, B. Y., & Peng, D. L. (1992). Decomposed storage in the Chinese lexicon. In H.-C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, J. (2005). 汉语词汇结构论 [The Structures of Chinese Lexicon]. Shanghai: Shanghai Lexicographical Publishing House.

  • Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1995). Morphological Structure in the Chinese Mental Lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes,10(6), 545–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2000). Lexical representation of compound words: Cross-linguistic evidence. Psychologia,43, 47–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, X., Marslen-Wilson, W., Taft, M., & Shu, H. (1999). Morphology, orthography, and phonology in reading Chinese compound words. Language and Cognitive Processes,14(5–6), 525–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Jing Sun or Hye K. Pae.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix: List of the Word Stimuli

Appendix: List of the Word Stimuli

No.

O+ TCC

O− TCC

Non-coordinative compounds

1

通过

按照

重点

2

思想

分别

战略

3

认识

事业

推动

4

眼睛

利益

银行

5

继续

材料

营养

6

价值

共同

将来

7

迅速

城市

财政

8

意思

音乐

到底

9

机械

真正

既然

10

剥削

创作

读者

11

疾病

年代

增强

12

婚姻

帮助

想象

13

热烈

家庭

元素

14

议论

模仿

欣赏

15

钢铁

身体

宪法

16

评论

整齐

眼前

17

抵抗

选择

道理

18

寂寞

错误

电视

19

艰难

颤抖

物理

20

船舶

消灭

总理

21

遥远

责任

见到

22

疼痛

停止

抓住

23

潮湿

固定

好像

24

饥饿

奇怪

英雄

25

跳跃

季节

心情

26

睡眠

锻炼

电话

27

挖掘

黑暗

统计

28

话语

居住

前提

29

捕捉

等待

结婚

30

吩咐

描绘

少数

31

谈论

温暖

真理

32

粗糙

包围

否则

33

逃避

尖锐

昨天

34

湖泊

和谐

医院

35

拥挤

旋转

当地

36

抚摸

街道

直线

37

场地

喜爱

逻辑

38

泛滥

明亮

专家

39

思念

珍贵

国外

40

安宁

喜悦

仔细

41

催促

补偿

完善

42

江河

寒冷

多数

43

扶持

恐惧

改进

44

拥抱

疯狂

抽象

45

衬衫

微弱

全体

46

病症

柔软

看法

47

保佑

采购

争取

48

祈祷

长久

身边

49

嫉妒

行走

冲突

50

清澈

验证

此时

51

编织

价钱

吃饭

52

冰冷

楼房

电影

53

伴侣

语文

能源

54

浑浊

猜测

佛教

55

妩媚

搜索

强度

56

缠绕

宽敞

脱离

57

呼叫

虚假

效率

58

践踏

破旧

有机

59

荒芜

言行

贵族

60

肥胖

首脑

地铁

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sun, J., Zhao, W. & Pae, H.K. Inter-character Orthographic Similarity Effects on the Recognition of Chinese Coordinative Compound Words. J Psycholinguist Res 49, 125–145 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-019-09674-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-019-09674-7

Keywords

Navigation