Skip to main content
Log in

Native-Language Phonological Interference in Early Hakka–Mandarin Bilinguals’ Visual Recognition of Chinese Two-Character Compounds: Evidence from the Semantic-Relatedness Decision Task

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous research has indicated that, in viewing a visual word, the activated phonological representation in turn activates its homophone, causing semantic interference. Using this mechanism of phonological mediation, this study investigated native-language phonological interference in visual recognition of Chinese two-character compounds by early Hakka–Mandarin bilinguals. A visual semantic-relatedness decision task in Chinese was given to native Mandarin speakers and early Hakka–Mandarin bilinguals. Both participant groups made more false positive errors and responded more slowly to the pair of two-character compounds containing a homophone; but only Hakka–Mandarin bilinguals made more false positive errors and responded more slowly to the pair containing a near-homophone. We concluded that phonology is needed in both native and nonnative speakers’ meaning access of Chinese two-character compounds and that native-language phonological interference is universal in L2 visual word recognition, not language type dependent; phonological and orthographic information are “interactive-compensatory” in helping Hakka readers’ resolve the interference.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. http://www.cncorpus.org.

References

  • Becker, C. A. (1976). Allocation of attention during visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 2, 556–566.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T. (1995). A direct realistic perspective on cross-language speech perception. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 171–204). Baltimore: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T., & Tyler, M. D. (2007). Non-native and second-language speech perception: Commonalities and complementarities. In O. Bohn & M. J. Munro (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning: In honor of James Emil Flege (pp. 13–34). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bosch, L., Costa, A., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2000). First and second language vowel perception in early bilinguals. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 189–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H. C., d’Arcais, G. B. F., & Cheung, S. L. (1995). Orthographic and phonological activation in recognizing Chinese characters. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 58(2), 144–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H.-C., & Shu, H. (2001). Lexical activation during the recognition of Chinese characters: Evidence against early phonological activation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(3), 511–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Constable, N. (2005). Guest people: Hakka identity in China and abroad. Seattle: University of Washington Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., & Otake, T. (2004). Pseudo-homophony in non-native listening. Paper presented to the 75th meeting of the Acoustic Society of America. New York: Acoustic Society of America.

  • Flege, J. (1995). Second-language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 233–277). Baltimore: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flege, J. E., & MacKay, I. R. A. (2004). Perceiving vowels in a second language. SSLA, 26, 1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forrest, R. A. D. (1948). The Chinese language. London: Faber and Faber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goto, H. (1971). Auditory perception by normal Japanese adults of the sounds “l” and “r”. Neuropsychologia, 9, 317–323.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B. (1972). One second of reading. In J. A. Kavanagh & I. G. Mattingly (Eds.), Language by eye and by ear (pp. 331–358). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guo, Y., & Burgund, E. D. (2010). Task effects in the mid-fusiform gyrus: A comparison of orthographic, phonological, and semantic processing of Chinese characters. Brain and Language, 115(2), 113–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halderman, L. K., Ashby, J., & Perfetti, C. A. (2012). An early and integral role in identifying words. Visual word recognition volume 1: Models and methods, orthography and phonology, 1, 207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hashimoto, M. J. (2010). The Hakka dialect: A linguistic study of its phonology, syntax and lexicon (Princeton/Cambridge Studies in Chinese Linguistics). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Havik, E., Roberts, L., van Hout, R., Schreuder, R., & Haverkort, M. (2009). Processing subject-object ambiguities in the L2: A self-paced reading study with German L2 learners of Dutch. Language Learning, 59(1), 73–112.

  • Humphreys, G. W., Evett, L. J., & Taylor, D. E. (1982). Automatic phonological priming in visual word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 10, 576–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingvalson, E. M., & Ettlinger, M. (2014). Bilingual speech perception and learning: A review of recent trends. International Journal of Bilingualism, 18(1), 35–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P., & Iverson, P. (1995). Linguistic experience and the “perceptual magnet effect”. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 121–153). Baltimore, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lam, A., Perfetti, C. A., & Bell, L. (1991). Automatic phonetic transfer in bidialectal reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 299–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesch, M. E., & Pollatsek, A. (1993). Automatic access of semantic information by phonological codes in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 19, 285–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luo, C. R., Johnson, R. A., & Gallo, D. A. (1998). Automatic activation of phonological information in reading: Evidence from the semantic relatedness decision task. Memory and Cognition, 26, 833–843.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Chen, H. C. (2003). Reading development in Chinese children. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mei, L., Xue, G., Lu, Z.-L., Chen, C. S., Zhang, M. X., He, Q. H., et al. (2014). Learning to read words in a new language shapes the neural organization of the prior languages. Neuropsychologia, 65, 156–168.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miyawaki, K., Strange, W., Verbrugge, R., Liberman, A. L., Jenkins, J. J., & Fujimura, O. (1975). An effect of linguistic experience: The discrimination of [r] and [l] by native speakers of Japanese and English. Perception and Psychophysics, 18, 331–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nixon, J. S., Chen, Y., & Schiller, N. O. (2015). Multi-level processing of phonetic variants in speech production and visual word processing: Evidence from Mandarin lexical tones. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 491–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ota, M., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Haywood, S. L. (2009). The KEY to the ROCK: Near-homophony in nonnative visual word recognition. Cognition, 111, 263–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ota, M., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Haywood, S. L. (2010). Is a FAN always FUN? Phonological and orthographic effects in bilingual visual word recognition. Language and Speech, 53(3), 383–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paap, K. R., Newsome, S. L., Mcdonald, L. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1982). An activation verification model for word and letter recognition: The word superiority effect. Psychological Review, 89, 573–594.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pallier, C., Colomé, A., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). The influence of native-language phonology on lexical access: Exemplar-based versus abstract lexical entries. Psychological Science, 12, 445–449.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pallier, C., Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (1997). A limit on behavioral plasticity in speech perception. Cognition, 64, 9–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Harris, L. N. (2013). Universal reading processes are modulated by language and writing system. Language Learning and Development, 9(4), 296–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (1998). The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 24, 101–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Zhang, S. (1995). Very early phonological activation in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 24–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Cao, F., & Booth, J. (2013). Specialization and universals in the development of reading skill: How Chinese research informs a universal science of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 5–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model: Some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 12(11), 43–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., Fiez, J., Nelson, J., Bolger, D. J., & Tan, L. H. (2007). Reading in two writing systems: Accommodation and assimilation of the brain’s reading network. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(2), 131–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2006). Reading Chinese characters: Orthography, phonology, meaning, and the lexical constituency model. In P. Li, L. H. Tan, E. Bates, & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), The handbook of East Asian psycholinguistics, Chinese (Vol. 1, pp. 225–236). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, H., Lewis, S. S., & Rubenstein, M. A. (1971). Evidence for phonemic recoding in visual word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 10, 645–658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Soto-Faraco, S. (1999). On-line processing of native and non-native phonemic contrasts in early bilinguals. Cognition, 72, 111–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shi, X. (2010). Vowel patterns of Chinese dialects. Beijing: Chinese Social Science Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1980). Toward an interactive compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, L. H., & Perfetti, C. A. (1997). Visual Chinese character recognition: Does phonological information mediate access to meaning? Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 41–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, L. H., & Perfetti, C. A. (1998). Phonological codes as early sources of constraint in Chinese word identification: A review of current discoveries and theoretical accounts. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 10, 165–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & Cognition, 15, 181–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C., Johnston, J. C., & Hale, B. L. (1988). Word identification in reading proceeds from spelling to sound to meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 14, 371–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C., Pennington, B., & Stone, G. (1990). Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics. Psychological Review, 97, 488–522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M., Koda, K., & Perfetti, C. A. (2003). Alphabetic and non-alphabetic L1 effects in English word identification: A comparison of Korean and Chinese English L2 learners. Cognition, 87, 129–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M., Lin, C. Y., & Yang, C. (2014). Contributions of phonology, orthography, and morphology in Chinese-English biliteracy acquisition: A one-Year longitudinal study. In X. Chen, Q. Wang, & Y. C. Luo (Eds.), Reading development and difficulties in monolingual and bilingual Chinese children (pp. 191–211). Berlin, Netherlands: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, A. W. K., & Chen, H. C. (2012). Is syntactic-category processing obligatory in visual word recognition? Evidence from Chinese. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 1334–1360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, A. W.-K., Wu, Y., & Chen, H.-C. (2014). Limited role of phonology in reading Chinese two-character compounds: Evidence from an ERP study. Neuroscience, 256, 342–351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamada, J. (2004). An L1-script-transfer-effect fallacy: A rejoinder to Wang et al. (2003). Cognition, 93, 127–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamada, R. A., & Tohkura, Y. I. (1990). Perception and production of syllable-initial English /r/ and /l/ by native speakers of Japanese. Proceedings of the 1990 international conference on spoken language processing, 757–760.

  • Yeung, S. S., & Ganotice, F. A. (2014). The role of phonological awareness in biliteracy acquisition among Hong Kong Chinese kindergarteners who learn English-as-a-second language (ESL). The Asia-Pacific Education Researcher, 23, 333–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, W. (2012). A standard dictionary of Hakka dialect. Guangzhou: Sun Yat-Sen University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, X., Samuel, A. G., & Liu, S. (2012). The perception and representation of segmental and prosodic Mandarin contrasts in native speakers of Cantonese. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 438–457.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1996). Direct visual access is the only way to access the chinese mental lexicon. In G. Cottrell (Ed.), Proceedings of the 18th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 714–719). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, X., Marslen-Wilson, W., Taft, M., & Shu, H. (1999). Morphology, orthography, and phonology in reading Chinese compound words. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14, 525–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shiyu Wu.

Additional information

This research was supported by The National Social Science Fund of China (Shiyu Wu) (Grant No. 14BYY007).

Appendix: List of Homophones, Minimal Pairs, and Visual Controls

Appendix: List of Homophones, Minimal Pairs, and Visual Controls

figure bh
figure bi

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wu, S., Ma, Z. Native-Language Phonological Interference in Early Hakka–Mandarin Bilinguals’ Visual Recognition of Chinese Two-Character Compounds: Evidence from the Semantic-Relatedness Decision Task. J Psycholinguist Res 46, 57–75 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-016-9420-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-016-9420-8

Keywords

Navigation