Skip to main content
Log in

A Behavioral Study of Regularity, Irregularity and Rules in the English Past Tense

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Opposing views of storage and processing of morphologically complex words (e.g., past tense) have been suggested: the dual system, whereby regular forms are not in the lexicon but are generated by rule, while irregular forms are explicitly represented; the single system, whereby regular and irregular forms are computed by a single system, using associative connections; and a system whereby phonological rules relate both regular and irregular past to present tense forms. Two reaction time experiments investigated the production of the past tense in English in response to the auditory presentation of the present tense of the verb. The first experiment addressed the methodology of presenting regulars and irregulars in blocked form as in a previous study (Jaeger et al. in Language 72:451–497, 1996). Blocked presentation results showed longer RTs for the elicitation of irregular pasts than for regular pasts; however, there were no differences between regular and irregular elicitation when the presentation was randomized, indicating that it is rules that are being primed. The second experiment tested whether the response time advantage found for blocked regular verbs in the first experiment might also extend to irregular verb forms exhibiting the same sub-regularity (e.g., sing-sang may prime ring-rang). Results showed a trend towards slower RTs when past tense forms from different sub-regularities follow one another, suggesting interference between one sub-regularity and another.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Participants’ uncertainty about the correct form of the past tense led to the redesign of the Experiment 2. In the original design of Experiment 2, stimuli were to be presented in small blocks of similar verbs, varying from two to five items: for regular verbs, those exhibiting the same allophone and for irregular, those following the same sub-regularity. On the assumption that priming would occur from one item within a sub-regularity to the next, we had originally hypothesized that participants would show speeded responses over the course of a block. What was unexpected was the number of errors participants made even when a verb’s sub-regularity had been primed by the verb immediately preceding it. Because the presence of an error would diminish the advantage inherent in sub-regularity, any block containing an error had to be eliminated. Eliminating blocks containing errors then led to the elimination of seven participants for whom there were fewer than 25 responses in a condition once blocks containing a non-technical error were eliminated. In addition, for the remaining 24 participants, another 25 % of the data, 286 of the 1,152 potential blocks, had to be eliminated. As a result, Experiment 2 was redesigned, and data were coded, as described above, according to the morphological relation of each verb to the verb immediately preceding it.

References

  • Albright, A., & Hayes, B. (2003). Rules vs. analogy in English past tenses: A computational/experimental study. Cognition, 90, 119–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ambidge, B. (2010). Children’s judgments of regular and irregular novel past-tense forms: New data on the English past-tense debate. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1497–1504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderwald, L. (2011). Norm vs. variation in British English irregular verbs: The case of past tense sang vs. sung. English Language and, Linguistics, 15, 85–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, L. (1997). A class of English irregular verbs. English Studies, 6, 545–555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., & Moscoso del Prado Martín, F. (2005). Semantic density and past-tense formation in three Germanic languages. Language, 81, 666–698.

  • Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloch, B. (1947). English verb inflection. Language, 23, 399–418. Reprinted in readings in linguistics I ed. by M. Joos, 243–151. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1966.

  • Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 425–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J., & Moder, C. (1983). Morphological classes as natural categories. Language, 59, 251–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J., & Slobin, D. (1982). Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language, 58, 265–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B., Davies, P., & Richman, B. (1971). American Heritage word frequency book. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.

  • Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound patterns of English. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clahsen, H., Hadler, M., & Weyerts, H. (2004). Speeded production of inflected words in children and adults. Journal of Child Language, 31, 683–712.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eddington, D. (2000). Analogy and the dual-route model of morphology. Lingua, 110, 281–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Embick, D., & Marantz, A. (2005). Cognitive neuroscience and the English past tense: comments on the paper by Ullman. Brain and Language, 93, 243–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, C. A., Napps, S. E., & Feldman, L. (1985). Relations among regular and irregular morphologically related words in the lexicon as revealed by repetition priming. Memory & Cognition, 13, 241–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halle, M., & Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In K. Hale & S. J. Keyser (Eds.), The view from building 20. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halle, M., & Mohanan, K. P. (1985). Segmental phonology of Modern English. Linguistic Inquiry, 16, 57–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, Y. T., & Pinker, S. (2010). Lexical semantics and irregular inflection. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 1411–1461.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeger, J. J., Lockwood, A. H., Kemmerer, D. L., Van Valin, R. D., Murphy, B. W., & Khalak, H. G. (1996). A positron emission tomographic study of regular and irregular verb morphology in English. Language, 72, 451–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joanisse, M. F., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2005). Imaging the past: Neural activation in frontal and temporal region during regular and irregular past-tense processing. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 282–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempley, S. T., & Morton, J. (1982). The effects of priming with regularly and irregularly related words in auditory word recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 441–454.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kielar, A., Joanisse, M. F., & Hare, M. (2008). Priming English past tense verbs: Rules or statistics? Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 327–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kielar, A., & Joanisse, M. F. (2009). Graded effects of regularity in language revealed by N400 indices of morphological priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1373–1398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavric, A., Pizzagalli, D., Forstmeier, S., & Rippon, G. (2001). Mapping dissociations in verb morphology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 301–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B., & Leinbach, J. (1991). Implementations are not conceptualizations: Revising the verb learning model. Cognition, 39, 121–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M.T., Hollander, M., Rosen, T.J. & Xu, F. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57, Serial No. 228. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  • Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (1998). Rules, representations, and the English past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 428–435.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., & Patterson, K. (2002). Rules or connections in past-tense inflections: What does the evidence rule out? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 465–472.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miozzo, M. (2003). On the processing of regular and irregular forms of verbs and nouns: Evidence from neuropsychology. Cognition, 87, 101–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Snowling, M. J., & Clarke, P. (2005). Production of the English past tense by children with language comprehension impairments. Journal of Child Language, 32, 117–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pastizzo, M. J., & Feldman, L. B. (2002). Discrepancies between orthographic and unrelated baselines in masked priming undermine a decompositional account of morphological facilitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 244–249.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules: The ingredients of language. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1991). Rules of language. Science, 253, 530–535.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prasada, S., & Pinker, S. (1993). Generalisation of regular and irregular morphological patterns. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 1–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., & Ullman, M. T. (2002). The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 456–462.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramscar, M. (2002). The role of meaning in inflection: Why the past tense does not require a rule. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 45–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rueckl, J. G., & Galantucci, B. (2005). The locus and time course of long-term morphological priming. Language and Language Processes, 20, 115–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rueckl, J. G., Mikolinski, M., Raveh, M., Miner, C. S., & Mars, F. (1997). Morphological priming, fragment completion, and connectionist networks. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 382–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tenses of English verbs: Implicit rules or parallel distributed processing? In J. L., McClelland, D. E., Rumehart, the PDP Research Group (Eds.) Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (pp. 216–271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Seidenberg, M. S. (1992). Connectionism without tears. In S. Davis (Ed.), Connectionism: Theory and practice (pp. 84–122). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., & Bruck, M. (1990). Consistency effects in the generation of past tense morphology. Paper presented at the thirty-first meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans, November 16–18.

  • Seidenberg, M. S., & Hoeffner, J. H. (1998). Evaluating behavioral and neuroimaging data on past tense processing. Language, 74, 104–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., Palumbo, L. C., Fulbright, R. K., Mencl, W. E., Van Dyke, J., Kollia, B., et al. (2010). Testing the limits of language production in long-term survivors of major stroke: A psycholinguistic and anatomic study. Aphasiology, 24, 1455–1485.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanners, R. F., Neiser, J. J., Hernon, W. P., & Hall, R. (1979). Memory representation for morphologically related words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 399–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tieken-Boon van Ostade, I. (2002). Robert Lowth and the strong verb system. Language Sciences, 24, 459–469.

  • Ullman, M. T. (2001). The declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 37–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T., Pancheva, R., Love, T., Yee, E., Swinney, D., & Hickok, G. (2005). Neural correlates of lexicon and grammar: Evidence from the production, reading and judgment of inflection in aphasia. Brain and Language, 93, 185–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfram, W., & Shilling-Estes, (2006). American English. Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yang, C. (2002). Knowledge and learning in natural language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIH Grant HD-01994 to Haskins Laboratories and grants from the Rhode Island College Faculty Research Committee. The author thanks Matthew Richardson, an anonymous reviewer, D. H. Whalen, Saudamini Roy, Julie Brown, Steve Frost, Doug Honorof, Marianne Pouplier, Ken Pugh, Einar Mencl, Jay Rueckl, and Charles Yang, for helpful discussions and technical assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Harriet S. Magen.

Appendices

Appendix 1

See Table 3.

Table 3 Word lists for Experiment 1

Appendix 2

See Table 4.

Table 4 Word lists for Experiment 2

Appendix 3

See Table 5.

Table 5 List of stimulus items used in Experiment 2

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Magen, H.S. A Behavioral Study of Regularity, Irregularity and Rules in the English Past Tense. J Psycholinguist Res 43, 791–814 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-013-9276-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-013-9276-0

Keywords

Navigation