Skip to main content
Log in

Exploring Telicity and Transitivity in Primordial Thought Language and Body Boundary Imagery

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Linguistics research on ‘unconscious knowledge’ related to the right brain-hemisphere represents a shift from the prevalent scientific investigation of the linguistic processes of grammatical structures associated with the dominant ‘verbal’ left brain-hemisphere. This study explores the relationship among primordial thought language, body boundary awareness and syntactic features—i.e. telicity, perfectivity and transitivity—in autobiographical narratives of everyday and dream memories. The results showed that event descriptions with atelic predicates and intransitive structures were more frequent in dream recall than in narratives of everyday memories. Primordial thought language and body boundary awareness, however, decreased with atelic predicates and transitive structures, which might indicate both the tendency of events to describe result states, such as achievements and accomplishments, as a means to bring about an unconscious wish fulfilment and the emphasis on event arguments to be realised without the inclusion of an external object. In narratives of everyday memories, penetration imagery increased with imperfective verb forms and decreased with perfective verb forms, and emotion lexis increased with atelic predicates and transitive structures, but not in dream memories.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anzieu, D. (1989). The skin ego. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argamon, S., & Levitan S. (2005). Measuring the usefulness of function words for authorship attribution. In Proceedings of the 2005 ACH/ALLC Conference. Victoria, Canada. Retrieved from http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.71.6935

  • Banich, M. T. (2004). Cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burris, C. T., & Rempel, J. K. (2004). ’It’s the end of the world as we know it’: Threat and the spatial-symbolic self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 19–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cariola, L. A. (2013). Assessing the inter-method reliability and correlational validity of the Body Type Dictionary (BTD). Literary and Linguistic Computing. Retrieved from http://www.llc.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2013/06/05/llc.fqt025.full.pdf

  • Cattell, R. (2006). An introduction to mind, consciousness and language. London: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, C. K., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2007). The psychological functions of function words. In K. Fiedler (Ed.), Social communication (pp. 343–359). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Comrie, B. (1976). Aspect: An introduction to the study of verbal aspect and related problems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M. A., & Bekerian, D. A. (1987). Organisation in autobiographical memory. Memory and Cognition, 15, 119–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Croft, W. (2012). Verbs: Aspect and causal structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dowty, D. (1979). Thematic proto-roles and argument selection. Language, 67, 547–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fairclough, N. (1992). Discourse and social change. Cambridge: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C. J. (1968). The case for case. Universak in Linguistic Theory. In E. Bach & R. T. Harms (Eds.), Linguistic theory (pp. 1–88). New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, S., & Cleveland, S. E. (1956). Body-image boundaries and style of life. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 373–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, S., & Cleveland, S. E. (1958). Body image and personality. New York: Dover Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foulkes, S. H. (1965). Therapeutic group analysis. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, J. (2005). The R commander: A basic statistics graphical user interface to R. Journal of Statistical Software, 14, 1–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (2001). The interpretation of dreams, S.E. 4. London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1900).

  • Guimón, J. (1997). Corporality and psychoses. In J. Guimón (Ed.), The body in psychotherapy (pp. 63–72). Basel: Karger.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hines, D., & Martindale, C. (1973). Functional brain asymmetry, primary process thinking, and natural language. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 34, 773.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogenraad, R., Daubies, C., Bestgen, Y., & Mahau, P. (2003). Une théorie et une méthode générale d’analyse textuelle assistée par ordinateur. Le système PROTAN (PROTocol ANalyzer). 32-bits version of November 10. (2003). by Pierre Mahau. Louvain-la-Neuve: Psychology Department, Catholic University of Louvain.

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1999). Producing spoken language: A blueprint of the speaker. In Colin M. Brown & P. Hagoort (Eds.), The neurocognition of language (pp. 83–122). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martindale, C. (1975a). Romantic progression: The psychology of literary history. Washington: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martindale, C. (1975b). The grammar of altered states of consciousness: A semiotic reinterpretation of aspects of psychoanalytic theory. Psychoanalysis and Contemporary Science, 4, 331–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martindale, C. (1976). Primitive mentality and the relationship between art and society. Scientific Aesthetics, 1, 5–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martindale, C. (1990). The clockwork muse: The predictability of artistic change. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martindale, C., Covello, E., & West, A. (1986). Primary process cognition and hemisphere asymmetry. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 147, 79–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martindale, C., & Fisher, R. (1977). The effects of psilocybin on primary process content in language. Confinia Psychiatrica, 20, 195–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matlock, T. (2011). The conceptual motivation of aspect. In K. Panther & G. Radden (Eds.), Motivation in grammar and the lexicon (pp. 133–147). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Matte-Blanco, I. (1959). Expression in symbolic logic of the characteristics of the system Ucs. Or the logic of the system Ucs. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 40, 1–5.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matte-Blanco, I. (1975). The unconscious as infinite sets. London: Duckworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. L., Moskovitz, D. J., & Steiner, H. (2008). Narration and vividness as measures of event-specificity in autobiographical memory. Discourse Processes, 45, 195–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Dwyer, B. T. (2006). Modern english structures: Form, function, and position. Ontario: Broadview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, E. (1995). Unconscious logic: An introduction to Matte Blanco’s bi-logic and its uses. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saraceni, C., Ruggeri, G., & Filocamo, D. (1980). Studio sperimentale con il test do Rorschach sulle modificazioni dell’immagine corporea in ipnosi. Archivio di Psicologia, Neirologia e Psichitria, 41, 50–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmeidler, G., & LeShan, L. (1970). An aspect of body image related to ESP scores. Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 64, 211–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C. (1904). The proof and measurement of association between two things. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 72–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trimble, M. R. (2007). The soul in the brain: The cerebral basis of language, art, and belief. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tustin, F. (1981). Autistic states in children. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vendler, Z. (1967). Linguistics in philosophy. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, L. (2006). Aspectual bootstrapping in language acquisition: Telicity and transitivity. Language Learning and Development, 2, 51–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: International University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, A. (1991). Primary process content in the King James Bible: The five stages of Christian mysticism. Computers and the Humanities, 25, 227–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West, A., & Martindale, C. (1988). Primary process content in paranoid schizophrenic speech. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 149, 547–553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • West, A., Martindale, C., Hines, D., & Roth, W. (1983). Marijuana-induced primary process content in the TAT. Journal of Personality Assessment, 47, 466–467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • West, A., Martindale, C., & Sutton-Smith, B. (1985). Age trends in the content of children’s spontaneous fantasy narratives. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 111, 391–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, A. (2002). The application of computer-content analysis in sexology: A case study of primary process content in fictional fetishistic narratives. Electronic Journal of Human Sexuality, \(5\). Retrieved from http://www.ejhs.org/volume5/wilson.html

  • Wilson, A. (2006). The development and application of a content analysis dictionary for body boundary research. Literary and Linguistic Computing, 21, 105–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, A. (2008). Psychosemiotic cycles and the liturgical year: A case study and framework for research. Göttingen: Cuvillier Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, A. (2009). Barrier and penetration imagery in altered states of consciousness discourse: replicating the five-stage model of Christian mysticism in the Bible. In W. Oleksy & P. Stalmaszczyk (Eds.), Cognitive approaches to language and linguistic data: Studies in honor of Barbara Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, Polish studies in English Literature (Vol. 27, pp. 357–372). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, A. (2010). The regressive imagery dictionary: A test of its concurrent validity in English, German, Latin, and Portuguese. Literary and Linguistic Computing, 26, 125–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wodak, R. (1981). Das Wort in der Gruppe: Linguistische Studien zur therapeutischen Kommunikation. Vienna: Akademie der Wissenschaft.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I would like to express my gratitude to my research assistant Bart Modderkolk, to my supervisor Dr. Andrew Wilson and also to Dr. Daniël Van Olmen for their help and support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura A. Cariola.

Appendix

Appendix

See Appendix Tables 2 and 3.

Table 2 Semantic categories and examples of primary and secondary process language in the RID (Martindale 1975a, 1990)
Table 3 Semantic categories and examples of barrier and penetration imagery in the BTD (Wilson 2006), including all clothing items, vehicles and buildings

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cariola, L.A. Exploring Telicity and Transitivity in Primordial Thought Language and Body Boundary Imagery. J Psycholinguist Res 43, 683–697 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-013-9272-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-013-9272-4

Keywords

Navigation