Skip to main content
Log in

Maternal Touch as a Channel of Communication at Age Four Months: Variations by Infant Gender and Maternal Depression

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Tactile contact is one of the earliest nonverbal channels through which parents shape the ways their sons and daughters feel in their own bodies and experience the relational world. In this study, we sought to explore the nuances of early tactile communication by examining a community sample of 126 mothers during interactions with their 4-month-old infants. Mothers and their infants were videotaped in the laboratory while engaging in face-to-face play. Each second of the first 2.5 min of videotaped interaction was assigned a 10-digit code for maternal tactile behavior, using the COSYMIT (Coding System for Mother-Infant Touch) to identify specific modes of touch. We then statistically examined the possibility that maternal tactile behavior would differ according to infant gender and maternal depression status. We found that infant gender and maternal depression were each significant predictors of mother-infant tactual patterns. Mothers interacting with a boy touched central body locations and varied their touch more than did those interacting with a girl. Mothers who reported depressive symptoms engaged in substantially less affectionate and more object-mediated touch than their non-depressed counterparts. These findings suggest that tactile behavior may serve as an early prelinguistic channel by which mothers transmit affective and sociocultural “messages” to their infants.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of Data and Materials

New York State Psychiatric Institute.

Code Availability

Not applicable.

Notes

  1. Werner’s core ideas were later elaborated and enriched considerably in his joint work with his protégé Bernard Kaplan as presented in their coauthored landmark book Symbol Formation (Wiley, 1963).

  2. Although the groups were labeled Black, Latina, and White, a closer examination of maternal self-reported ethnicity suggested that these categories would be more accurately described as Black (non-Latina), Latina (Black or White), and White (non-Latina).

  3. In subsequent research (e.g., Beebe et al., 2010) we have successfully used a reduced, 11-point variation on this scale, but in the present study we opted to use the more comprehensive (16-category) scale.

References

  • App, B., McIntosh, D. N., Reed, C. L., & Hertenstein, M. J. (2011). Nonverbal channel use in communication of emotion: How may depend on why. Emotion, 11(3), 603–617. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023164

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arteche, A., Joormann, J., Harvey, A., Craske, M., Gotlib, I. H., Lehtonen, A., Counsell, N., & Stein, A. (2011). The effects of postnatal maternal depression and anxiety on the processing of infant faces. Journal of Affective Disorders, 133(1–2), 197–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2011.04.015

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Aznar, A., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2016). Parent-child positive touch: Gender, age, and task differences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 40(4), 317–333. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-016-0236-x

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe, B. (2000). Co-constructing mother-infant distress: The microsynchrony of maternal impingement and infant avoidance in the face-to-face encounter. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 20(3), 421–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe, B. (2014). My journey in infant research and psychoanalysis: Microanalysis, a social microscope. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 31(1), 4–25. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035575

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe, B., Jaffe, J., Buck, K., Chen, H., Cohen, P., Feldstein, S., & Andrews, H. (2008). Six-week postpartum maternal depressive symptoms and 4-month mother-infant self- and interactive contingency. Infant Mental Health Journal, 29(5), 442–471. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20191

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe, B., Jaffe, J., Markese, S., Buck, K., Chen, H., Cohen, P., Bahrick, L., Andrews, H., & Feldstein, S. (2010). The origins of 12-month attachment: A microanalysis of 4-month mother-infant interaction. Attachment & Human Development, 12(1–2), 3–141. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730903338985

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe, B. & Lachmann, F. (2014). The origins of attachment: Infant research and adult treatment. Routledge.

  • Beebe, B., Lachmann, F., & Jaffe, J. (1997a). A transformational model of presymbolic representations: Reply to commentaries. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 7(2), 215–224. https://doi.org/10.1080/10481889709539177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe, B., Lachmann, F., & Jaffe, J. (1997b). Mother–infant interaction structures and presymbolic self- and object representations. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 7(2), 133–182. https://doi.org/10.1080/10481889709539172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe, B., Lachmann, F., Markese, S., Buck, K. A., Bahrick, L. E., Chen, H., Cohen, P., Andrews, H., Feldstein, S., & Jaffe, J. (2012). On the origins of disorganized attachment and internal working models: Paper II. An empirical microanalysis of 4-month mother-infant interaction. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 22(3), 352–374. https://doi.org/10.1080/10481885.2012.679606

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, R. H., Bolling, D. Z., Anderson, L. C., Pelphrey, K. A., & Kaiser, M. D. (2014). fNIRS detects temporal lobe response to affective touch. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(4), 470–476.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bigelow, A. E., & Power, M. (2020). Mother-infant skin-to-skin contact: Short- and long-term effects for mothers and their children born full-term. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01921.

  • Bohne, A., Nordahl, D., Lindahl, Å. A., Ulvenes, P., Wang, C. E., & Pfuhl, G. (2021). Emotional infant face processing in women with major depression and expecting parents with depressive symptoms. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.657269

  • Bremner, A. J., & Spence, C. (2017). The development of tactile perception. In J. B. Benson (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 227–268). Elsevier Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carozza, S., & Leong, V. (2021). The role of affectionate caregiver touch in early neurodevelopment and parent-infant interactional synchrony. Frontiers in Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2020.613378

  • Cascio, C. J., Moore, D., & McGlone, F. (2019). Social touch and human development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 35, 5–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.009

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Y., Becker, B., Zhang, Y., Cui, H., Du, J., Wernicke, J., Montag, C., Kendrick, K. M., & Yao, S. (2020). Oxytocin increases the pleasantness of affective touch and orbitofrontal cortex activity independent of valence. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 39, 99–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2020.08.003

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, J. F., & Campbell, S. B. (1991). Influence of maternal depression on infant affect regulation. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology. A developmental approach to affective disorders. (Vol. 4). Rochester: University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, J. F., Campbell, S. B., Matias, R., & Hopkins, J. (1990). Face-to-face interactions of postpartum depressed and nondepressed mother-infant pairs at 2 months. Developmental Psychology, 26(1), 15–23. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.1.15

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, J. F., Matias, R., Tronick, E. Z., Connell, D., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (1986). Face-to-face interactions of depressed mothers and their infants. In E. Z. Tronick & T. Field (Eds.), Maternal depression and infant disturbance (pp. 31–45). Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crucianelli, L., Wheatley, L., Filippetti, M. L., Jenkinson, P. M., Kirk, E., & Fotopoulou, A. K. (2019). The mindedness of maternal touch: An investigation of maternal mind-mindedness and mother-infant touch interactions. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 35, 47–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.01.010

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Downing, G. (2005). Emotion, body, and parent-infant interaction. In J. Nadel & D. Muir (Eds.), Emotional development: Recent research advances (pp. 429–449). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fairhurst, M. T., Löken, L., & Grossmann, T. (2014). Physiological and behavioral responses reveal 9-month-old infants’ sensitivity to pleasant touch. Psychological Science, 25(5), 1124–1131. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614527114

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R. (2011). Maternal touch and the developing infant. In M. J. Hertenstein & S. J. Weiss (Eds.), The handbook of touch: Neuroscience, behavioral, and health perspectives (pp. 373–407). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R., Singer, M., & Zagoory, O. (2010). Touch attenuates infants’ physiological reactivity to stress. Developmental Science, 13(2), 271–278. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00890.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferber, S. G. (2004). The nature of touch in mothers experiencing maternity blues: The contribution of parity. Early Human Development, 79(1), 65–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.04.011

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferber, S. G., Feldman, R., & Makhoul, I. R. (2008). The development of maternal touch across the first year of life. Early Human Development, 84(6), 363–370. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.09.019

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. M. (1995). Infant massage therapy. In T. Field (Ed.), Touch in early development (pp. 105–114). Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. (2002). Infants’ need for touch. Human Development, 45(2), 100–103. https://doi.org/10.1159/000048156

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. (2019). Social touch, CT touch and massage therapy: A narrative review. Developmental Review, 51, 123–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2019.01.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, L. K. (1957). Tactile communication. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 56, 209–255.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hardin, J. S., Jones, N. A., Mize, K. D., & Platt, M. (2020). Parent-training with kangaroo care impacts infant neurophysiological development & mother-infant neuroendocrine activity. Infant Behavior and Development. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101416

  • Harrison-Speake, K., & Willis, F. N. (1995). Ratings of the appropriateness of touch among family members. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 19(2), 85–100. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02173168

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haviland, J. M., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1981). The development of sex differences in nonverbal signals: Fallacies, facts, and fantasies. In C. Mayo & N. Henley (Eds.), Gender and nonverbal behavior (pp. 183–203). Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hentel, A. (2000, July 18). Maternal depression at 6 weeks is associated with infant self-comfort at 4 months. Paper presented at the International Congress of Infant Studies, Brighton, England.

  • Herrera, E., Reissland, N., & Shepherd, J. (2004). Maternal touch and maternal child-directed speech: Effects of depressed mood in the postnatal period. Journal of Affective Disorders, 81(1), 29–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2003.07.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertenstein, M. J. (2002). Touch: Its communicative functions in infancy. Human Development, 45(2), 70–94. https://doi.org/10.1159/000048154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertenstein, M. J., Butts, A., & Hile, S. (2007). The communication of anger: Beyond the face. In E. I. Clausen (Ed.), Psychology of anger (pp. 7–11). Nova Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertenstein, M. J., Keltner, D., App, B., Bulleit, B. A., & Jaskolka, A. R. (2006a). Touch communicates distinct emotions. Emotion, 6(3), 528–533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertenstein, M. J., Verkamp, J. M., Kerestes, A. M., & Holmes, R. M. (2006b). The communicative functions of touch in humans, nonhuman primates, and rats: A review and synthesis of the empirical research. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 132(1), 5–94. https://doi.org/10.3200/mono.132.1.5-94

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, N. A., & Mize, K. D. (2007). Touch interventions positively affect development. In L. L’Abate (Ed.), Low-cost approaches to promote physical and mental health (pp. 353–369). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jönsson, E. H., Kotilahti, K., Heiskala, J., Wasling, H. B., Olausson, H., Croy, I., Mustaniemi, H., Hiltunen, P., Tuulari, J. J., Scheinin, N. M., Karlsson, L., Karlsson, H., & Nissilä, I. (2018). Affective and non-affective touch evoke differential brain responses in 2-month-old infants. NeuroImage, 169, 162–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.024

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, P. S., Danko, C. M., Cejka, A. M., & Everhart, K. D. (2015). Maternal depression and the learning-promoting effects of infant-directed speech: Roles of maternal sensitivity, depression diagnosis, and speech acoustic cues. Infant Behavior & Development, 41, 52–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.06.011

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keren, M., Feldman, R., Eidelman, A. I., Sirota, L., & Lester, B. (2003). Clinical interview for high-risk parents of premature infants (CLIP) as a predictor of early disruptions in the mother-infant relationship at the nursery. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24(2), 93–110. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.10049

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knapp, M., Hall, J. A., & Horgan, T.G. (2021). The effects of touch on human communication. In Nonverbal communication in human interaction (9th edn., pp. 167–188). Kendall Hunt.

  • Kuzela, A. L., Stifter, C. A., & Worobey, J. (1990). Breastfeeding and mother-infant interactions. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 8(3), 185–194. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646839008403623

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lander, J., Brady-Fryer, B., Metcalfe, J. B., Nazarali, S., & Muttitt, S. (1997). Comparison of ring block, dorsal penile nerve block, and topical anesthesia for neonatal circumcision: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 278(24), 2157–2162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malatesta, C. Z., & Haviland, J. M. (1982). Learning display rules: The socialization of emotion expression in infancy. Child Development, 53(4), 991–1003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malphurs, J. E., Raag, T., Field, T., Pickens, J., & Pelaez-Nogueras, M. (1996). Touch by intrusive and withdrawn mothers with depressive symptoms. Early Development and Parenting, 5(2), 111–115. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1099-0917(199606)5:2%3c111::aid-edp122%3e3.0.co;2-#

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mantis, I., Mercuri, M., Stack, D. M., & Field, T. M. (2019). Depressed and non-depressed mothers’ touching during social interactions with their infants. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 35, 57–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.01.005

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marx, V., & Nagy, E. (2017). Fetal behavioral responses to the touch of the mother’s abdomen: A Frame-by-frame analysis. Infant Behavior and Development, 47, 83–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.03.005

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mateus, V., Osório, A., Miguel, H. O., Cruz, S., & Sampaio, A. (2021). Maternal sensitivity and infant neural response to touch: An fNIRS study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(12), 1256–1263. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab069

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miani, A., Di Bernardo, G. A., Højgaard, A. D., Earp, B. D., Zak, P. J., Landau, A. M., Hoppe, J., & Winterdahl, M. (2020). Neonatal male circumcision is associated with altered adult socio-affective processing. Heliyon. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e05566

  • Miguel, H. O., Lisboa, I. C., Goncalves, O., & Sampaio, A. (2019). Brain mechanisms for processing discriminative and affective touch in 7-month-old infants. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 35, 20–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.008

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montirosso, R., & McGlone, F. (2020). The body comes first. Embodied reparation and the co-creation of infant bodily-self. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 113, 77–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.003

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, A. J., Posada, G. E., & Goldyn, D. T. (2006). Presence and quality of touch influence coregulation in mother-infant dyads. Infancy, 9(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0901_1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, I., Löken, L. S., & Olausson, H. (2010). The skin as a social organ. Experimental Brain Research, 204(3), 305–314. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2007-y

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muir, D. W. (2002). Adult communications with infants through touch: The forgotten sense. Human Development, 45(2), 95–99. https://doi.org/10.1159/000048155

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., Kempton, C., Woolgar, M., & Hooper, R. (1993). Depressed mothers’ speech to their infants and its relation to infant gender and cognitive development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(7), 1083–1101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb01775.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nguyen, T., Abney, D. H., Salamander, D., Bertenthal, B. I., & Hoehl, S. (2021). Proximity and touch are associated with neural but not physiological synchrony in naturalistic mother-infant interactions. NeuroImage, 244, 118599. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118599

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peláez-Nogueras, M., Field, T., Gewirtz, J. L., Cigales, M., Gonzalez, A., Sanchez, A., & Richardson, S. C. (1997). The effects of systematic stroking versus tickling and poking on infant behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18(2), 169–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(97)90034-4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peláez-Nogueras, M., Field, T. M., Hossain, Z., & Pickens, J. (1996). Depressed mothers’ touching increases infants’ positive affect and attention in still-face interactions. Child Development, 67(4), 1780–1792. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131731

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pirazzoli, L., Lloyd-Fox, S., Braukmann, R., Johnson, M., & Gliga, T. (2019). Hand or spoon? Exploring the neural basis of affective touch in 5-month-old infants. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 35, 28–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.06.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385–401. https://doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shields, M., & Sparling, J. (1993). Fathers’ play and touch behavior with their three-month-old infants. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 13(1), 39–59. https://doi.org/10.1300/J006v13n01_03

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slomian, J., Honvo, G., Emonts, P., Reginster, J. Y., & Bruyère, O. (2019). Consequences of maternal postpartum depression: A systematic review of maternal and infant outcomes. Women’s Health, 15, 1745506519844044. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745506519844044

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stack, D. M., & Arnold, S. L. (1998). Changes in mothers’ touch and hand gestures influence infant behavior during face-to-face interchanges. Infant Behavior and Development, 21(3), 451–468. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90019-4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stack, D. M., & Jean, A. D. (2011). Communicating through touch: Touching during parent-infant interactions. In M. J. Hertenstein & S. J. Weiss (Eds.), The handbook of touch: Neuroscience, behavioral, and health perspectives (pp. 273–298). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stack, D. M., & Muir, D. W. (1990). Tactile stimulation as a component of social interchange: New interpretations for the still-face effect. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8(2), 131–145. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1990.tb00828.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stack, D. M., & Muir, D. W. (1992). Adult tactile stimulation during face-to-face interactions modulates five-month-olds’ affect and attention. Child Development, 63(6), 1509–1525. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131572

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stepakoff, S. A. (1999). Mother-infant tactile communication at four months: Effects of infant gender, maternal ethnicity, and maternal depression (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, St. John’s University).

  • Stepakoff, S. A., Beebe, B. & Jaffe, J. (2000, July 18). Mother-infant tactile communication at four months: Infant gender, maternal ethnicity, and maternal depression. Paper presented at the International Congress of Infant Studies, Brighton, England.

  • Stern, M., & Karraker, K. H. (1989). Sex stereotyping of infants: A review of gender labeling studies. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 20(9–10), 501–522. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00288198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taddio, A., Katz, J., Ilersich, A. L., & Koren, G. (1997). Effect of neonatal circumcision on pain response during subsequent routine vaccination. The Lancet, 349(9052), 599–603. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(96)10316-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevathan, W. R. (1981). Maternal touch at 1st contact with the newborn infant. Developmental Psychobiology, 14(6), 549–558. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420140608

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E. Z. (1995). Touch in mother-infant interaction. In T. M. Field (Ed.), Touch in early development (pp. 53–65). Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E. Z., & Cohn, J. F. (1989). Infant-mother face-to-face interaction: Age and gender differences in coordination and the occurrence of miscoordination. Child Development, 60(1), 85–92. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131074

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E. Z., Morelli, G. A., & Ivey, P. K. (1992). The Efe forager infant and toddler’s pattern of social relationships: Multiple and simultaneous. Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 568–577. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.4.568

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E. Z., & Weinberg, M. K. (1989). Maternal regulatory scoring system (MRSS) (Unpublished manuscript, The Child Development Unit, Children’s Hospital, Boston).

  • Tronick, E. Z., & Weinberg, M. K. (1997). Depressed mothers and infants: Failure to form dyadic states of consciousness. In L. Murray & P. J. Cooper (Eds.), Postpartum depression and child development (pp. 54–81). Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuulari, J. J., Scheinin, N. M., Lehtola, S., Merisaari, H., Saunavaara, J., Parkkola, R., Sehlstedt, I., Karlsson, L., Karlsson, H., & Björnsdotter, M. (2019). Neural correlates of gentle skin stroking in early infancy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 35, 36–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.004

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Underdown, A. (2009). The power of touch: Exploring infant massage. In J. Barlow & P. O. Svanberg (Eds.) Keeping the baby in mind: Infant mental health in practice (pp. 17–28). Routledge.

  • Vickers, A., Ohlsson, A., Lacy, J., & Horsley, A. (2004). Massage for promoting growth and development of preterm and/or low birth-weight infants. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD000390.pub2

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • von Mohr, M., Crowley, M. J., Walthall, J., Mayes, L. C., Pelphrey, K. A., & Rutherford, H. J. (2018). EEG captures affective touch: CT-optimal touch and neural oscillations. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(1), 155–166. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-017-0560-6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wall-Wieler, E., Roos, L. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2020). Maternal depression in early childhood and developmental vulnerability at school entry. Pediatrics, 146(3), e20200794. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2020-0794

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L., He, J., & Zhang, X. (2013). The efficacy of massage on preterm infants: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Perinatology, 30(9), 731–738. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0032-1332801

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinberg, M. K. (1992). Sex differences in 6-month-old infants' affect and behavior: Impact on maternal caregiving (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The University of Massachusetts at Amherst).

  • Weinberg, M. K., Tronick, E. Z., Olson, K. L., & Cohen, L. (1998, April 4). Maternal depression and anxiety: The relation between maternal self-reports of functioning and mother-infant interaction (Conference session, The International Congress of Infant Studies, Atlanta, Georgia).

  • Weiss, S. J. (1990). Parental touching: Correlates of a child’s body concept and body sentiment. In K. E. Barnard & T. B. Brazelton (Eds.), Touch: The foundation of experience (pp. 425–459). International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, S. J., Wilson, P., Seed, M. S., & Paul, S. M. (2001). Early tactile experience of low birth weight children: Links to later mental health and social adaptation. Infant and Child Development, 10(3), 93–115. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.236

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1940). Comparative psychology of mental development. Harper & Brothers.

  • Winnicott, D. W. (1971). Transitional objects and transitional phenomena. In Playing and reality (pp. 1–25). Tavistock/Routledge (Original work published in 1953).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to express our sincere gratitude to Ms. Gavkhar Abdurokhmonova, research assistant to Dr. Beebe from 2019 to 2022, for providing helpful updates to the literature review and for her valuable feedback on several earlier drafts of this article. We also wish to thank the journal editors and the anonymous reviewers for their attentiveness and useful recommendations while this article was under review.

Funding

The research on which this article is based was funded by The National Institute of Mental Health (Grant MH56130) to Dr. Beatrice Beebe at New York State Psychiatric Institute, affiliated with Columbia University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Beatrice Beebe was the Principal Investigator in the large longitudinal research project of which this study formed one small part. Dr. Beebe was also the director of the infant laboratory in which this research was conducted. Dr. Stepakoff and Dr. Beebe contributed to the conception and design of this study. Data collection and analysis were performed collaboratively along with members of the research team. Dr. Stepakoff was a member of the research team convened and led by Dr. Beebe who also supervised her doctoral dissertation. Dr. Stepakoff’s dissertation served as the basis for this article.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shanee Stepakoff.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Not applicable.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all participants according to the standards in place at the New York State Psychiatric Institute, an affiliate of Columbia University. The study was conducted according to NYSPI’s policies and protocols for research with human subjects.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stepakoff, S., Beebe, B. Maternal Touch as a Channel of Communication at Age Four Months: Variations by Infant Gender and Maternal Depression. J Nonverbal Behav (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00442-9

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00442-9

Keywords

Navigation