Abe, K., & Watanabe, D. (2011). Songbirds possess the spontaneous ability to discriminate syntactic rules. Nature Publishing Group, 14(8), 0–6. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2869.
Article
Google Scholar
Altenmüller, E., Schmidt, S., & Zimmermann, E. (Eds.). (2013). The evolution of emotional communication: From sounds in nonhuman mammals to speech and music in man. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Andruski, J. E., & Kuhl, P. K. (1996). The acoustic structure of vowels in mothers’ speech to infants and adults. In Proceeding of fourth international conference on spoken language processing. ICSLP’96 (vol. 3, pp. 1545–1548). IEEE.
Aryani, A., Conrad, M., Schmidtke, D., & Jacobs, A. (2018). Why 'piss’ is ruder than 'pee’? The role of sound in affective meaning making. PLoS ONE, 13(6), e0198430. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198430.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Aryani, A., Hsu, C. T., & Jacobs, A. M. (2019). Affective iconic words benefit from additional sound–meaning integration in the left amygdala. Human Brain Mapping, 40(18), 5289–5300. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24772.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Aryani, A., & Jacobs, A. M. (2018). Affective congruence between sound and meaning of words facilitates semantic decision. Behavioral Sciences, 8(56), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs8060056.
Article
Google Scholar
Auracher, J. (2017). Sound iconicity of abstract concepts: Place of articulation is implicitly associated with abstract concepts of size and social dominance. PLoS ONE. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187196.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 614–636. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.614.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C., & Bryant, G. (2008). Vocal emotion recognition across disparate cultures. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8(1), 135–148. https://doi.org/10.1163/156770908x289242.
Article
Google Scholar
Beecher, M. D., & Brenowitz, E. A. (2005). Functional aspects of song learning in songbirds. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 20(3), 143–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.004.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Berwick, R. C., Okanoya, K., Beckers, G. J. L., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2011). Songs to syntax: The linguistics of birdsong. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 113–121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.002.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Blasi, D. E., Moran, S., Moisik, S. R., Widmer, P., Dediu, D., & Bickel, B. (2019). Human sound systems are shaped by post-Neolithic changes in bite configuration. Science, 363, eaav3218. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aav3218.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Boë, L. J., Berthommier, F., Legou, T., Captier, G., Kemp, C., Sawallis, T. R., Fagot, J. (2017). Evidence of a vocalic proto-system in the baboon (Papio papio) suggests pre-hominin speech precursors. PLoS ONE, 12(1), e0169321.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J., Beckers, G. J. L., Huybregts, M. A. C., Berwick, R. C., & Everaert, M. B. H. (2018). Meaningful syntactic structure in songbird vocalizations? PLoS Biology, 16(6), e2005157. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2005157.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Bowling, D. L., Garcia, M., Dunn, J. C., Ruprecht, R., Stewart, A., Frommolt, K. H., & Fitch, W. T. (2017). Body size and vocalization in primates and carnivores. Scientific Reports. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep41070.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Briefer, E. (2012). Vocal expression of emotions in mammals: Mechanisms of production and evidence. Journal of Zoology, 288(1), 1–20.
Article
Google Scholar
Brown, S. (2017). A joint prosodic origin of language and music. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1894. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01894.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Bryant, G. A. (2013). Animal signals and emotion in music: Coordinating affect across groups. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00990.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Camperio Ciani, A. (2000). When to get mad: Adaptive significance of rage in animals. Psychopathology, 33(4), 191–197. https://doi.org/10.1159/000029142.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Cerchio, S., Jacobsen, J. K., & Norris, T. F. (2001). Temporal and geographical variation in songs of humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae: Synchronous change in Hawaiian and Mexican breeding assemblages. Animal Behaviour, 62(2), 313–329. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1747.
Article
Google Scholar
Charlton, B. D. (2014). Menstrual cycle phase alters women’s sexual preferences for composers of more complex music. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.0403.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Charlton, B. D., Ellis, W. A. H., McKinnon, A. J., Cowin, G. J., Brumm, J., Nilsson, K., & Fitch, W. T. (2011). Cues to body size in the formant spacing of male koala (Phascolarctos cinereus) bellows: Honesty in an exaggerated trait. Journal of Experimental Biology, 214(20), 3414–3422. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.061358.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Charlton, B. D., & Reby, D. (2016). The evolution of acoustic size exaggeration in terrestrial mammals. Nature Communications. 1https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms12739.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Charlton, B. D., Reby, D., & McComb, K. (2007a). Female red deer prefer the roars of larger males. Biology Letters, 3(4), 382–385. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2007.0244.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Charlton, B. D., Reby, D., & McComb, K. (2007b). Female perception of size-related formant shifts in red deer, Cervus elaphus. Animal Behaviour, 74(4), 707–714. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.09.021.
Article
Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1956). Three models for the description of language. IRE Transactions on Information Theory, 2(3), 113–124. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226700002024.
Article
Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959). On certain formal properties of grammars. Information and Control, 2(2), 137–167. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0019-9958(59)90362-6.
Article
Google Scholar
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2015). The language faculty that wasn’t: A usage-based account of natural language recursion. Frontiers in Psychology. Frontiers. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01182.
Article
Google Scholar
Collier, K., Bickel, B., van Schaik, C. P., Manser, M. B., & Townsend, S. W. (2014). Language evolution: Syntax before phonology? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1788), 20140263. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.0263.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Congdon, J. V., Hahn, A. H., Filippi, P., Campbell, K. A., Hoang, J., Scully, E. N., et al. (2019). Hear them roar: A comparison of black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) and human (Homo sapiens) perception of rousal in Vocalizations across all classes of terrestrial vertebrates. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 133(4), 520–541. https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000187.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Cross, N., & Rogers, L. J. (2006). Mobbing vocalizations as a coping response in the common marmoset. Hormones and Behavior, 49(2), 237–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.007.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Cutler, A., Dahan, D., & Van Donselaar, W. (1997). Prosody in the comprehension of spoken language: A literature review. Language and Speech, 40(2), 141–201. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383099704000203.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.
Book
Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. New York: Oxford University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Davis, C., Chong, C. S., & Kim, J. (2017). The effect of spectral profile on the intelligibility of emotional speech in noise (pp. 581–585). The MARCS Institute, Western Sydney University, Australia, Interspeech.
Dawkins, R., & Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal signals: Information or manipulation? Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, 2, 282–309.
Google Scholar
de Boer, B. (2005). Infant-directed speech and evolution of language. In M. Tallerman (Ed.), Evolutionary prerequisites for language (pp. 100–121). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
de Boer, B., Wich, S. A., Hardus, M. E., & Lameira, A. R. (2015). Acoustic models of orangutan hand-assisted alarm calls. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 218, 907–914. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.110577.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Desrochers, A. U., Be, M., & Bourque, J. (2002). Do mobbing calls affect the perception of predation risk by forest birds? Animal Behaviour, 64, 709–714. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2002.4013.
Article
Google Scholar
Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science, 298(5596), 1191–1194. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1076358.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Dooling, R. J., & Brown, S. D. (1990). Speech perception by budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus): Spoken vowels. Perception and Psychophysics, 47(6), 568–574. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203109.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32(1), 163–181. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.32.061002.093158.
Article
Google Scholar
Dupuis, K., & Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2014). Intelligibility of emotional speech in younger and older adults. Ear and Hearing, 35(6), 695–707. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0000000000000082.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Engesser, S., Ridley, A. R., & Townsend, S. W. (2016). Meaningful call combinations and compositional processing in the southern pied babbler. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(21), 5976–5981. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1600970113.
Article
Google Scholar
Engesser, S., & Townsend, S. W. (2019). Combinatoriality in the vocal systems of nonhuman animals. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1493.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Englund, K. T. (2005). Voice onset time in infant directed speech over the first six months. First Language, 25(2), 219–234. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723705050286.
Article
Google Scholar
Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(5), 429–448. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999094X.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Everaert, M. B. H., Huybregts, M. A. C., Chomsky, N., Berwick, R. C., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2015). Structures, not strings: Linguistics as part of the cognitive sciences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(12), 729–743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.09.008.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Fallow, P. M., Gardner, J. L., & Magrath, R. D. (2011). Sound familiar? Acoustic similarity provokes responses to unfamiliar heterospecific alarm calls. Behavioral Ecology, 22(2), 401–410. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arq221.
Article
Google Scholar
Fant, G. (1960). Acoustic theory of speech production. The Hague: Mouton.
Google Scholar
Faragó, T., Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á, Huber, L., Virányi, Z., & Range, F. (2010). Dogs’ expectation about signalers’ body size by virtue of their growls. PLoS ONE, 5(12), e15175. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015175.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. The Adapted Mind, 1:391–428. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00852474.
Article
Google Scholar
Fernald, A., & Mazzie, C. (1991). Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 209–221.
Article
Google Scholar
Fernald, A., & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology, 20(1), 104.
Article
Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., De Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language, 16(3), 477–501. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900010679.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Filippi, P. (2016). Emotional and interactional prosody across animal communication systems: A comparative approach to the emergence of language. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1393. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01393.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Filippi, P., Congdon, J. V., Hoang, J., Bowling, D. L., Reber, S. A., Pašukonis, A., et al. (2017a). Humans recognize emotional arousal in vocalizations across all classes of terrestrial vertebrates: Evidence for acoustic universals. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284, 20170990. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.0990.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Filippi, P., & Gingras, B. (2018). Emotion communication in animal vocalizations, music and language: An evolutionary perspective. In E. M. Luef & M. M. Marin (Eds.), The talking species (pp. 105–125). Graz: Uni-Press Graz Verlag GmbH.
Google Scholar
Filippi, P., Gingras, B., & Fitch, W. T. (2014). Pitch enhancement facilitates word learning across visual contexts. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01468.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Filippi, P., Gogoleva, S. S., Volodina, E. V., Volodin, I. A., & Boer, B. D. (2017b). Humans identify negative (but not positive) arousal in silver fox vocalizations: Implications for the adaptive value of interspecific eavesdropping. Current Zoology, 63(4), 445–456.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Filippi, P., Hoeschele, M., Spierings, M., & Bowling, D. L. (2019). Temporal modulation in speech, music, and animal vocal communication: Evidence of conserved function. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14228.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Filippi, P., Laaha, S., & Fitch, W. T. (2017c). Utterance-final position and pitch marking aid word learning in school-age children. Royal Society Open Science. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.161035.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Filippi, P., Ocklenburg, S., Bowling, D. L., Heege, L., Güntürkün, O., Newen, A., & de Boer, B. (2017d). More than words (and faces): Evidence for a Stroop effect of prosody in emotion word processing. Cognition and Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1177489.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Finn, B., & Roediger, H. L. (2011). Enhancing retention through reconsolidation: Negative emotional arousal following retrieval enhances later recall. Psychological Science, 22(6), 781–786. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611407932.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Fischer, J., & Price, T. (2017). Meaning, intention, and inference in primate vocal communication. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 82, 22–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.014.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (1997). Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in rhesus macaques. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102(2), 1213–1222. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.421048.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2000). The evolution of speech: A comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01494-7.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2010). The evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2017). Empirical approaches to the study of language evolution. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 24(1), 3–33. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1236-5.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2018a). What animals can teach us about human language: The phonological continuity hypothesis. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 21, 68–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.014.
Article
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2018b). Bio-linguistics: Monkeys break through the syntax barrier. Current Biology, 28(12), R695–R697. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.087.
Article
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T., De Boer, B., Mathur, N., & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2016). Monkey vocal tracts are speech-ready. Science Advances. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.1600723.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T., & Friederici, A. D. (2012). Artificial grammar learning meets formal language theory: An overview. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 1933–1955. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0103.
Article
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T., & Hauser, M. D. (2004). Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science, 303(5656), 377–380. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1089401.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T., Huber, L., & Bugnyar, T. (2010). Social cognition and the evolution of language: Constructing cognitive phylogenies. Neuron, 65(6), 795–814. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.011.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T., & Kelley, J. P. (2000). Perception of vocal tract resonances by whooping cranes Grus americana. Ethology, 106(6), 559–574. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00572.x.
Article
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T., & Reby, D. (2001). The descended larynx is not uniquely human. Proceedings. Biological Sciences/The Royal Society, 268(1477), 1669–75. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1704.
Article
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T., & Zuberbühler, K. (2013). Primate precursors to human language: Beyond discontinuity. In E. Altenmüller, S. Schmidt & E. Zimmermann (Eds.), The evolution of emotional communication: From sounds in nonhuman mammals to speech and music in man (pp. 26–48). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Forkman, B., Boissy, A., Meunier-Salaün, M.-C., Canali, E., & Jones, R. B. (2007). A critical review of fear tests used on cattle, pigs, sheep, poultry and horses. Physiology and Behavior, 92(3), 340–374. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PHYSBEH.2007.03.016.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Garcia, M., Herbst, C. T., Bowling, D. L., Dunn, J. C., & Fitch, W. T. (2017). Acoustic allometry revisited: Morphological determinants of fundamental frequency in primate vocal production. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 10450. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-11000-x.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Garcia, M., Wondrak, M., Huber, L., & Fitch, W. T. (2016). Honest signaling in domestic piglets (Sus scrofa domesticus): Vocal allometry and the information content of grunt calls. Journal of Experimental Biology, 219(12), 1913–1921. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.138255.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Gentner, T. Q., Fenn, K. M., Margoliash, D., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2006). Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature, 440(7088), 1204–1207. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04675.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Guillet, R., & Arndt, J. (2009). Taboo words: The effect of emotion on memory for peripheral information. Memory and Cognition, 37(6), 866–879. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.37.6.866.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Gussenhoven, C. (2002). Intonation and biology. Liber Amicorum Bernard Bichakjian (Festschrift for Bernard Bichakjian), 59–82.
Gussenhoven, C. (2016). Foundations of intonational meaning: Anatomical and physiological factors. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(2), 425–434. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12197.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.298.5598.1569.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Heimbauer, L. A., Beran, M. J., & Owren, M. J. (2011). A chimpanzee recognizes synthetic speech with significantly reduced acoustic cues to phonetic content. Current Biology, 21(14), 1210–1214. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CUB.2011.06.007.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Jäger, G., & Rogers, J. (2012). Formal language theory: Refining the Chomsky hierarchy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 1956–1970. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0077.
Article
Google Scholar
Jarvis, E. D. (2006). Selection for and against vocal learning in birds and mammals. Ornithological Science, 5(1), 5–14. https://doi.org/10.2326/osj.5.5.
Article
Google Scholar
Jiang, X., Long, T., Cao, W., Li, J., Dehaene, S., & Wang, L. (2018). Production of supra-regular spatial sequences by macaque monkeys. Current Biology, 28(12), 1851–1859.e4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.047.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Johnstone, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2000). Vocal communication of emotion. The Handbook of Emotion (pp. 220–235). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00084-5.
Jürgens, U. (2002). Neural pathways underlying vocal control. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(2), 235–258.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Jürgens, U. (2009). The neural control of vocalization in mammals: A review. Journal of Voice, 23(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2007.07.005.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Fischer, J. (2004). Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for “fast mapping. Science, 304(5677), 1682–1683. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1097859.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Kitchen, D. M., Bergman, T. J., Cheney, D. L., Nicholson, J. R., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2010). Comparing responses of four ungulate species to playbacks of baboon alarm calls. Animal Cognition, 13(6), 861–870. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010-0334-9.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Klieve, S., & Jeanes, R. C. (2001). Perception of prosodic features by children with cochlear implants: Is it sufficient for understanding meaning differences in language? Deafness and Education International, 3(1), 15–37. https://doi.org/10.1179/146431501790561061.
Article
Google Scholar
Kotz, S. A., & Paulmann, S. (2011). Emotion, language, and the brain. Linguistics and Language Compass, 5(3), 108–125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00267.x.
Article
Google Scholar
Kret, M. E., Jaasma, L., Bionda, T., & Wijnen, J. G. (2016). Bonobos (Pan paniscus) show an attentional bias toward conspecifics’ emotions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(14), 3761–3766. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1522060113.
Article
Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevnikova, E. V., Ryskina, V. L., Lacerda, F. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science, 277(5326), 684–686.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., & Miller, J. D. (1975). Speech perception by the chinchilla: Voiced-voiceless distinction in alveolar plosive consonants. Science, 190(4209), 69–72. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1166301.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., & Padden, D. M. (1983). Enhanced discriminability at the phonetic boundaries for the place feature in macaques. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 73(3), 1003–1010. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.389148.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Kumar, S., Filipski, A., Swarna, V., Walker, A., & Hedges, S. B. (2005). Placing confidence limits on the molecular age of the human–chimpanzee divergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(52), 18842–18847.
Article
Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P. (1996). Elements of acoustic phonetics. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/10.2307/417823.
Book
Google Scholar
Lea, A. J., Barrera, J. P., Tom, L. M., & Blumstein, D. T. (2008). Heterospecific eavesdropping in a nonsocial species. Behavioral Ecology, 19(5), 1041–1046. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arn064.
Article
Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (2016). Turn-taking in human communication—Origins and implications for language processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(1), 6–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.010.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C., & Holler, J. (2014). The origin of human communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences: Biological Sciences, 369, 20130302.
Article
Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. H., Klatt, D. H., & Wilson, W. H. (1969). Vocal tract limitations on the vowel repertoires of rhesus monkey and other nonhuman primates. Science, 164(3884), 1185–1187. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.164.3884.1185.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Lingle, S., & Riede, T. (2014). Deer mothers are sensitive to infant distress vocalizations of diverse mammalian species. The American Naturalist, 184(4), 510–522.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Lockwood, G., & Dingemanse, M. (2015). Iconicity in the lab: A review of behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging research into sound-symbolism. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1246. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01246.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Ma, W., Golinkoff, R. M., Houston, D. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2011). Word learning in infant-and adult-directed speech. Language Learning and Development, 7(3), 185–201. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2011.579839.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Macedonia, J. M., & Evans, C. S. (1993). Essay on contemporary issues in ethology: Variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals. Ethology, 93(3), 177–197. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1993.tb00988.x.
Article
Google Scholar
Magrath, R. D., Pitcher, B. J., & Gardner, J. L. (2009). Recognition of other species’ aerial alarm calls: Speaking the same language or learning another? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1657), 769–774. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1368.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Marler, P., Evans, C. S., & Hauser, M. D. (1992). Animal signals: Motivational, referential, or both? In H. Papoušek, U. Jürgens, & M. Papoušek (Eds.), Studies in emotion and social interaction Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental approaches (pp. 66–86). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Martins, M. D. (2012). Distinctive signatures of recursion. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 2055–2064.
Article
Google Scholar
Martins, P. T., & Boeckx, C. (2020). Vocal learning: Beyond the continuum. PLoS Biology, 18(3), e3000672.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
McGaugh, J. L. (2004). The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27(1), 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144157.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Meguerditchian, A., Vauclair, J., & Hopkins, W. D. (2013). On the origins of human handedness and language: A comparative review of hand preferences for bimanual coordinated actions and gestural communication in nonhuman primates. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(6), 637–650. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21150.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Mendl, M., Burman, O. H. P., & Paul, E. S. (2010). An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proceedings. Biological sciences/The Royal Society, 277(1696), 2895–904. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0303.
Article
Google Scholar
Miller, G. (2000). Evolution of human music through sexual selection. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker & S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music (pp. 329–360). Cambridge: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/5190.003.0025.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Morton, E. S. (1977). On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. The American Naturalist, 111(981), 855–869. https://doi.org/10.1086/283219.
Article
Google Scholar
Nencheva, M. L., Piazza, E. A., & Lew-Williams, C. (2020). The moment-to-moment pitch dynamics of child-directed speech shape toddlers’ attention and learning. Developmental Science, e12997. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12997.
Nesse, R. M. (1990). Evolutionary explanations of emotions. Human Nature, 1(3), 261–289.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., Hauser, M. D., Spaepen, G., & Aslin, R. N. (2004). Learning at a distance II. Statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies in a non-human primate. Cognitive Psychology, 49(2), 85–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2003.12.002.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Nottebohm, F. (2002). The origins of vocal learning. The American Naturalist, 106(947), 116–140. https://doi.org/10.1086/282756.
Article
Google Scholar
Nowicki, S., & Searcy, W. A. (2014). The evolution of vocal learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 48–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2014.06.007.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
O’Donnell, T. J., Hauser, M. D., & Fitch, W. T. (2005). Using mathematical models of language experimentally. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(6), 284–289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.011.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Ohala, J. J. (1984). An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of F0 of voice. Phonetica, 41(1), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1159/000261706.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Ouattara, K., Lemasson, A., & Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Campbell’s monkeys use affixation to alter call meaning. PLoS ONE, 4(11), e7808. doi:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007808.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Owings, D. H., & Morton, E. S. (1998). Animal vocal communication: A new approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139167901.
Book
Google Scholar
Owren, M. J., & Rendall, D. (2001). Sound on the rebound: Bringing form and function back to the forefront in understanding nonhuman primate vocal signaling. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 10(2), 58–71. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.1014.
Article
Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2009). The emotional antecedents to the evolution of music and language. Musicae Scientiae, 13(2_suppl), 229–259. https://doi.org/10.1177/1029864909013002111.
Article
Google Scholar
Parker, A. R. (2007). Evolving the narrow language faculty: was recursion the pivotal step? In The evolution of language: Proceedings of the 6th international conference (EVOLANG06) (pp. 239–246). https://doi.org/10.1142/9789812774262_0031.
Pepperberg, I. M. (2006). Cognitive and communicative abilities of grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 100(1–2), 77–86.
Article
Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (2010). Vocal learning in Grey parrots: A brief review of perception, production, and cross-species comparisons. Brain and Language, 115(1), 81–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.002.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Perruchet, P., & Rey, A. (2005). Does the mastery of center-embedded linguistic structures distinguish humans from nonhuman primates? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(2), 307–313. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196377.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Phelps, E. A., & LeDoux, J. E. (2005). Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: From animal models to human behavior. Neuron, 48(2), 175–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Pinker, S., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: What’s special about it? Cognition, 95(2), 201–236.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Pisanski, K., Fraccaro, P. J., Tigue, C. C., O’Connor, J. J. M., Röder, S., Andrews, P. W., et al. (2014). Vocal indicators of body size in men and women: A meta-analysis. Animal Behaviour, 95, 89–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.06.011.
Article
Google Scholar
Poole, J. H., Tyack, P. L., Stoeger-Horwath, A. S., & Watwood, S. (2005). Elephants are capable of vocal learning. Nature, 434(7032), 455–456. https://doi.org/10.1038/434455a.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Prat, Y., Taub, M., & Yovel, Y. (2015). Vocal learning in a social mammal: Demonstrated by isolation and playback experiments in bats. Science Advances, 1(2), e1500019. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.1500019.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Price, T., Wadewitz, P., Cheney, D., Seyfarth, R., Hammerschmidt, K., & Fischer, J. (2015). Vervets revisited: A quantitative analysis of alarm call structure and context specificity. Scientific Reports, 5(13220), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep13220.
Article
Google Scholar
Pulvermller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 576–582.
Article
Google Scholar
Puts, D. A., Gaulin, S. J. C., & Verdolini, K. (2006). Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(4), 283–296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.11.003.
Article
Google Scholar
Ralls, K., Fiorelli, P., & Gish, S. (1985). Vocalizations and vocal mimicry in captive harbor seals, Phoca vitulina. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 63, 1050–1056. doi:https://doi.org/10.1139/z85-157.
Article
Google Scholar
Ravignani, A., Sonnweber, R. S., Stobbe, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2013). Action at a distance: Dependency sensitivity in a New World primate. Biology Letters, 9, 20130852. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2013.0852.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Ravignani, A., Verga, L., & Greenfield, M. D. (2019). Interactive rhythms across species: The evolutionary biology of animal chorusing and turn-taking. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1453(1), 12.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Ravignani, A., Westphal-Fitch, G., Aust, U., Schlumpp, M. M., & Fitch, W. T. (2015). More than one way to see it: Individual heuristics in avian visual computation. Cognition, 143, 13–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.021.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Reber, S. A., Boeckle, M., Szipl, G., Janisch, J., Bugnyar, T., & Fitch, W. T. (2016). Territorial raven pairs are sensitive to structural changes in simulated acoustic displays of conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 116, 153–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.04.005.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Reber, S. A., Šlipogor, V., Oh, J., Ravignani, A., Hoeschele, M., Bugnyar, T., & Fitch, W. T. (2019). Common marmosets are sensitive to simple dependencies at variable distances in an artificial grammar. Evolution and Human Behavior, 40(2), 214–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.11.006.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Reichmuth, C., & Casey, C. (2014). Vocal learning in seals, sea lions, and walruses. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 66–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2014.06.011.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Rendall, D., Owren, M. J., & Ryan, M. J. (2009). What do animal signals mean? Animal Behaviour, 78(2), 233–240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.007.
Article
Google Scholar
Riegel, M., Wierzba, M., Grabowska, A., Jednoróg, K., & Marchewka, A. (2016). Effect of emotion on memory for words and their context. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 524(8), 1636–1645. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.23928.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Russell, A. F., & Townsend, S. W. (2017). Communication: Animal steps on the road to syntax? Current Biology, 27(15), R753–R755. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.066.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161–1178. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0077714.
Article
Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Pourtois, G., Schwartz, S., Seghier, M. L., Scherer, K. R., & Vuilleumier, P. (2005). Emotion and attention interactions in social cognition: Brain regions involved in processing anger prosody. NeuroImage, 28(4), 848–858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.023.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2015). Emotional vocalizations are recognized across cultures regardless of the valence of distractors. Psychological Science, 26(3), 354–356. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614560771.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Murphy, J., Sevcik, R. A., Brakke, K. E., Williams, S. L., & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1993). Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 1–221.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2018). Emotional expressions as speech act analogs. Philosophy of Science, 85(5), 1038–1053. https://doi.org/10.1086/699667.
Article
Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., Banse, R., & Wallbott, H. G. (2001). Emotion inferences from vocal expression correlate across languages and cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(1), 76–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022101032001009.
Article
Google Scholar
Schirmer, A., & Kotz, S. A. (2006). Beyond the right hemisphere: Brain mechanisms mediating vocal emotional processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(1), 24–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.009.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Schirmer, A., Kotz, S. A., & Friederici, A. D. (2002). Sex differentiates the role of emotional prosody during word processing. Cognitive Brain Research, 14(2), 228–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00108-8.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Chimpanzees know that others make inferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(7), 3077–3079. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1000469108.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
See, R. L., Driscoll, V. D., Gfeller, K., Kliethermes, S., & Oleson, J. (2013). Speech intonation and melodic contour recognition in children with cochlear implants and with normal hearing. Otology and Neurotology, 34(3), 490–498. https://doi.org/10.1097/MAO.0b013e318287c985.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., & Marler, P. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: Evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science, 210(4471), 801–803.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. (2008). Emotion, decision making, and the amygdala. Neuron, 58(5), 662–671. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.020.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Simonyan, K. (2014). The laryngeal motor cortex: Its organization and connectivity. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 15–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2014.05.006.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Simonyan, K., & Horwitz, B. (2011). Laryngeal motor cortex and control of speech in humans. The Neuroscientist, 17(2), 197–208.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Singh, L., Nestor, S., Parikh, C., & Yull, A. (2009). Influences of infant-directed speech on early word recognition. Infancy, 14(6), 654–666. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000903263973.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Soderstrom, M., Seidl, A., Kemler Nelson, D. G., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2003). The prosodic bootstrapping of phrases: Evidence from prelinguistic infants. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(2), 249–267. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00024-X.
Article
Google Scholar
Sonnweber, R., Ravignani, A., & Fitch, W. T. (2015). Non-adjacent visual dependency learning in chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 18(3), 733–745. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0840-x.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Spierings, M. J., & ten Cate, C. (2016). Budgerigars and zebra finches differ in how they generalize in an artificial grammar learning experiment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(27), E3977–E3984. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1600483113.
Article
Google Scholar
Stansbury, A. L., & Janik, V. M. (2019). Formant modification through vocal production learning in gray seals. Current Biology, 29(13), 2244–2249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.05.071.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Stefanski, R., & Klatt, D. (1974). How does a mynah bird mimic human speech? The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 55(4), 822–832. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3437210.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Stevens, N. J., Seiffert, E. R., O’Connor, P. M., Roberts, E. M., Schmitz, M. D., Krause, C., Temu, J. (2013). Palaeontological evidence for an Oligocene divergence between Old World monkeys and apes. Nature, 497(7451), 611–614.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Stobbe, N., Westphal-Fitch, G., Aust, U., & Tecumseh Fitch, W. (2012). Visual artificial grammar learning: Comparative research on humans, kea (Nestor notabilis) and pigeons (Columba livia). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 1995–2006. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0096.
Article
Google Scholar
Stoeger, A. S., Mietchen, D., Oh, S., de Silva, S., Herbst, C. T., Kwon, S., & Fitch, W. T. (2012). An asian elephant imitates human speech. Current Biology, 22(22), 2144–2148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.022.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2008). Affective arousal as information: How affective arousal influences judgments, learning, and memory. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(5), 1824–1843. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00138.x.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Sutherland, M. R., & Mather, M. (2018). Arousal (but not valence) amplifies the impact of salience. Cognition and Emotion, 32(3), 616–622. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1330189.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Suzuki, T. N., Wheatcroft, D., & Griesser, M. (2016). Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls. Nature Communications, 7, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms10986.
Article
Google Scholar
Taylor, A., & Reby, D. (2010). The contribution of source-filter theory to mammal vocal communication research. Journal of Zoology, 280(3), 221–236. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.2009.00661.x.
Article
Google Scholar
Taylor, A. M., Reby, D., & McComb, K. (2008). Human listeners attend to size information in domestic dog growls. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123(5), 2903–2909. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2896962.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Tchernichovski, O., & Oller, D. K. (2016). Vocal development: How marmoset infants express their feelings. Current Biology, 26(10), R422–R424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.063.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Templeton, C. N., Greene, E., & Davis, K. (2005). Allometry of alarm calls: Black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science, 308(5730):1934-1937.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
ten Cate, C., & Okanoya, K. (2012). Revisiting the syntactic abilities of nonhuman animals: Natural vocalizations and artificial grammar learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 1984–1994. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0055.
Article
Google Scholar
Thompson, W. F., Marin, M. M., & Stewart, L. (2012). Reduced sensitivity to emotional prosody in congenital amusia rekindles the musical protolanguage hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(46), 19027–19032. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1210344109.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Titze, I. R. (1994). Principles of voice production. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.
Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Hare, B. (2003). Chimpanzees understand psychological states–the question is which ones and to what extent. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(4), 153–156.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Townsend, S. W., Engesser, S., Stoll, S., Zuberbühler, K., & Bickel, B. (2018). Compositionality in animals and humans. PLoS Biology. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2006425.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Trainor, L., & Desjardins, R. (2002). Pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech affect infants’ ability to discriminate vowels. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(2), 335–340.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Trainor, L. J., Austin, C. M., & Desjardins, R. N. (2000). Is infant-directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotion? Psychological Science, 11(3), 188–195. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00240.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Van Heijningen, C. A. A., De Visser, J., Zuidema, W., & Cate, T., C (2009). Simple rules can explain discrimination of putative recursive syntactic structures by a songbird species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(48), 20538–20543. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908113106.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Versace, E., Rogge, J. R., Shelton-May, N., & Ravignani, A. (2019). Positional encoding in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Animal Cognition, 22(5), 825–838. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-019-01277-y.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Waxman, S. R., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). Early word-learning entails reference, not merely associations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(6), 258–263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.03.006.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., Pons, F., Dietrich, C., Kajikawa, S., Fais, L., & Amano, S. (2007). Infant-directed speech supports phonetic category learning in English and Japanese q. Cognition, 103, 147–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.03.006.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Wheeler, B. C., & Fischer, J. (2012). Functionally referential signals: A promising paradigm whose time has passed. Evolutionary Anthropology, 21(5), 195–205. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21319.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Wiley, R. H. (1983). The evolution of communication: information and manipulation. Animal Behavior, 2, 156–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0271-5309(97)00009-8.
Article
Google Scholar
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2007). Rapid word learning under uncertainty via cross-situational statistics: Research article. Psychological Science, 18(5), 414–420. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01915.x.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Zhang, Y. S., & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2016). Perinatally influenced autonomic system fluctuations drive infant vocal sequences. Current Biology, 26(10), 1249–1260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.023.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2020). Syntax and compositionality in animal communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375(1789), 20190062.
Article
Google Scholar