Skip to main content
Log in

The Interaction of Sex, Verbal, and Nonverbal Cues in Same-Sex First Encounters

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This experiment examined sex differences in responses to various combinations of verbal and nonverbal content during a same-sex interaction. Fifty men and thirty women participated in a same-sex interview task with a confederate posing as another participant. Confederates disclosed either superficial or emotional information, and they faced away from or toward the participant, when answering questions. Results revealed that men attended to verbal information to evaluate the appropriateness of their own personal disclosure, whereas women attended to both verbal and nonverbal cues to evaluate the conversation partner and the appropriateness of their own personal disclosure.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A power analysis was conducted based on the effect sizes obtained from similar laboratory experiments. Because no experiments examined the proposed three-way interaction, the effect size was estimated as moderate (0.30). Results of this analysis indicated that a sample size of 80 would provide sufficient power for all hypothesized effects (betas for all effects > 0.80).

  2. We ensured that participants were reacting to the independent variables and not to the characteristics of a given confederate in two ways. First, by recruiting two males and three females as confederates for this experiment, we had multiple instances of a stimulus category (termed stimulus sampling by Wells and Windschitl 1999). This is particularly recommended for interaction experiments involving standardized stimuli and confederates (see Guerrero and Le Poire 2005 for a review). Second, we conducted a series of One-way ANOVA with the five confederates as levels of the independent variable and all outcome measures as the dependent variables. These analyses yielded no significant differences among confederates (all ps > 0.10), suggesting that participants were reacting to the manipulated variables of the experiment and not to unique characteristics of the confederates.

  3. The other five questions served as fillers. They were, “How well did you get to know this person?” “What are your overall impressions of this person?” “What are your overall impressions of Philosophy majors?” “How similar is this person to other college students?” and “What are some other things you would have liked to learn about this person?”.

  4. The MANOVA revealed significant main effects of Sex and Topic, as well as a Topic × Style interaction effect (ps < 0.05). The Sex Main Effect revealed that men’s perceptions of the conversation were more positive than women’s perceptions. The Topic Main Effect revealed that participants in the Emotional Disclosure condition reported engaging in more emotional disclosure themselves, and rated the conversation more positively, than participants in the Superficial Disclosure condition. The Topic × Style interaction effect revealed that confederates who engaged in superficial disclosure and a nonverbally distant style were rated the least positive. Participants also engaged in more personal disclosure when the confederate disclosed emotional information with an intimate nonverbal style than when he or she was distant.

References

  • Akert, R. M., & Panter, A. T. (1988). Extraversion and the ability to decode nonverbal communication. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 965–972.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The development of interpersonal relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, P. A. (1999). Nonverbal communication: Forms and functions. Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, P. A., Guerrero, L. K., & Jones, S. M. (2006). Nonverbal behavior in intimate interactions and intimate relationships. In V. Manusov & M. L. Patterson (Eds.), The Sage handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 259–277). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M., & Dean, J. (1965). Eye-contact, distance and affiliation. Sociometry, 28, 289–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M., & Ingham, R. (1972). Gaze, mutual gaze and proximity. Semiotica, 6, 32–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M., & Kendon, A. (1967). The experimental analysis of social performance. In L. Berkkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (3rd ed., pp. 55–98). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, M. S., & Berman, J. S. (2001). Is psychotherapy more effective when therapists disclose information about themselves? Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 69, 597–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Sommer, K. L. (1997). What do men want? Gender differences and two spheres of belongingness: A comment on Cross and Madson (1997). Psychological Bulletin, 122, 38–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, R. R. (1981). Friendships of women and men. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 5, 402–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bischoping, K. (1993). Gender differences in conversation topic, 1922–1990. Sex Roles, 28, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braatoy, T. F. (1954). Fundamentals of psychoanalytic technique. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., & Woodall, W. G. (1996). Nonverbal communication: The unspoken dialogue (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaikin, A. L., Derlega, V. J., Bayma, B., & Shaw, J. (1975). Neuroticism and disclosure reciprocity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 13–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, R. A. (2006). A comparison of topics and objectives in a cross section of young men’s and women’s everyday conversations. In K. Dindia & D. J. Canary (Eds.), Sex differences and similarities in communication (2nd ed., pp. 303–319). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, N. L., & Miller, L. C. (1994). Self-disclosure and liking: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 457–475.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Condon, W. S., & Ogston, W. D. (1966). Sound film analysis of normal and pathological behavior patterns. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 143, 338–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cross, S. E., Bacon, P. L., & Morris, M. L. (2000). The relational-interdependent self-construal and relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 791–808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cutrona, C. E., Hessling, R. M., & Suhr, J. (1997). The influence of husband and wife personality on marital social support interactions. Personal Relationships, 4, 379–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. H., & Kraus, L. A. (1997). Personality and empathic accuracy. In W. J. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy (pp. 144–168). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derlega, V. J., & Chaikin, A. L. (1977). Norms affecting self-disclosure in men and women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 376–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derlega, V. J., Metts, S., Petronio, S., & Margulis, S. T. (1993). Self-disclosure. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, F. (1947). Analysis of postural behavior. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 16, 195–213.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, F. (1952). Analytic posturology. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 21, 196–214.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, F. M. (1990). Status, sex, and smiling: The effect of role on smiling in men and women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 531–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dindia, K. (1994). The intrapersonal-interpersonal dialectical process of self-disclosure. In S. Duck (Ed.), Dynamics of relationships (pp. 27–57). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dindia, K. (2000). Self-disclosure, identity, and relationship development: A dialectical perspective. In K. Dindia & S. Duck (Eds.), Communication and personal relationships (pp. 147–162). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dindia, K., & Allen, M. (1992). Sex differences in self-disclosure: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 106–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich, H. J., & Graeven, D. B. (1971). Reciprocal self-disclosure in a dyad. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 389–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and codings. Semiotica, 1, 49–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Exline, R. V. (1963). Explorations in the process of personal perception: Visual interaction in relation to competition, sex, and need for affiliation. Journal of Personality, 31, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Exline, R. V. (1972). Visual interaction: The glances of power and preference. In J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 19, pp. 163–206). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Exline, R., Gray, D., & Schuette, D. (1965). Visual behavior in a dyad as affected by interview content and sex of respondent. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 201–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Exline, R. V., & Winters, L. C. (1965). Affective relations and mutual glances in dyads. In S. Tomkins & C. Izzard (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and personality (pp. 319–330). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, B. A. (1996). Friendship processes. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, S. (1984). The sociability aspect of extraversion as a situation-specific dimension. Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 7–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frances, S. J. (1979). Sex differences in nonverbal behavior. Sex Roles, 5, 519–535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Funder, D. C., & Harris, M. J. (1986). On the several facets of personality assessment: The case of social acuity. Journal of Personality, 54, 528–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodstein, L., & Reinecker, V. (1974). Factors affecting self-disclosure: A review of the literature. Progress in Experimental Personality Research, 7, 49–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gore, J. S., Cross, S. E., & Morris, M. L. (2006). Let’s be friends: Relational self-construal and the development of intimacy. Personal Relationships, 13, 83–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, H. M., & Ambady, N. (2006). Methods for the study of nonverbal communication. In V. Manusov & M. L. Patterson (Eds.), The Sage handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 41–58). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guerrero, L. K., Eloy, S. V., & Wabnik, A. I. (1993). Linking maintenance strategies to relationship development: A reconceptualization. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 273–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guerrero, L. K., & Le Poire, B. A. (2005). Nonverbal research involving experimental manipulations by confederates. In V. Manusov (Ed.), The sourcebook of nonverbal measures: Going beyond words (pp. 507–522). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E. T. (1963). A system for the notation of proxemic behavior. American Anthropologist, 65, 1003–1026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845–857.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Irish, J. T., Roter, D. L., Ehrlich, C. M., & Miller, L. H. (1994). Gender in medical encounters: An analysis of physician and patient communication in a primary care setting. Health Psychology, 13, 384–392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henley, N. M. (1973). Status and sex: Some touching observations. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 2, 91–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, C. T., & Stull, D. E. (1987). Gender and self-disclosure: Strategies for exploring issues. In V. J. Derlega & J. H. Berg (Eds.), Self-disclosure: Theory, research, and therapy (pp. 81–100). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. T. (1932). A study of the expression of bodily posture. Journal of General Psychology, 7, 405–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Graziano, W. G. (2001). Agreeableness as a moderator of interpersonal conflict. Journal of Personality, 69, 323–362.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, S. M., & Guerrero, L. K. (2001). The effects of nonverbal immediacy and verbal person centeredness in the emotional support process. Human Communication Research, 27, 567–596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jourard, S. M., & Lasakow, P. (1958). Some factors in self-disclosure. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 56, 91–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, Y. C. (1992). The construction of the sense of intimacy from everyday social interaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Rochester.

  • Lippa, R. (1998). The nonverbal display and judgment of extraversion, masculinity, femininity, and gender diagnosticity: A lens model analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 32, 80–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayo, C., & Henley, N. (Eds.). (1981). Gender and nonverbal behavior. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. (1972). Nonverbal communication. In J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 19, pp. 107–161). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muirhead, R. D., & Goldman, M. (1979). Mutual eye contact as affected by seating position, sex, and age. Journal of Social Psychology, 109, 201–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pegalis, L. J., Shaffer, D. R., Bazzini, D. G., & Greenier, K. (1994). On the ability to elicit self-disclosure: Are there gender-based and contextual limitations on the opener effect? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 412–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Planalp, S., DeFrancisco, V. I., & Rutherford, D. (1996). Varieties of cues to emotion in naturally occurring situations. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 137–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prager, K. J. (2000). Intimacy in personal relationships. In C. Hendrick & S. Hendrick (Eds.), Close relationships: A sourcebook (pp. 229–242). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reece, M. M., & Whitman, R. N. (1962). Expressive movements, warmth, and verbal reinforcement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64, 234–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reich, W. (1945). Character analysis. New York: Orgone Institute Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reis, H. T., & Patrick, B. C. (1996). Attachment and intimacy: Component processes. In A. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 523–563). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reis, H. T., & Shaver, P. (1988). Intimacy as an interpersonal process. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships (pp. 367–389). Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Depaulo, B. M. (1979). Sex differences in accommodation in nonverbal communication. In R. Rosenthal (Ed.), Skill in nonverbal communication: Individual differences (pp. 68–103). Cambridge, MA: Oegelschlager, Gunn & Hain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D. (1979). Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS test. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samter, W., Burleson, B. R., Kunkel, A. W., & Werking, K. J. (1994). Gender and beliefs about communication in intimate relationships: Moderating effects of type of communication and type of relationship (or, when gender differences make a difference—and when they don’t). Paper presented at the annual meetings of the International Communication Association, Sydney, Australia.

  • Sherrod, D. (1989). The influence of gender on same-sex friendships. In C. Hendrick (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology: Vol. 10. Close relationships (pp. 164–186). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1991). Maintenance strategies and romantic relationship type, gender and relational characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 217–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, J., Childs, L., & Fuehrer, A. (1981). Gender and sex roles as predictors of self-disclosure. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 28, 510–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tannen, D. (1990a). Gender differences in topical coherence: Creating involvement in best friends’ talk. Discourse Processes. Special issue: Gender and conversational interaction, 13, 73–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannen, D. (1990b). Gender differences in conversational coherence: Physical alignment and topical cohesion. In B. Dorval (Ed.), Conversational organization and its development (pp. 167–206). Westport, CT: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walther, J. B. (2006). Nonverbal dynamics in computer-mediated communication, or :(and the net :(‘s with you, :) and you :) alone. In V. Manusov & M. L. Patterson (Eds.), The Sage handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 461–479). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisbrod, R. M. (1965). Looking behavior in a discussion group. Unpublished manuscript, Cornell University.

  • Wells, G. L., & Windschitl, P. D. (1999). Stimulus sampling and social psychological experimentation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1115–1125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winstead, B. A. (1986). Sex differences in same-sex friendships. In V. J. Derlega & B. A. Winstead (Eds.), Friendship and social interaction (pp. 81–97). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. T., & Inman, C. C. (1993). In a different mode: Masculine styles of communicating closeness. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 21, 279–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, P. H. (1989). Friendship. In D. Levinson (Ed.), Encyclopedia of marriage and the family (Vol. 1, pp. 315–320). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jonathan S. Gore.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Example Scripts for Superficial Disclosure Answers

  • Question #1: “Tell me about yourself”

  • Answer: “I’m a senior Philosophy major here at ISU, and I’m planning on graduating in May. Although most of my classes have been in philosophy, I’ve also taken some Psychology courses, like the introductory course and the social psych course. I think of myself as smart and inquisitive, but also someone who likes to go out and have fun from time to time. I am _____ feet _____ inches tall, I have _____ eyes, and _____ hair (note: this information varied based on the confederate), as you can see. I’m 21-years-old. I grew up in Omaha, and came to Ames in 2001, so I’ve basically stayed in the Midwest my whole life.”

  • Question #2: “Tell me about your family”

  • Answer: “Like I said before, I grew up in Omaha, which is where my family is now. I’m the oldest of 3 kids. I have a brother, Jake, who is 17, and getting ready to graduate from high school, and a sister, Sue, who is 13 and getting ready to start high school. I guess you can say my family is middle class. My mom is an elementary school teacher (she teaches fifth grade), and my dad works in administration at the post office. They’ve been together for about 25 years, and they both are from Omaha, so they’ve basically been there their whole lives.”

Appendix 2: Example Scripts for Emotional Disclosure Answers

  • Question #1: “Tell me about yourself”

  • Answer: “I’m a senior Philosophy major here at ISU, and I’m planning on graduating in May. I’m trying to narrow down what I want to do with my degree, so I’m really at a transitional part of my life. I try to stay on top of my schoolwork so that I can get a good job, but I also try to balance my work with my friends and my family, and my girlfriend/boyfriend. It’s not always easy, but I think it’s important to try to balance those things out so that you can be well-rounded and happy with your life. I grew up in Omaha, so I am still really close to my family.”

  • Question #2: “Tell me about your family”

  • Answer: “Like I said before, I grew up in Omaha, which is where my family is now. My family has had their ups and downs, but overall we’re very supportive of each other. I’m the oldest of three kids, and I’m the first in my family to graduate from a 4-year university (including my parents), so I really want to make them proud and do something worthwhile with my degree. My parents grew up in Omaha and are very close, which I would like to have for my marriage (when the time comes). Sometimes I wonder if I can live up to that standard, though.”

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gore, J.S. The Interaction of Sex, Verbal, and Nonverbal Cues in Same-Sex First Encounters. J Nonverbal Behav 33, 279–299 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-009-0074-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-009-0074-1

Keywords

Navigation