Skip to main content
Log in

Gender Differences in the Incidence of Depression and Anxiety: Econometric Evidence from the USA

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Using data from the Collaborative Psychiatric Epidemiology Surveys (CPES) for the United States for the period 2001–2003, this paper addresses a vexed question relating to inter-gender differences in depression rates, namely how much of the observed difference in depression rates between men and women may be explained by differences between them in their exposure, and how much may be explained by differences between them in their response, to depression-inducing factors. The contribution of this paper is to propose a method for disentangling these two influences and to apply it to US data. The central conclusion of the paper was differences between men and women in rates of depression and anxiety were largely to be explained by differences in their responses to depression-inducing factors: the percentage contribution of inter-gender response differences to explaining the overall difference in inter-gender probabilities of being depressed was 93 percent for “sad, empty type depression”; 92 percent for “very discouraged” type depression; and 69 percent for “loss of interest” type depression.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For a public policy approach to the pursuit of happiness.

  2. Inter alia Blanchflower and Oswald (2002), Clark (1996, 1999, 2001), Clark and Oswald (1994), Easterlin (1974, 1987, 2001), Frank (1985, 1997, 1999), Frey and Stutzer (2002), Hirsch (1976), Layard (2002, 2003), Oswald (1997), Scitovsky (1976).

  3. For example, Borooah (2006) in a study of Northern Ireland reported that only four percent of those with severe mental health problems described themselves as happy and 60 percent described themselves as unhappy; equally tellingly, only 32 percent of those whose mental health problems were not severe described themselves as happy—the same proportion as those with severe heart problems who regarded themselves as happy.

  4. On this last point, see Mangan (2000).

  5. The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis plays a major role in regulating stress responses and, compared to men, women are more likely to have a dysfunctional HPA responses to stress (Weiss et al. 1999).

  6. With the consequence that conflicts in, or the ending of, relationships were more likely to produce depression in women than in men.

  7. A greater propensity to rumination in response to stress increases the risk of developing depression (Noel-Hoeksema et al. 1999).

  8. Another possibility is that gender differences in depression rates may be the result of men responding to stress through alternative modes such as antisocial behaviour and alcohol abuse (Kessler et al. 1994; Meltzer et al. 1995).

  9. A problem with self-reported information is that of recall. If it is a natural instinct to suppress memories of unpleasant events, and if the young are more likely to be susceptible to depression and anxiety, then older persons in a sample are likely to "forget" that they were depressed or anxious when they were young while, for the younger persons in the sample such memories are likely to be vivid. Consequently, in a cross-section of people, older, compared to younger, respondents would report lower rates of depression or anxiety purely for reasons of differences in recall.

  10. Gender specific response rates were not provided.

  11. The proportions were computed for persons who reported non-missing values for all the variables used in the logistic regressions (Tables 4, 5, 6, 7, and 8): 10,089 persons of whom 5,862 were women and 4,227 were men.

  12. Those whose income-to-poverty line ratio was lower than the mean ratio.

  13. Childhood traumas were any of the following: fidgety childhood; frequently in trouble with adults for 6 months or more during childhood or adolescence; lying, stealing as child or teenager; ran away frequently, played truant, or stayed out late as child or teenager; had separation anxiety, for 1 month or more, as a child.

  14. Both of these were measured by a person’s World Health Organisation Disability Assessment Score (WHO-DAS): the higher the score, the greater the disability.

  15. The logit equation is \( {\frac{{\Pr (Y_{i} = 1)}}{{1 - \Pr (Y_{i} = 1)}}} = \exp \{ \sum\nolimits_{j = 1}^{J} {X_{ij} } \beta_{j} \} = \exp \{ z_{i} \} \) for J coefficients, βj and for observations on J variables.

  16. That is, from applying different coefficient vectors to a given vector of attributes.

References

  • Alegria, M., Jackson, J. S., Kessler, R. C., & Takeuchi, D. (2007). Collaborative Psychiatric Epidemiology Surveys (CPES), 2001–2003 [United States] [Computer file]. ICPSR20240-v5, Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, Survey Research Center [producer], 2007. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor], 2008-06-19.

  • Blanchflower, D., & Oswald, A. (2002). Well-being over time in Britain and the USA, NBER working papers, no. 7487. Cambridge, Mass: National Bureau of Economic Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blinder, A. S. (1973). Wage discrimination: Reduced form and structural Estimates. Journal of Human Resources, 8, 436–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Böckerman, P., & Ilmakunnas, P. (2009). Unemployment and self-assessed health: Evidence from panel data. Health Economics, 18, 161–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borooah, V. K. (2006). What makes people happy? Some evidence from Northern Ireland. Journal of Happiness Studies, 7, 427–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borooah, V. K., & Iyer, S. (2005). The decomposition of inter-group differences in a logit model: Extending the Oaxaca–Blinder approach with an application to school enrolment in India. Journal of Economic and Social Measurement, 30, 279–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E. (1996). Job satisfaction in Britain. British Journal of Industrial Relations, 34, 189–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E. (1999). Are wages habit forming? Evidence from micro data. Journal of Economic Behaviour and Organisation, 39, 179–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E. (2001). What really matters in a job? Hedonic measurement using quit data. Labour Economics, 8, 223–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., Diener, E., Georgellis, Y., & Lucas, R. (2008). Lags and leads in life satisfaction: A test of the baseline hypothesis. Economic Journal, 118, F222–F243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., & Oswald, A. (1994). Unhappiness and unemployment. Economic Journal, 104, 648–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Culbertson, F. M. (1997). Depression and gender: An international review. American Psychologist, 52, 25–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlop, D. D., Song, J., Lyons, J. S., Manheim, L. M., & Chang, R. W. (2003). Racial/ethnic differences in rates of depression among preretirement adults. American Journal of Public Health, 93, 1945–1952.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (1974). “Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence. In P. A. David & M. W. Reder (Eds.), Nations and households in economic growth: Essays in honour of Moses Abramowitz. Academic Press: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (1987). Birth and fortune: The impact of numbers on personal welfare (2nd ed.). Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (2001). Income and happiness: Towards a unified theory. Economic Journal, 111, 465–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H. (1985). Choosing the right pond. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H. (1997). The frame of reference as a public good. Economic Journal, 107, 1832–1847.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H. (1999). Luxury fever: Money and happiness in an era of excess. Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2002). Happiness and economics. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch, F. (1976). The social limits to growth. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., et al. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Layard, R. (2002). Rethinking public economics: Implications of rivalry and habit. Centre for Economic Performance, London: London School of Economics.

  • Layard, R. (2003). Happiness: Has social science a clue? Lionel Robbins Memorial Lectures 2002/3, London: London School of Economics.

  • Layard, R. (2006). The depression report: A new deal for depression and anxiety disorders, Centre for Economic Performance, London: London School of Economics.

  • Mangan, J. (2000). Workers without traditional employment: An international study of non-standard work. Cheltenhan: Edward Elgar.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzer, H., Gill, B., Petticrew, M., & Hinds, K. (1995). OCPS surveys of psychiatric morbidity in Great Britain, Report 1: The prevalence of psychiatric morbidity among adults living in private households, London: Her Majesty’s Stationery Office.

  • Murakumi, J. (2002). Gender and depression: Explaining the different rates of depression between men and women. Perspectives in Psychology, 5, 27–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, H. S. (1998). Discrimination and detailed decomposition in a logit model. Economics Letters, 61, 115–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noel-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Sex differences in depression. Stanford CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noel-Hoeksema, S. (2001). Gender differences in depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 173–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noel-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining the gender difference in depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1061–1072.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oaxaca, R. (1973). Male-female wage differentials in urban labor markets. International Economic Review, 14, 693–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oswald, A. (1997). Happiness and economic performance. Economic Journal, 107, 1815–1831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piccinelli, M., & Wilkinson, G. (2000). Gender differences in depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 486–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scitovsky, T. (1976). The joyless economy. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, E. L., Longhurst, J. G., & Mazure, C. M. (1999). Childhood sexual abuse as a risk factor for depression in women: Psychosocial and neurobiological correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 816–828.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weissman, M. M., Bland, R. C., Canino, G. J., Faravelli, C., Greenwald, S., Hwu, H.-G., et al. (1996). Cross-national epidemiology of major depression and bipolar disorder. Journal of the American Medical Association, 276, 293–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Vani K. Borooah.

Additional information

The data used in this paper are from the Collaborative Psychiatric Epidemiology Surveys (CPES) 2001–2003 [United States] provided online by the Inter-University Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR) http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/ncs/. I am grateful to three anonymous referees whose comments have greatly improved the paper. However, I am solely responsible for its deficiencies.

Technical Appendix: The Decomposition of Probabilities

Technical Appendix: The Decomposition of Probabilities

More formally, there are N people (indexed, i = 1…N) of whom N M are men and N W are women: k = M (men), W (women.). Define the variable Y i such that Y i  = 1, if the person has had a condition (depression, anxiety), Y i  = 0, otherwise. Then, under a logit model, the likelihood of a man or woman having had the condition is:

$$ \Pr (Y_{i} = 1) = {\frac{{\exp ({\mathbf{X}}_{{\mathbf{i}}}^{k} {\varvec{\upbeta}}^{k} )}}{{1 + \exp ({\mathbf{X}}_{{\mathbf{i}}}^{k} {\varvec{\upbeta}}^{k} )}}} = F({\mathbf{X}}_{{\mathbf{i}}}^{k} {\varvec{\hat{\upbeta }}}^{k} ), \quad k = M,W $$
(1)

where \( \mathbf{X}^{k}_{i} = \left\{ {X_{ij} ,\,j = 1 \ldots J} \right\} \) represents the vector of observations, for person i of group k, on J variables which determine the likelihood of the person having that condition, and \( {\varvec{\hat{\upbeta }}}^{k} = \left\{ {\beta^{k}_{j} ,\,j = 1 \ldots J} \right\} \) is the associated vector of coefficient estimates for persons from group k.

The average probability of a man or woman having had the condition is:

$$ \bar{Y}^{k} = \bar{P}(\mathbf{X}^{k}_{i} ,{\varvec{\hat{\upbeta }}}^{k} ) = N_{k}^{ - 1} \sum\limits_{i = 1}^{{N_{k} }} {F(\mathbf{X}^{k}_{i} ,{\varvec{\hat{\upbeta }}}^{k} )} \quad k = M,W $$
(2)

So that:

$$ \bar{Y}^{W} - \bar{Y}^{M} = \left[ {\bar{P}\left( {\mathbf{X}^{M}_{i} ,{\varvec{\hat{\upbeta }}}^{W} } \right) - \bar{P}\left( {\mathbf{X}^{M}_{i} ,{\varvec{\hat{\upbeta }}}^{M} } \right)} \right] + \left[ {\bar{P}\left( {\mathbf{X}^{W}_{i} ,{\varvec{\hat{\upbeta }}}^{W} } \right) - \bar{P}\left( {\mathbf{X}^{M}_{i} ,{\varvec{\hat{\upbeta }}}^{W} } \right)} \right] $$
(3)

Alternatively:

$$ \bar{Y}^{W} - \bar{Y}^{M} = \left[ {\bar{P}\left( {\mathbf{X}^{W}_{i} ,{\varvec{\hat{\upbeta }}}^{W} } \right) - \bar{P}\left( {\mathbf{X}^{W}_{i} ,{\varvec{\hat{\upbeta }}}^{M} } \right)} \right] + \left[ {\bar{P}\left( {\mathbf{X}^{W}_{i} ,{\varvec{\hat{\upbeta }}}^{M} } \right) - \bar{P}\left( {\mathbf{X}^{M}_{i} ,{\varvec{\hat{\upbeta }}}^{M} } \right)} \right]. $$
(4)

The first term in square brackets, in Eqs. 3 and 4, represents the “response effect”: it is the difference in average rates (of having had a condition) between women and men resulting from inter-gender differences in responses (as exemplified by differences in the coefficient vectors) to a given vector of attribute values.Footnote 16 The second term in square brackets in Eqs. 3 and 4 represents the “attributes effect”: it is the difference in average rates (of having had a condition) between women and men resulting from inter-gender differences in attributes, when these attributes are evaluated using a common coefficient vector.

So for example, in Eq. 3, the difference in sample means is decomposed by asking what the average rates for men would have been, had they been treated as women; in Eq. 4, it is decomposed by asking what the average rate for women would have been, had they been treated as men. In other words, the common coefficient vector used in computing the attribute effect is, for Eq. 3, the female vector and, for Eq. 4, the male vector.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Borooah, V.K. Gender Differences in the Incidence of Depression and Anxiety: Econometric Evidence from the USA. J Happiness Stud 11, 663–682 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-009-9155-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-009-9155-4

Keywords

Navigation