Introduction

All organisms release chemical compounds into the environment. Some of these compounds are shed as excreta or produced during the decomposition process after death, whereas others are metabolic products that can convey specific information to conspecific or heterospecific individuals (Vos et al. 2006). Such infochemicals often affect interactions between organisms and thus may be profoundly important in influencing individual fitness, population dynamics, and the structure of ecological webs in terrestrial and aquatic ecosystems (Dicke and Sabelis 1988; Vos et al. 2006; Parsons et al. 2018; Müller et al. 2020). In fur seals for example, chemical fingerprints help to determine mother–offspring similarity and colony membership (Stoffel et al. 2015), and in many other taxa infochemicals shape preferences for particular mates or ensure recognition and avoidance of individuals from species that are otherwise ecologically similar (Caspers et al. 2009). Infochemicals also shape the outcomes of competitive, mutualistic, commensal, predatory, and parasitic interactions among species (e.g., Lewis et al. 2013; Allen et al. 2017; Chrétien et al. 2021; Scogings et al. 2021), and are being used increasingly in programs of conservation management (Norbury et al. 2021).

Olfactory interactions between prey and predators are prevalent and can have particularly important effects across two or more trophic levels (Jones et al. 2016). For prey organisms, selective benefits should accrue to individuals that recognize and respond appropriately to the olfactory cues of their predators. Among vertebrates, for example, prey may respond to a predatory odour cue by investigating, freezing, or fleeing from the site of the cue, and show acute or chronic stress (Fardell et al. 2020) as well as latent effects on their levels of vigilance behaviour, habitat use, temporal and spatial activity (Fenn and Macdonald 1995; Parsons and Blumstein 2010a, b; Cox et al. 2012; Parsons et al. 2018). These responses are often calibrated according to the specific identity of the predator and the level of threat that it poses to prey (Dickman 1992; Anson and Dickman 2013; Grau et al. 2019), and to factors such as the source of the odour (e.g. urine or faeces), its age and intensity (Fendt et al. 2020), and the prior experience of prey individuals with predators (Bleicher et al. 2018). In mammals olfactory information is processed by the amygdala and hypothalamus, allowing for learned behavioural responses of prey to predators (Takahashi 2014; Canteras et al. 2015). Intriguingly, prey may respond positively to the odour of dominant, or apex, predators if those predators actively suppress subordinate predators that pose greater risks to the prey; here the apex predator provides net benefit to the prey if its per capita predatory effect on prey is less than that of the subordinate predators (Jones et al. 2016). In situations where prey are naïve to predators and do not recognize their olfactory cues, such as where prey or predator species have been introduced to a new environment, prey mortality may be high and local populations will be at high risk of extinction (Salo et al. 2007).

For olfactory predators, by contrast, individuals should benefit if they can efficiently identify and locate prey by their odour (Hughes et al. 2010). If prey organisms are buried, sessile, or cryptically camouflaged (e.g., eggs in birds' nests), olfaction may be the only sensory modality by which predators can detect them (Conover 2007). For example, common shrews Sorex araneus use odour to locate fly pupae buried at depths up to 16 cm (Churchfield 1980), as do desert rodents when excavating buried seeds (Taraborelli et al. 2009) and grey squirrels Sciurus carolinensis selecting early-germinating scatter-hoarded seeds in preference to dormant seeds (Sundaram et al. 2020). If prey organisms are mobile, predators are more likely to hunt them by using odour cues deposited by prey in the environment or at focal sites such as burrows, nests or shelter sites. For example, wolves Canis lupus and other predators such as sharks can follow blood trails to locate prey (Tester 1963; Gable et al. 2016), while black rats Rattus rattus and other mammalian predators use odour to find bird nests (Price and Banks 2012; Ibáñez-Álamo et al. 2015). There is evidence that predators can detect and home in on prey by tracking the alarm odours that prey emit when fearful (Cocke and Thiessen 1986; Müller-Schwarze 2006), and emerging evidence that predators can distinguish the odours of prey that differ in physical condition, selecting those that are easiest and most energetically profitable to hunt (Newman and Buesching 2019). Reward-based learning allows predators to associate different prey odours with different levels of reward or energetic gain (MacArthur and Pianka 1966; Garvey et al. 2017). Using the vomeronasal system, predators also may exploit the ‘olfactory web’ and eavesdrop on the olfactory cues of other predators to reduce inter-species competitive interactions (Banks et al. 2016). Factors such as the age, strength, and patchiness of odour cues also affect detection success by predators (Carthey et al. 2011; Norbury et al. 2021).

Reptiles are seldomly featured as the top-down force in vertebrate predator–prey systems, rather they are commonly featured as prey items for larger mammalian or avian predators (e.g., Woinarski et al. 2018; Stobo-Wilson et al. 2021). However, there is compelling evidence that, in some contexts, reptilian predators represent a 'forgotten majority' (Sutherland and Bryant 2014), greatly influencing other vertebrates via competitive and predatory interactions (Savidge 1987; Sutherland et al. 2011; Radford et al. 2020), or as ecosystem engineers (Doody et al. 2020). Because of the selective advantages to prey of reducing their risk of detection by predators, and to predators of overcoming prey defences, arms-races can be expected that progressively modify the processes and outcomes of predator–prey dynamics (Banks et al. 2014). In a recent review, Newman and Buesching (2019) considered the arms-race between eavesdropping predators and their prey, and how conspicuous prey odours are deployed and spied upon, to be understudied but important areas in research in vertebrate systems. There is a particular dearth of information on how reptilian predators and their prey use infochemicals, although some snakes detect and track prey odours (e.g., Burghardt and Denny 1983; Webb and Shine 1992), as do some Varanus spp. (Pianka and Vitt 2003), We address these knowledge gaps here by exploring the role of olfaction in the interaction between an apex reptilian predator, the Australian sand goanna Varanus gouldii (Varanidae: 1–2 kg), and its mammalian prey. We examine whether these interactants recognize and respond to each others' odours, and also ask how the prey species minimize their risk of being detected and eaten by this goanna.

We selected this predator–prey system for several reasons. First, V. gouldii appears able to exploit prey odours (Garrett and Card 1993; Garrett et al. 1996), using its forked tongue to detect prey infochemicals before digging prey such as insects or small mammals from their burrows or under leaf litter (Pianka 1970, 1994; Thompson 1995). Prey is likely to be hunted once detected provided that it returns a net energetic gain (Losos and Greene 1988; Kaufman et al. 1996), with individuals rapidly learning new foraging techniques to reach detectable but not freely available foods (Cooper et al. 2019). In one study V. gouldii focussed its activity around the nests of a large (200–300 g) rodent, the greater stick-nest rat Leporillus conditor, apparently, because the smell of the rats' urine signalled a potential food source (Bolton and Moseby 2004). Second, in arid Australia small mammals use burrows to escape the climatic extremes, potentially leaving residual odours at burrow entrances that could be exploited by V. gouldii. Rodents construct 25–100 cm deep burrows, whereas co-occurring small dasyurid marsupials use soil cracks or usurp the burrows of larger animals (Dickman 1993, 2003). Many of these species are highly mobile and frequently move burrow locations (Letnic 2002; Haythornthwaite and Dickman 2006), resulting in home ranges that are unstable or nomadic. Baker and Dickman (2018) speculated that such nomadism—which is highly unusual and perhaps globally unique in small desert mammals—reduces the accumulation of prey odours at burrows and hence the risk of being detected and depredated by V. gouldii, although this hypothesis remains to be tested.

We tested the following predictions:

  1. 1.

    Small mammals will recognize and avoid the odour of V. gouldii,

  2. 2.

    Varanus gouldii i) will be attracted to the odour of small mammals, and ii) will be more attracted to the odours of species that maximize its energetic return, and

  3. 3.

    Small mammals will be less mobile and will show higher burrow fidelity where V. gouldii is absent compared with where it is present.

The main small mammals in our study system were the spinifex hopping-mouse Notomys alexis (~ 30 g), long-haired rat Rattus villosissimus (~ 120 g), sandy inland mouse Pseudomys hermannsburgensis (~ 12 g) and lesser hairy-footed dunnart Sminthopsis youngsoni (~ 10 g). The first two species dig deep (1 m) multi-entrance burrows that would be energetically costly for V. gouldii to excavate. By contrast, the latter two species occupy burrows that are simple and usually < 30 cm deep, and hence likely to be more profitable for V. gouldii to investigate and excavate. We used faecal analysis to confirm that these species occur in the diet of V. gouldii (Online Resource 1).

Methods and Materials

Study Area

Fieldwork was carried out at three sites on Ethabuka Reserve, a 215,000 ha area dominated by long red sand dunes on the north-eastern edge of the Simpson Desert, Queensland, Australia. The sites were Main Camp (23°46' S, 138°28' E), Field River (23°48' S, 138°04' E) and Way Site (23°47' S, 138°22' E), all of which contained populations of the study species ((Downey and Dickman 1993; Dickman et al. 2014). The dominant vegetation of the dune field is hard spinifex (Triodia basedowii) with stands of gidgee trees (Acacia georginae) on harder soil in the valleys between dunes (Wardle et al. 2015). Daily temperatures in summer exceed 40 °C and fall below 5 °C overnight in winter, and annual rainfall averages < 200 mm (Greenville et al. 2012). Wildfires may occur 1–2 years after heavy rains and have a mean minimum return interval of 26–27 years (Greenville et al. 2009). Heavy rain (552 mm) fell in 2010 near the beginning of the present study and was followed by wildfires in 2011 (Verhoeven et al. 2020). Our experiments were carried out in unburnt vegetation.

Experiments

Prediction 1: Small Mammals will Recognize and Avoid the Odour of V. gouldii

To test our first prediction, we used faecal material from V. gouldii as an odour source to present to small mammals. Varanus gouldii was captured during a long-term live trapping program in the study area (Dickman et al. 1999a, 2010, 2014; Greenville et al. 2016a) or by hand noosing following opportunistic sightings, and faeces produced during handling were placed in vials and frozen at -2 – -4 °C within 30 min. We used a giving-up density (GUD) experiment to gauge small mammal responses to the odour. The GUD is the amount of food that remains when an animal has finished foraging in a patch with an enriched food source. We provided food in an inedible matrix and allowed animals to choose between treatments with these food sources. If there are no constraints on foraging, individuals should spend adequate time in patches and consume as much food as needed. However, if an animal experiences a stressor, such as predation risk, in a food patch then GUDs increase, and animals spend less time in the patch (Brown 1988; Kotler et al. 1991).

To implement a GUD experiment, we established 54 sites, each > 25 m apart, along the bases or mid-sections of sand dunes where V. gouldii and small mammals were active. At each site we set up a food patch. This was a half-buried plastic bowl (15 cm diameter, 4 cm deep) containing food of high value to small mammals: mealworms Tenebrio molitor, which were energetically profitable for S. youngsoni (Fisher and Dickman 1993) or peanut quarters, which we considered would be of value to the study rodents (Murray and Dickman 1994; Murray et al. 1999). Either 10 mealworms or 10 peanut quarters were placed in each bowl, mixed into 200 ml of sifted sand. The underside of the bowls was smeared with a mixture of Vaseline and Coopex insecticide powder (Bayer, Pymble, New South Wales) to repel ants from the bowls. Twenty-seven bowls were set with each type of food, with the order of placement randomized. To assess small mammal responses to V. gouldii odour, we used cotton buds—6 cm-long sticks tipped with absorbent cotton wool (Johnson & Johnson, Sydney, New South Wales)—for odour presentation. The tip of a cotton bud was applied to fresh goanna faeces and then the stick was inserted vertically into the centre of a food bowl such that the odour-bearing cotton tip was ~ 2 cm above the bowl surface. To quantify the magnitude of response to V. gouldii odour, we also used cotton buds dipped in eucalyptus oil as a pungency control treatment (i.e., representing a strong but familiar odour) and cotton buds dipped in water as a procedural control (Kovacs et al. 2012). The three treatment odours were allocated randomly to bowls with each food type, so that n = 9 for each odour for the bowls containing mealworms and n = 9 for bowls with peanut quarters.

As our target small mammal species are nocturnal, the bowls were set up in the early evening and revisited near dawn when foraging was expected to have concluded. The GUD (number of mealworms or peanuts remaining in each bowl) was then recorded. The bowls were set with food but without the odour treatment for one night to allow animals to accustom to them. The bowls were then recharged each evening with fresh food (mealworms and peanut quarters) and new cotton buds with fresh odours (sand goanna, pungency, procedural control) over seven consecutive nights. Disposable latex gloves were used at all times to minimize cross-contamination of odours. To identify the small mammal species that had visited the food bowls, and thus whose GUDs were being measured, we smoothed the sand in a 10 cm-radius around each bowl and examined the footprints that were left each morning. The prints of the target species are readily distinguishable by differences in size, numbers of toes and imprint of the heel (Moseby et al. 2009; Dickman et al. 2010). GUDs could be recorded reliably if a bowl had been visited by one species or, if two or more species had visited, we could clearly discern the last forager by the overprinting of its tracks on those of earlier foragers. We discarded the results for any bowls if we could not reliably read the prints or if the site had been disturbed by wind or other species such as Australian ravens Corvus coronoides. The experiment testing our first prediction was carried out once, at Main Camp, in November 2014.

Prediction 2: Varanus gouldii will be Attracted to the Odour of Small Mammals

To test our second prediction, we created artificial burrows that simulated those of the study mammals. The burrows were made by hammering PVC pipe into the soil and then gently removing it, with soil inside, to create a vertical 'burrow' that was 2.5 cm or 3.5 cm wide and ~ 25 cm deep. The narrower (2.5 cm) burrows were used on one occasion only (September 2012) and then abandoned in favour of the wider burrows to facilitate ease of creation and subsequent manipulation of odours. Clusters of four burrows were constructed at 32 – 50 sites along the bases or mid-sections of sand dunes, with each cluster spaced > 200 m from the next. In sand dune habitat V. gouldii occupies activity areas of 5.9 ha (274 m linear distance if the area is assumed to be circular), but take a week to cover them (Bolton and Moseby 2004). Thus we assumed that our burrow clusters would be accessed by no more than a single V. gouldii provided they were set for a week or less. By contrast, the artificial burrows within a site were spaced 2 – 3 m apart so that an individual V. gouldii encountering one burrow in a cluster would have an approximately equal chance of encountering the others.

We tested the ability of V. gouldii to find and discriminate prey odours by placing small balls of cotton wool bearing the body odour of the study species at the bottom of each artificial burrow. Body odours were used in preference to urinary or faecal odours as integumentary chemicals are most likely to be deposited in burrows as small mammals enter and exit them. Within each cluster, we presented the odour of three species of small mammals—one odour per artificial burrow—and placed fresh cotton wool in the fourth burrow to serve as an odourless control. Field visits in September and November 2012 used the odours of N. alexis, P. hermannsburgensis, and R. villosissimus; the latter species disappeared from the study area in 2012 (Greenville et al. 2013), and its odours were replaced by those of S. youngsoni for experiments carried out in September 2013, April and November 2014. We term these two periods phase 1 and phase 2, respectively. Cotton wool was imbued with the odour of these species by providing captive or wild-caught animals with balls of this material as bedding for periods of > 12 h. The material was then frozen at -2 – -4 °C to reduce degradation of volatile components (Fardell et al. 2021), and inserted into the artificial burrows by operators wearing latex gloves and using clean forceps. As the cotton wool could be seen at the bottom of the burrows when the sun was overhead, a small (4 × 4 cm) patch of clean brown cloth was placed above the cotton wool to obscure it and ensure that odour was the only cue provided to foraging V. gouldii. Sand in a 30 cm radius around each artificial burrow was smoothed to capture the tracks of visiting V. gouldii.

The burrows and their experimental odours were set up in the mornings before V. gouldii became active, and were checked in the evening. We recorded the tracks of V. gouldii in the 30 cm radius around burrows and whether burrows had been dug into. This goanna characteristically excavates burrows by tearing at the soil from one side of a burrow with their forelimbs and following the burrow to the bottom where prey is usually located. Here, we recorded digging if a burrow had been partially or fully excavated. If a burrow in a cluster had been dug, we relocated the burrow cluster to a new position about 10 m away. All burrow sites were moved to new positions after 2 – 7 days; varanids quickly learn to distinguish profitable from non-profitable feeding sites and modes of prey capture (Manrod et al. 2008), and we assumed that individual V. gouldii would lose interest in small mammal odours with successive unsuccessful digs. Cotton wool balls bearing the experimental odours were replenished every 2 days to maintain their freshness, and inspections of goanna activity were made each evening.

Prediction 3: Small Mammals will be less Mobile and will Show Higher Burrow Fidelity in the Absence than the Presence of V. gouldii

Test of our third prediction required a small scale experimental removal and relocation of V. gouldii so that we could compare small mammal mobility and burrow fidelity in the presence and absence of this apex reptilian predator. We selected an area of ~ 50 ha at Main Camp with access tracks and three trapping grids on which we could capture V. gouldii, and commenced a removal program in September 2009. Between this time and November 2011, 39 V. gouldii were captured and relocated in similar habitat > 5 km away. This goanna is not territorial and occupies areas that shift over time as animals track prey (Bolton and Moseby 2004), and our tracking of a subset (n = 11) of relocated individuals indicated that they rapidly established new burrows and maintained their mass and condition following translocation (CD, NH, unpub. data).

To gauge the degree to which the removal area was free of V. gouldii, we set up three 30 m × 1 m transects on each of the three removal grids and a further nine transects on unsealed vehicle tracks through the removal area. The transects were raked and then smoothed by dragging a half-filled hessian sack along their length to create a suitable surface to record animal tracks, and were checked daily for three days on each of six field trips between September 2009 and November 2011. The tracks of V. gouldii were recorded on 12 of the 18 transects (67%) in September 2009 but fell to 0 on three field trips in 2011 as the removal protocol progressed. It is likely that some V. gouldii activity remained in the removal area in 2011 as we found tracks away from the transects on two occasions in that year. However, the results indicate that activity of V. gouldii on the removal area had been reduced to nearly zero. By contrast, similar monitoring at the same time of a control area 7 km away, where no removals were undertaken, showed that V. gouldii activity on transects remained relatively unchanged. There, 10 of 18 transects (56%) yielded sand goanna tracks in September 2009, compared with 8 – 14 of the 18 transects (44% – 78%) on the field trips in 2011.

In view of the above results, we used radio-tracking to quantify small mammal mobility and burrow fidelity in the control and removal areas on three occasions in May, August, and October – November 2011. Small mammals were live-trapped on long-term trapping grids in each area (Dickman et al. 1999b, 2014) and equipped with single-stage radio-tags with 10-cm trailing whip antennae that were attached either using a plastic cable tie collar (N. alexis and P. hermannsburgensis) or by cyanoacrylate glue to fur between the scapulae (S. youngsoni); no R. villosissimus were radio-tracked. Tags weighed 0.4 – 1.0 g and were attached to animals that were randomly selected from those captured provided that their weight gain with the tag was < 5%. Tags were supplied by Biotrack (Wareham, United Kingdom), Holohil (Ontario, Canada), or Titley Electronics (Ballina, New South Wales, Australia) and used frequencies between 150 and 151 MHz. Detailed protocols for tag fitting and animal release are provided by Dickman et al. (2010) and Haythornthwaite and Dickman (2006).

Animals were located at night every 1–4 h using a 3-element hand-held Yagi antenna and a Regal 1000 (Titley Electronics, Ballina, New South Wales, Australia) or TR-2 (Telonics Inc., Mesa, Arizona) receiver. If animals were in open areas, we approached them from downwind, walking on sand to reduce noise and using red light to establish visual contact to determine their location precisely. If animals were in covered areas or could not be approached closely, we estimated locations by triangulation of 2–3 bearings taken in the direction of the peak signal using a prismatic compass. Bearings were taken from known triangulation points ensuring that angles were > 20° and < 120° from each other (Kenward 2001). Pilot trials using tags placed at known locations indicated that bearings were accurate to within ± 5° up to distances of ~ 110 m (Dickman et al. 2010). By day, we located animals in burrows by walking along dune crests to pick up their signals and then walking along the line of peak signal strength. Burrows were pinpointed by removing the Yagi antenna and homing in on the signal, to within 1–2 m of the animal, using the coaxial cable and receiver. Animal locations and bearings were flagged using marker tape and later placed on fine-scale maps of the study area.

Although all components of this research were approved by the University of Sydney Animal Care and Ethics Committee, we took particular care with animal handling in the tests of our third prediction. No animals were used in radio-tracking if they appeared distressed upon capture and handling, and were instead released at the point of capture. Animals that were recaptured at the conclusion of radio-tracking had their tags removed; none had lost more than 4% of their body mass and all appeared to be in good condition.

Data Processing and Statistical Analyses

To test our first prediction concerning recognition and avoidance by small mammals of V. gouldii odour, we compared GUDs between the three odour treatments (no odour – control; pungency control – eucalyptus; goanna faecal material – goanna) at all sites that were visited, using a linear mixed-effects model. Treatment and small mammal species were set as fixed effects and survey day as a random effect in a lmer model using the lme4 package (Bates et al. 2015) in R. Stepwise comparisons indicated that food type and GUD station had little effect on model results, and thus were not retained in the most parsimonious model. Model fit was assessed using residual diagnostics (Hartig 2021). Post hoc comparisons between the treatment groups and for each species between treatments were made on the final model using estimated marginal means via emmeans (Lenth 2021), which allows for the error variances that have been specified in the model, and Tukey P-value adjustments for comparing the families of three and twelve estimates.

To test our second prediction concerning the attraction of V. gouldii to small mammal odour, we counted the numbers of artificial burrows that elicited a response from V. gouldii. Responses were recorded as digging activity or as tracks within 30 cm of the artificial burrows that indicated investigative activity, and were analyzed separately. Analyses were carried out separately for the species complements present in 2012 (i.e., with the inclusion of R. villosissimus – phase 1) and 2013–2014 (with S. youngsoni – phase 2). Survey session was considered a nominal factor that differed for each new location of the artificial burrows within one of the three main study locations. Survey day within each survey session was also considered a nominal factor. Generalized least squares (GLS) models were used to account for heterogeneity of variances, which were observed across the treatments, survey days, survey sessions, and locations. Following Zuur et al. (2009) we incorporated the nominal variables using the nlme package (Pinheiro et al. 2021) in R to implement GLS with a variance structure that allowed for differences in variance across survey days per survey session per location. We also incorporated an exponential variance structure for the covariance to account for the variance spread across different survey sessions alone or by survey day per survey session, depending on the data and best fit model. Log likelihood and Akaike information criterion comparisons were used to determine the most parsimonious models. The fixed explanatory variables retained in all models were treatment odour, survey day, and survey session; location was not retained as it had no significant effect. The only other difference between the models was that survey day was not retained in the exponential variance structure for models of the track data for both species complements. Graphical validation of the optimal model was obtained by comparing box plots of the raw data against the model estimates, by plotting the residuals against the fitted values for the nominal explanatory variables, and via histograms and Q-Q plots of the model residuals (Zuur et al. 2009). Post hoc comparisons between the treatment groups were made on the final model using estimated marginal means via emmeans (Lenth 2021), which allows for the error variances that have been specified in the GLS model, and Tukey P-value adjustments for comparing a family of four estimates. All analyses were carried out in R (R core team 2021).

To analyse small mammal movements and burrow fidelity (prediction 3), we mapped the locations of animals that we had sighted and estimated the positions of triangulated animals using an iterative maximum likelihood estimator (LOAS, Ecological Software Solutions, Sacramento, California). Movements were calculated as the distance covered between successive signal locations, per unit time (m/h), with the distance (di) moved by an individual between its first signal location i(xi, yi) and the next (xi+1, yi+1) calculated using White and Garrott’s (1990) equation:

$$di=\surd {\left({x}_{i}+1 - {x}_{i}\right)}^{2}+{({y}_{i}+1-{y}_{i})}^{2}$$

We assessed small mammal fidelity to burrows, (f), using the index: f = Nmax/N.

where Nmax is the maximum number of visits by an individual to the same burrow over the period of radio-tracking, and N is the total number of its visits to all burrows. Index values thus range from f = 1 for an individual that uses a single burrow to f = 1/N for an individual that uses all burrows once. The index formula is the same as the Berger-Parker index of species dominance (Magurran 2004), and its use here follows Dickman et al. (2010). Only small numbers of the study species were radio-tracked on each of the three sampling occasions, so results were pooled and rates of movement and burrow fidelity compared for each species between the control and V. gouldii removal areas using analysis of variance. The burrow fidelity data for S. youngsoni were log-transformed to improve the variance structure prior to analysis; no other transformations were required.

Results

Prediction 1: Small Mammals will Recognize and Avoid the Odour of V. gouldii

On average, more than half the GUD sites (57%) were visited each night by the study species, with another 11% of sites disturbed by wind or other species. Rattus villosissimus was absent from the study area in November 2014 and hence provided no results. However, small numbers of another Sminthopsis species, the hairy-footed dunnart S. hirtipes (15 g), were present, allowing its response to V. gouldii odour to be investigated. More visits were made by the study species to GUD sites with no odour over the seven nights of the experiment (total visits = 91: N. alexis, n = 17; P. hermannsburgensis, n = 31; S. youngsoni, n = 39; S. hirtipes, n = 4) than to sites with the pungency control (total visits = 80: N. alexis, n = 16; P. hermannsburgensis, n = 28; S. youngsoni, n = 32; S. hirtipes, n = 4), with fewest visits made to sites with the odour of V. gouldii (total visits = 43: N. alexis, n = 9; P. hermannsburgensis, n = 14; S. youngsoni, n = 18; S. hirtipes, n = 2).

The foraging responses of all four species were similar across the three odour treatments. GUDs were significantly higher (more food remained after foraging) in the treatment bearing the odour of V. gouldii than in the control and pungency control treatments (Table 1, Fig. 1).

Table 1 Linear mixed-effects model results of small mammal giving-up density (GUD) responses to treatments of no odour (Control), a pungency control (Eucalyptus), and sand goanna Varanus gouldii faecal material (Goanna) at food patches. Estimates are given as the first treatment in the contrast as compared to the second, larger estimated mean. Significant results are denoted by an asterisk
Fig. 1
figure 1

Boxplots of raw data showing the giving-up density (GUD) responses of four species of small mammals to treatments of no odour (Control), a pungency control (Eucalyptus), and sand goanna Varanus gouldii faecal material (Goanna) at food patches. The y-axis shows the number of food items, mealworms or peanuts, left behind at food patches after overnight periods of foraging (the GUD)

Prediction 2: Varanus gouldii will be Attracted to the Odour of Small Mammals

In phase 1, when R. villosissimus was present, V. gouldii dug up or scratched at the entrance of artificial burrows containing the odour of R. villosissimus on 80 occasions over 1365 'burrow-days' (1 burrow-day = 1 burrow containing the species' odour over 1 day), and in phase 2 V. gouldii showed digging activity at burrows containing the odour of S. youngsoni on 88 occasions over 2028 burrow-days. Over both phases V. gouldii showed digging activity at burrows containing the odour of N. alexis on 79 occasions and at burrows with P. hermannsburgensis odour on 203 occasions over a total of 3393 burrow-days. Control burrows were excavated or scratched at on only seven occasions over 3393 burrow-days. Tracks of V. gouldii, with no digging activity, followed a similar pattern but were recorded less frequently (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Boxplots of raw data showing the digging (dig) and investigative (tracks) responses of sand goannas Varanus gouldii at artificial burrows that contained no odour (control) and the odours of small mammalian prey species in two time periods, Phase 1: A) Notomys alexis, Pseudomys hermannsburgensis, and Rattus villosissimus; and Phase 2: B) Notomys alexis, Pseudomys hermannsburgensis, and Sminthopsis youngsoni. The y-axis shows the count of artificial burrows that were observed to have been dug up or visited each day

Model results showed that V. gouldii excavated burrows containing small mammal odour significantly more often than the odourless control burrows in both phase 1 and phase 2 (Table 2, Fig. 2). Digs at P. hermannsburgensis burrows occurred consistently in both phases (Table 2, Fig. 2), but were not significantly more frequent than those at the burrows of R. villosissimus in phase 1 or S. youngsonii in phase 2 (Table 3). However, V. gouldii dug at burrows containing the odour of N. alexis significantly less often than at those containing the odours of other small mammals (Table 3). The investigative response of V. gouldii, as shown by tracks, was also less at control, odour-free burrows than at all other treatment burrows except for those containing the odour of S. youngsoni; burrows with the odour of this latter species elicited very little response from V. gouldii (Table 2, Fig. 2).

Table 2 Generalised least squares model results of digging and investigative (track) responses of sand goannas Varanus gouldii at artificial burrows that contained no odour (Control) or the odours of small mammalian prey species in two time periods: Phase 1, Notomys alexis (Na), Pseudomys hermannsburgensis (Ph), and Rattus villosissimus (Rv); and Phase 2, Notomys alexis (Na), Pseudomys hermannsburgensis (Ph), and Sminthopsis youngsoni (Sy). Significant results are denoted by an asterisk
Table 3 Estimated marginal means in treatment group comparisons from generalised least squares models testing digging and investigative (track) responses of sand goannas Varanus gouldii at artificial burrows that contained the odours of small mammalian prey species in two time periods: Phase 1: Notomys alexis (Na), Pseudomys hermannsburgensis (Ph), and Rattus villosissimus (Rv); and Phase 2: Notomys alexis (Na), Pseudomys hermannsburgensis (Ph), and Sminthopsis youngsoni (Sy). Significant results are denoted by an asterisk

Prediction 3: Small Mammals will be less Mobile and will Show Higher Burrow Fidelity in the Absence than the Presence of V. gouldii

Between six and eight individuals of each small mammals species were radio-tracked over periods of 2–7 days, with 5–31 location fixes obtained per individual. Rates of movement (m/h: grand mean ± SE) varied between species (N. alexis: 87.8 ± 22.4; P. hermannsburgensis: 93.0 ± 22.2; S. youngsoni: 239.8 ± 39.1), but were no different for each species when compared between the control and V. gouldii removal areas (Table 4). Overall, animals used 42 different burrows on 78 occasions, with individuals of each species using either the same burrow or different burrows for 4–5 consecutive days. Burrow fidelity (grand mean ± SE) was relatively high for N. alexis (0.807 ± 0.077) and no different for this species between the control and V. gouldii removal areas (Table 4). Overall burrow fidelity was less for both P. hermannsburgensis (0.604 ± 0.078) and S. youngsoni (0.586 ± 0.140), with fidelity to burrows less in the control area than in the area where V. gouldii had been removed (Table 4).

Table 4 Rates of movement (m/h) and fidelity to the same burrow by three species of radio-tracked small mammals in areas where the sand goanna Varanus gouldii was present (+ Goanna) and absent (- Goanna); means are shown ± SE with the number of each species in each treatment that were tracked (n), and significant results are denoted by an asterisk

Discussion

The results provide general support for our three initial predictions. Thus, small mammals that form part of the diet of V. gouldii recognised and avoided the odour of this apex reptilian predator, feeding less intensively at sites with V. gouldii odour than at sites with a pungency control or no odour. In its turn, V. gouldii was attracted to artificial burrows containing the odour of small mammals, and dug more frequently at burrows with small mammal odour than at odourless control burrows. Finally, two of three species of small mammal showed lower fidelity to their burrows in the presence than in the absence of V. gouldii, suggesting that this goanna exerts an influence on the tenure of small mammals in their burrow systems. These findings are the first to demonstrate reciprocal odour-based interactions between an apex reptilian predator and small mammalian prey, and provide novel evidence that top down predation by a varanid contributes to the nomadic burrow-shifting behaviour that distinguishes many of Australia's small desert mammals from their counterparts in other world deserts. We discuss the implications of our results in more detail below.

The GUD results provided a strong indication that small mammals recognize and avoid the odour of V. gouldii, and the similarity in GUD response by each species to the no-odour and pungency control treatments suggests further that this response is specific and not due to odour strength or volatility. The distribution of V. gouldii largely encompasses that of each of the small mammal species, with the area of overlap exceeding a million square kilometres in arid central Australia (cf. distribution maps in Cogger 2014 and Van Dyck and Strahan 2008). As the aridification of Australia began in the mid-Miocene some 15 million years ago and intensified over the last 1 – 4 million years (Byrne et al. 2008), it is therefore likely that coevolution between V. gouldii and its mammalian prey has been prolonged and extensive. Comparative research on the responses of mammals to other varanid predators is limited, but our findings accord with strong aversive responses that are shown by the common ringtail possum Pseudocheirus peregrinus to the faecal odour of the lace monitor Varanus varius (Anson and Dickman 2013).

Although odour cues such as predator faeces provide ostensibly well-founded information to prey about predation risk, prey responses are often variable and balanced according to the degree of risk that they perceive (Dickman 1992; Apfelbach et al. 2005; Sih et al. 2010) or to differences in past experience, age or health (Bedoya-Pérez et al. 2019). Our GUD results suggest that V. gouldii faecal odour conveys considerable risk to small mammals. This varanid includes small mammals in its diet and the presence of fresh faeces therefore should provide reliable information that a predator is not far away (Canteras et al. 2015). However, all the small mammals are nocturnal and V. gouldii is diurnal (Gordon et al. 2010), so it might be expected that small mammals could forage relatively safely even in places where odour of the varanid is strong. There are two reasons why such foraging may still be risky. Firstly, in the hot and often humid conditions that prevail over the Austral summer in arid Australia, some crepuscular activity of V. gouldii and small mammals is likely, thus increasing the chance of direct encounters between predator and prey. Secondly, if site-based foraging is prolonged—as would be needed to produce low GUDs—this would intensify the deposition of odour by small mammals at the foraging sites, and thus leave stronger cues to investigate for nearby V. gouldii. In both cases small mammals should reduce their risk of predation by minimising their foraging time where fresh V. gouldii faecal odour is present compared with where this odour cue is absent.

The results showed further that the four species of small mammals to visit GUD bowls responded similarly to the odour treatments. Given that N. alexis and S. hirtipes occurred less frequently than P. hermannsburgensis in the diet of V. gouldii (Online Resource 1), this might suggest that there are differences in predation risk among these species and hence also that species-specific differences in the magnitude of their GUD responses would be expected. This expectation is all the more reasonable if V. gouldii targets some species more intensively than others or if the small mammal species differ in their ability to escape once detected, as occurs in the interaction of small mammals with feral cats Felis catus and red foxes Vulpes vulpes in our study area (Spencer et al. 2014a,b). Alternatively, as V. gouldii is an opportunistic predator that consumes a wide range of vertebrate and invertebrate prey (Cross et al. 2020), it is possible that V. gouldii finds and consumes small mammal species in relation to their abundance. Thus, in the long term trapping record for the study area, from 1990, P. hermannsburgensis was the most frequently captured small mammal and S. hirtipes one of the most scarce (Greenville et al. 2016b). If V. gouldii exploits small mammals roughly in relation to their abundance it implies that the per capita risk of predation for each species is similar and could account for the between-species similarity in the GUD results. A larger dietary sample for V. gouldii is needed to confirm this.

Another factor that will influence the vulnerability of small mammals to predation from V. gouldii is the extent to which this varanid can effectively exploit their burrow odours. The results derived from our second prediction confirm that V. gouldii is attracted to small mammal burrow odours but is markedly less interested in burrows that are free of odour. Thus, burrows with no odour were dug into on just seven occasions compared with a total of 450 occasions for burrows containing the odour of a small mammal. On six of the seven occasions when a control burrow was excavated, a burrow containing small mammal odour in the same site cluster was also dug up or investigated. The tracks of V. gouldii were recorded at control burrows on 29 occasions, and on most of these the tracks indicated that the varanid had simply walked across the smoothed sand plot without changing stride. By contrast, tracks were recorded at 164 burrows that contained small mammal odour, and at most of these there was evidence that the varanid had broken stride or altered direction to investigate further, even if no subsequent digging occurred. These findings support our second prediction and indicate that V. gouldii responds strongly to the burrow odour of small mammals by increasing its exploratory or digging activity. Tracking of prey odours has been documented in other terrestrial vertebrates (Müller-Schwarze 2006), marine predators such as sharks (Tester 1963), and in many insects that follow windborne odour plumes (Cardé 2021), suggesting that olfactory eavesdropping provides benefits for a diverse range of predators.

Despite our results, there is evidence that V. gouldii does not respond to the odours of different small mammal species equally. This varanid dug less and showed less investigatory interest (via its tracks) at burrows containing the odour of N. alexis than the other species we studied. This result supports part ii of our second prediction that V. gouldii should be less attracted to the odours of deep-burrowing species that would be costly to excavate, in contrast to the odours of shallow burrowers such as P. hermannsburgensis and S. youngsoni. However, V. gouldii also dug frequently and showed interest in burrows containing the odour of R. villosissimus, which we had not expected. Burrows of the latter species may descend to at least 80 cm and have more than a dozen entry or exit holes that are connected by ~ 20 m of tunnel (Predavec and Dickman 1994). We suggest two possible reasons for the unexpected interest of V. gouldii in burrows imbued with the odour of R. villosissimus. Firstly, the large size of the rat (120 g) would provide a greater energetic yield once captured than any of the other study species and hence would reward a greater allocation of effort in hunting it. If multiple rats occupy a burrow system, this would additionally maximise the chance that one or more individuals could be captured. Secondly, R. villosissimus excavates a second type of burrow that is shallower and simpler than the first. This burrow type is excavated mainly on the sides of dunes (Predavec and Dickman 1994) where most of our artificial burrows were set. Digging R. villosissimus from such burrows would likely maximise the energetic returns of V. gouldii.

Support for our third prediction, that small mammals would be less mobile and show higher burrow fidelity in the absence than in the presence of V. gouldii, was mixed. Rates of movement of small mammals were the same whether V. gouldii was present or not, but fidelity to burrows increased in two of the three species that were radio-tracked in the area where V. gouldii was removed. Our sample sizes in the tracking experiment were small and rates of movement were highly variable within and between species, so it is possible that our results lacked the power to detect any differences in movements that were present. The rates of movement of both the native rodents were similar to those reported in previous studies (Letnic 2002; Dickman et al. 2010, 2011), as were the greater rates of movement of S. youngsoni (Letnic 2002; Haythornthwaite and Dickman 2006). Several authors have interpreted the extensive movements and drifting home ranges of small mammals in arid Australia as responses to continual shifts in the availability of food or other resources across the landscape (e.g., Morton 1978; Read 1984; Dickman et al. 1995; Pavey et al. 2017). However, our radio-tracking took place soon after heavy rains when small mammal populations were at peak abundance and resources could be expected to be reliably present (Dickman et al. 2014; Greenville et al. 2016b). It would be of considerable interest to evaluate whether small mammals exhibit different patterns of movement in response to the presence of V. gouldii when conditions are less productive.

Despite the similar rates of movement of our study species across the experimental treatments, both P. hermannsburgensis and S. youngsoni returned to the same burrows more consistently in the absence than in the presence of V. gouldii. Burrows are probably costly to excavate, even in sandy soils, but should repay the return on investment if they provide security and the excavators can return to them on a regular basis. By extension, use of different burrows is likely to be more costly. Additional costs would be incurred if new burrows are continually constructed or, as with S. youngsoni, if animals take the risk of using burrows constructed by other fauna that may be hostile to their burrows being shared or usurped. Our results suggest that the presence of V. gouldii skews the cost:benefit ratio of burrow use such that the costs of continually moving to different burrows are exceeded by the costs of being detected and depredated by this varanid. In other words, small mammals experience a clear fitness benefit from burrow-hopping if V. gouldii is present. If V. gouldii is scarce, by contrast, the risk of being discovered in a burrow by the predator is diminished and the benefits of consistent burrow use then result in increased burrow fidelity. Notably, N. alexis showed no difference in burrow fidelity between the control and treatment areas. This species digs multi-entrance burrows that descend to 1 m and can evade predators in the open by its rapidity of movement (up to 4.5 m/s: Stanley 1971) and ability to change direction quickly and unpredictably (Watts and Aslin 1981). This species thus may face little risk of injury or death in the presence of V. gouldii, even if it is detected, and individuals presumably derive net benefit from making consistent use of their burrows whether the sand goanna is present or absent.

We interpret patterns of burrow usage in small mammals such as P. hermannsburgensis and S. youngsoni in terms of odour-mediated interactions. On the one hand, high burrow fidelity by these species likely results in accumulations of odours at burrows and their immediate surrounds, providing a strong cue for an olfactory predator such as V. gouldii to investigate and excavate them. Low burrow fidelity should reduce these olfactory cues. On the other hand, the observed reduction in burrow fidelity by two of our three study species in the presence of V. gouldii implies that they had detected the predator and were responding to it. Although we cannot confirm that olfaction was the modality used, it is the only modality that seems plausible: odour from sources such as goanna faeces is long lasting (> 24 h in our experiments) and clearly recognized by small mammals. It is highly improbable, moreover, that small mammals could reliably use auditory or visual cues to determine the presence of V. gouldii owing to the short period of joint overlap in their daily activities. We conclude therefore that olfaction plays a large role in the predator–prey dynamics of this apex reptilian predator and its mammalian prey, and is the most likely driver of the highly unusual and perhaps unique burrow-shifting behaviour that is displayed by many of Australia's small desert mammals. Further experimental work is needed to clarify which infochemicals small mammals use to detect and respond to V. gouldii at the landscape level, and whether differences in prey age, health, or experience alter the level of risk that they perceive. Future research could profitably also explore the extent to which infochemicals play a role in shaping other interactions (e.g., diffuse competition Dickman et al. 2020) between small mammals and reptiles elsewhere in arid Australia and desert regions in southern Africa and North America (Pianka and Vitt 2003) where species-rich assemblages of these taxa occur.