Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Reading Failure in a Completely Transparent Orthography Representing a Morphologically Highly Complex Agglutinative Language: the Case of Turkish

  • ORIGINAL ARTICLE
  • Published:
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigates differences in the word processing skills of students with and without reading difficulties who read Turkish, an entirely transparent orthography. Thirty-five students diagnosed as poor readers and 51 typically developing controls were tested across two experiments, one that assessed their ability to process identicalness of isolated real words as opposed to pseudowords and another that assessed their ability to judge semantic relatedness of two real words. Participants were from two education levels; half of them were 3rd-4th graders and half were 6th–7th graders. An integrative view of the findings points to an apparent failure of Turkish poor readers to develop a lexicalized reading route that mediates word recognition by means of permanent orthographic knowledge. Moreover, their ability to effectively process word letter sequences along a non-lexical grapheme-to-phoneme conversion-based reading route was found to be seriously restricted. Findings are discussed with direct reference to orthographic transparency, dual-route reading theory and the orthographic self-teaching concept.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Avdyli, R., Castejon, L., & Cuetos, F. (2014). Lexical effects in word naming in Spanish children. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 17(e23), 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergmann, J., & Wimmer, H. (2008). A dual-route perspective on poor reading in a regular orthography: Evidence from phonological and orthographic lexical decisions. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25(5), 653–676.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bianco, M., Bressoux, P., Doyen, A. L., Lambert, E., Lima, L., Pellenq, C., & Zorman, M. (2010). Early training in oral comprehension and phonological skills: Results of a three-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14(3), 211–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (2005). Modeling reading: The dual-route approach. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 6–23). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, J. W. (2001). The national reading panel report. Reading Research Quarterly, 36(3), 326–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Filippo, G., de Luca, M., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Lexicality and stimulus length effects in Italian dyslexics: Role of the over additivity effect. Child Neuropsychology, 12(2), 141–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers, 35(1), 116–124.

  • Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 263–329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, C., Tolar, T. D., Reese, L., Francis, D. J., Bazán, A. R., & Mejía-Arauz, R. (2014). How important is teaching phonemic awareness to children learning to read in Spanish? American Educational Research Journal, 51(3), 604–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guldenoglu, B. (2016). The effects of syllable-awareness skills on the word-reading performances of students reading in a transparent orthography. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 8(3), 425–442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, N. E., & Coltheart, M. (2001). Routes to reading success and failure: Toward an integrated cognitive psychology of atypical reading. New York: Psychological Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. D. (1999). Seeking a role for grammar: A review of some recent studies. Foreign Language Annals, 32, 245–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S.D. (2001). The comprehension hypothesis and its rivals. Selected papers from the eleventh international symposium on English teaching/fourth pan-Asian conference, 395–404.

  • Krashen, S. D. (2002). Explorations in language acquisition and use: The Taipei lectures. Taipei: Crane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I. (2006). Early language development as related to the acquisition of reading. European Review, 14, 65–79. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1062798706000068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322–352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2005). Reading experience and changes in the processing of letters, written words, and pseudo-homophones: Comparing fifth-grade students and university students. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164, 407–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2007). The role of phonology in the word decoding skills of poor readers: Evidence from individuals with prelingual deafness or diagnosed dyslexia. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 19, 385–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P., Kargin, T., & Guldenoglu, B. (2014). Differences in the Reading of shallow and deep orthography: Developmental evidence from Hebrew and Turkish readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 37(4), 409–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller-Shaul, S. (2005). The characteristics of young and adult dyslexic readers on reading and reading related cognitive tasks as compared to normal readers. Dyslexia, 11, 132–151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, K., Ramus, F., Bartling, J., Bruder, J., Kunze, S., Neuhoff, N., et al. (2014). Cognitive mechanisms underlying reading and spelling development in five European orthographies. Learning and Instruction, 29, 65–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moss, H. E., & Tyler, L. K. (1995). Investigating semantic memory impairments: The contribution of semantic priming. Memory, 3(3–4), 359–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moss, H. E., Tyler, L. K., Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (1995). Exploring the loss of semantic memory in semantic dementia: Evidence from a primed monitoring study. Neuropsychology, 9(1), 16–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. (1998a). Semantic processing and the development of word recognition skills: Evidence from children with reading comprehension difficulties. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 85–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1998b). Individual differences in contextual facilitation: Evidence from dyslexia and poor reading comprehension. Child Development, 69, 996–1011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1999). Developmental differences in sensitivity to semantic relations among good and poor comprehenders: Evidence from semantic priming. Cognition, 70(1), –13.

  • Niemi, P., Poskiparta, E., & Vauras, M. (2001). Benefits of training in linguistic awareness dissipate by grade 3. The Journal of the Hellenic Psychological Society, 8, 330–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Öney, B., & Durgunoğlu, A. (1997). Beginning to read in Turkish: A phonologically transparent orthography. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 18, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raman, I. (2006). On the age-of-acquisition effects in word naming and orthographic transparency: Mapping specific or universal? Visual Cognition, 13(7–8), 1044–1053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raman, I., Baluch, B., & Sneddon, P. (1996). What is the cognitive system's preferred route for deriving phonology from print? European Psychologist, 1, 221–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raman, I., Raman, E., & Mertan, B. (2014). A standardized set of 260 pictures for Turkish: Norms of name and image agreement, age of acquisition, visual complexity, and conceptual familiarity. Behavior Research Methods, 46(2), 588–595.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Report of the National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction (NIH publication no. 00–4769). Washington: GPO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., Coltheart, M., & Castles, A. (2015). Getting to the bottom of orthographic depth. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(6), 1614–1629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., & Fowler, A. E. (2004). Questions people ask about the role of phonological processes in learning to read. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 483–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2004). Orthographic learning at a glance: On the time course and developmental onset of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 267–298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an "outlier" orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 584–615. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2005). Dyslexia (specific reading disability). Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1301–1309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2013). Psychopathology of dyslexia and reading disorders. In A. Davis (Ed.), Psychopathology of childhood and adolescence: A neuropsychological approach (pp. 109–126). New York: Springer Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Svensson, N., & Jacobson, C. (2005). How persistent are phonological difficulties? A longitudinal study of reading retarded children. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 12(1), 3–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Troia, G. (2004). Phonological processing and its influence on literacy learning. In C. Stone, E. Silliman, B. Ehren, & K. Appel (Eds.), Handbook of language and literacy: Development and disorders (pp. 271–301). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E. (2008). Recent developments in reading intervention research: Introduction to special issue. Reading and Writing, 21, 299–316. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9108-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W., & Greaney, K. (2010). Defining dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(3), 229–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zecker, S. G., & Zinner, T. E. (1987). Semantic code deficit for reading disabled children on an auditory lexical decision task. Journal of Reading Behavior, 19(2), 177–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86(3), 169–193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to express their gratitude to all educational institutes and participants that took part in this study.

Funding

This study was funded by the SLC on Visual Language and Visual Learning (VL2), NSF: Grant # SBE-0541953

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Miller.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

The reported study does not violate any fundamental ethical considerations for the protection of humans or animals. Its compliance with ethical standards has been approved by the IRB of the authors’ respective academic institutions.

Informed Consent

The study was executed only after getting approval from all relevant agencies, including from the participants themselves.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 6 Examples of stimulus pairs used in the three experimental paradigms

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Miller, P., Guldenoglu, B. & Kargin, T. Reading Failure in a Completely Transparent Orthography Representing a Morphologically Highly Complex Agglutinative Language: the Case of Turkish. J Dev Phys Disabil 31, 669–689 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-019-09667-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-019-09667-3

Keywords

Navigation