Skip to main content
Log in

Interplay between order-parameter and system parameter dynamics: considerations on perceptual-cognitive-behavioral mode-mode transitions exhibiting positive and negative hysteresis and on response times

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Biological Physics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A mathematical model is presented for the emergence of perceptual-cognitive-behavioral modes in psychophysical experiments in which participants are confronted with two alternatives. The model is based on the theory of self-organization and, in particular, the order parameter concept such that the emergence of a mode is conceptualized as an instability leading to the emergence of an appropriately defined order parameter. The order parameter model is merged with a second model that describes adaptation in terms of a system parameter dynamics. It is shown that the two-component model predicts hysteretic mode-mode transitions when control parameters are increased or decreased beyond critical values. The two-component model can account for both positive and negative hysteresis effects due to the interaction between order parameter and system parameter dynamics. Moreover, the model-based analysis reveals that response time curves look rather flat when response times are relatively decoupled from the mode-mode transition phenomenon. In general, response time curves exhibit a peaked close to the mode-mode transition point. In this context, the possibility is discussed that such peaked response time curves belong to the class of critical phenomena of self-organizing systems. In order to illustrate the relevance of peaked response time curves for future research and research reported in the past, results from a perceptual judgment experiment are reported, in which participants judged their ability to stand on a tilted slope for various angles of inclination. Response time curves were found that exhibited a peak around the mode-mode-transition points between “yes” and “no” responses.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In our study, growth parameters occur in the order parameter dynamics as a-priori unknown parameters as a result of the proposed top-down modeling approach. As such the growth parameters reflect functions of physical parameter that would occur in mechanistic, bottom-up modeling approaches. For example, for the amplitude equations governing the emergence of roll patterns due to a Benard instability the growth parameters can be expressed in terms of physical parameters from hydrodynamics [22]. In our context, it is assumed that the growth parameters depend both on physical quantities of the environment (e.g., inclination angle of a ramp or platform for standing upon), body-dimensions (body height, weight, etc) and parameters of the neural and muscular-skeletal system (e.g., synaptic weights). For a given subject and a given task these physical parameters may vary in a certain range which then implies that the growth parameters take on values from a certain interval.

  2. In fact, the notion of a search algorithm is not useful for any physical system being self-organized or not. For example, let us consider a diffusion process. Let us assume a closed container fill with air. The container walls and the air molecules are at room temperature. Inside the container there is a bottle of perfume. The lid can be opened by remote control. Let us assume we open the lid. The perfume molecules diffuse out of the bottle and distribute themselves across the container. Eventually, the molecules are distributed uniformly in space and exhibit a Maxwell–Boltzmann velocity distribution. In doing so, the stationary distribution corresponds to a maximum entropy solution. Consequently, the perfume molecules effectively solve an optimization problem under constraints. That is, the molecules configure themselves such that the entropy is maximized under given isothermal boundary conditions and spatial boundaries given by the container walls. However, the molecules do not perform a search for the correct solution. The molecules simply satisfy a Boltzmann transport equation (a diffusion equation) and the time it takes to approach the stationary state will be determined by the diffusion constant (a parameter of the transport equation). This is another example of a time scale (here the relaxation time of the diffusion process) that is determined by the laws of physics and is not related to the performance of a search algorithm.

References

  1. Ditzinger, T., Haken, H.: Oscillations in the perception of ambiguous patterns: a model based on synergetics. Biol. Cybern. 61, 279–287 (1989)

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  2. Newell, K.M., Scully, D.M., Tenenbaum, F., Hardiman, S.: Body scale the development of prehension. Dev. Psychobiol. 22, 1–13 (1989)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Newell, K.M., Scully, D.M., McDonald, P.V., Baillargeon, R.: Task constraints configuration, infant grip. Dev. Psychobiol. 22, 817–832 (1989)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Richardson, M.J., Marsh, K.L., Baron, R.M.: Judging and actualizing intrapersonal and interpersonal affordances. J. Exp. Psychol. - Hum. Percept. Perform 33, 845–859 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Lopresti-Goodman, S.M., Richardson, M., Baron, M.J., Carello, C., Marsh, K.L.: Task constraints on affordance boundaries. Mot. Control. 13, 69–83 (2009)

    Google Scholar 

  6. Lopresti-Goodman, S.M., Turvey, M.T., Frank, T.D.: Behavioral dynamics of the affordance “graspable”, attention perception. Psychophys. 73, 1948–1965 (2011)

    Google Scholar 

  7. Stephen, D.G., Isenhower, R.W., Dixon, J.A.: Dynamics of representational change: entropy, action, and cognition. J. Exp. Psychol. - Hum. Percept. Perform 35, 1811–1832 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Warren, W.H.: Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. J. Exp. Psychol. - Hum. Percept. Perform. 10, 683–703 (1984)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Faisal, A.A., Selen, L.P.J., Wolpert, D.M.: Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 292–303 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Gibson, J.J.: The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Houghton-Mifflin, Boston (1979)

  11. Turvey, M.T.: Coordination. Am. Psychol. 45, 938–953 (1990)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Beek, P.J., Peper, C.E., Stegeman, D.F.: Dynamical models of movement coordination. Hum. Mov. Sci. 14, 573–608 (1995)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Kelso, J.A.S.: Dynamic Patterns - The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. MIT Press, Cambridge (1995)

  14. Haken, H.: Principles of Brain Functioning. Springer, Berlin (1996)

    Book  MATH  Google Scholar 

  15. Warren, W.H.: The dynamics of perception and action. Psychol. Rev. 113, 358–389 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Araujo, D., Davids, K., Hristovski, R.: The ecological dynamics of decision-making in sport. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 7, 653–676 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Haken, H. Synergetics. An Introduction. Springer, Berlin (1977)

  18. Haken, H.: Synergetic Computers and Cognition. Springer, Berlin (1991)

  19. Tschacher, W., Dauwalder, J.P.: Situated cognition, ecological perception, and synergetics: a novel perspective for cognitive psychology? In: Tschacher, W., Dauwalder, J.P. (eds.) : Dynamics, Synergetics, Autonomous Agents, pp 83–104. World Scientific, Singapore (1999)

  20. Schiepek, G., Strunk, G.: The identification of critical fluctuations and phase transitions in short term and coarse-grained time series: a method for real-time monitoring of human change processes. Biol. Cybern. 102, 197–207 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Saltzman, E.L., Munhall, K.G.: Skill acquisition and development: the roles of state-, parameter-, and graph-dynamics. J. Motor Behav. 24, 49–57 (1992)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Frank, T.D.: Multistable pattern formation systems: candidates for physical intelligence. Ecol. Psychol. 24, 220–240 (2012)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Bestehorn, M., Haken, H.: Associative memory of a dynamical system: an example of the convection instability. Z. Physik. B 82, 305–308 (1991)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  24. Bestehorn, M., Friedrich, R., Haken, H.: Two-dimensional traveling wave patterns in nonequilibrium systems. Z. Physik B 75, 265–274 (1989)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  25. Frank, T.D., van der Kamp, J., Savelsbergh, G.J.P.: On a multistable dynamic model of behavioral and perceptual infant development. Dev. Psychobiol. 52, 352–371 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Frank, T.D., Richardson, M.J., Lopresti-Goodman, S.M., Turvey, M.T.: Order parameter dynamics of body-scaled hysteresis and mode transitions in grasping behavior. J. Biol. Phys. 35, 127–147 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Frank, T.D.: Motor development during infancy: a nonlinear physics approach to emergence, multistability, and simulation. In: Columbus, A.M. (ed.) Advances in Psychology Research, vol. 83, Chap. 9, pp. 143–160. Nova Publ., New York (2011)

  28. Frank, T.D.: On a multistable competitive network model in the case of an inhomogeneous growth rate spectrum with an application to priming. Phys. Lett. A 373, 4127–4133 (2009)

    Article  MATH  ADS  Google Scholar 

  29. Frank, T.D.: Psycho-thermodynamics of priming, recognition latencies, retrieval-induced forgetting, priming-induced recognition failures and psychopathological perception. In: Hsu, N., Schütt, Z. (eds.) Psychology of Priming, Chap. 9, pp 175–204. Nova Publ., New York (2012)

  30. Fuchs, A., Haken, H.: Pattern recognition and associative memory as dynamical processes in a synergetic system. II. Decomposition of complex scenes, simultaneous invariance with respect to translation, rotation, and scaling. Biol. Cybern. 60, 107–109 (1988)

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  31. Daffertshofer, A., Haken, H.: A new approach to recognition of deformed patterns. Pattern Recog. 27, 1697–1705 (1994)

  32. Yudashkin, A.A.: Bifurcations of steady-state solutions in the synergetic neural network and control of pattern recognition. Auto Remote Control. 57, 1647–1653 (1996)

    MATH  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  33. Bressloff, P.C., Roper, P.: Stochastic dynamics of the diffusive Haken model with subthreshold periodic forcing. Phys. Rev. E 58, 2282–2287 (1998)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  34. Nakagawa, M.: A circularly connected synergetic neural network. IEICE Trans. Fund. Electr. Commu. Comput. Sci. E83A, 909–922 (2000)

  35. Frank, T.D.: New perspectives on pattern recognition algorithm based on Haken’s synergetic computer network. In: Fournier, M.D. (ed.) Perspective on Pattern Recognition, Chap. 7, pp 153–172. Nova Publ., New York (2011)

  36. Haken, H., Schanz, M., Starke, J.: Treatment of combinatorial optimization problems using selection equations with cost terms. Part I Physica. D 134, 227–241 (1999)

  37. Starke, J., Schanz, M., Haken, H.: Treatment of combinatorial optimization problems using selection equations with cost terms. Part II Physica. D 134, 242–252 (1999)

    Article  MATH  MathSciNet  ADS  Google Scholar 

  38. Starke, J., Ellsaessar, C., Fukuda, T.: Self-organized control in cooperative robots using a pattern formation principle. Phys. Lett. A 375, 2094–2098 (2011)

  39. Frank, T.D.: Multistable selection equations of pattern formation type in the case of inhomogeneous growth rates: with applications to two-dimensional assignment problems. Phys. Lett. A 375, 1465–1469 (2011)

    Article  MATH  ADS  Google Scholar 

  40. Daffertshofer, A.: How do ensembles occupy space?. Eur. Phys. J. Spec. Top. 157, 79–91 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Stanley, H.E. Introduction to Phase Transitions and Critical Phenomena. Oxford University Press, New York (1971)

  42. Binnen, J.J., Dowrick, N.J., Fisher, A.J., Newman, M.E.J. The theory of critical phenomena. Oxford University Press, New York (1992)

  43. Nicolis, G. Introduction to Nonlinear Sciences. Cambridge University Press. Cambridge (1995)

  44. Nitzan, A., Ortoleva, P., Deutch, J., Ross, J.: Fluctuations and transitions at chemical instabilities: the analogy to phase transition. J. Chem. Phys. 61, 1056–1074 (1974)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  45. Frank, T.D., Daffertshofer, A., Beek, P.J., Haken, H.: Impacts of noise on a field theoretical model of the human brain. Physica. D 127, 233–249 (1999)

    Article  MATH  ADS  Google Scholar 

  46. Allain, C., Cummins, H.C., Lallemand, P.: Critical slowing down near the Rayleigh-Benard convection instability. Le J. de Phys. Lett. 24, L474–L477 (1978)

    Google Scholar 

  47. Wesfreid, J., Pomeau, Y., Dubois, M., Normand, C., Berge, P.: Critical effects in Rayleigh–Benard convection. Le J. de Phys. Lett. 7, 726–731 (1978)

    Google Scholar 

  48. Ecke, R.E., Haucke, H., Maeno, Y., Wheatley, J.C.: Critical dynamics at a Hopf bifurcation to oscillatory Rayleigh–Benard convection. Phys. Rev. A 33, 1870–1878 (1986)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  49. Bakonyi, A., Michaelis, D., Peschel, U., Onishchukov, G., Lederer, F.: Dissipative solitons and their critical slowing down near a supercritical bifurcation. J. Opt. Soc. Am. B 19, 487–491 (2002)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  50. Kelso, J.A.S., Scholz, J.P., Schöner, G.: Non-equilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations. Phys. Lett. A 118, 279–284 (1986)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  51. Schmidt, R.C., Carello, C., Turvey, M.T.: Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. J. Exp. Psychol. - Hum. Percept. Perform. 53, 247–257 (1990)

    Google Scholar 

  52. Scholz, J.P., Kelso, J.A.S., Schöner, G.S.: Non-equilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical slowing down and switching time, Phys. Lett. A 123, 390–394 (1987)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  53. Wallenstein, G.V., Kelso, J.A.S., Bressler, S.L.: Phase transitions in spatiotemporal patterns of brain activity and behavior. Physica D 84, 626–634 (1995)

    Article  MATH  ADS  Google Scholar 

  54. Wallenstein, G.V., Nash, A.J., Kelso, J.A.S.: Frequency and phase characteristics of slow cortical potentials preceding bimanual coordination, Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol 94, 50–59 (1995)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Diedrich, F.J., Warren, W.H.: Why change gaits? Dynamics of the walk-run transition. J. Exp. Psychol. - Hum. Percept. Perform. 21, 183–202 (1995)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Tuller, B., Case, P., Ding, M., Kelso, J.A.S.: The nonlinear dynamics of speech recognition. J. Exp. Psychol. - Hum. Percept. Perform. 20, 3–16 (1994)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Mackey, M.C., Glass, L.: Oscillations and chaos in physiological control systems. Science 197, 287–289 (1977)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  58. Glass, L., Mackey, M.C.: From Clocks to Chaos. Princeton University Press, Princeton (1988)

  59. Frank, T.D.: A limit cycle model for cycling mood variations of bipolar disorder patients derived from cellular biochemical reaction equations. Commun. Nonlinear Sci. Numer. Simul. 18, 2107–2119 (2013)

    Article  MATH  MathSciNet  ADS  Google Scholar 

  60. Haken, H., Schiepek, G.: Synergetik der Psychologie. Hogrefe, Gottingen (2006). (in German)

  61. Schöner, G. S., Kelso, J.A.S.: A synergetic theory of environmentally specified and learned patterns of movement coordination. I. Relative phase dynamics. II. Component oscillator dynamics. Biol. Cybern. 58, 71–89 (1988)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Frank, T.D., Blau, J.J.C., Turvey, M.T.: Nonlinear attractor dynamics in the fundamental and extended prism adaptation paradigm. Phys. Lett. A 373, 1022–1030 (2009)

    Article  MATH  ADS  Google Scholar 

  63. Frank, T.D., Blau, J.J. C., Turvey, M. T.: Symmetry breaking analysis of prism adaptations’s latent aftereffect. Cogn. Sci. 36, 674–697 (2012)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Kelso, J.A.S., Dumas, G., Tognoli, E., de Guzman, G.C.: Bridging neural, behavioral and social coordination dynamics using the human dynamic clamp Program No. 667.20/III45.2013. Neuroscience Meeting Planner. San Diego, Society for Neuroscience (2013). Online

  65. Lopresti-Goodman, S.M., Turvey, M.T., Frank, T.D.: Negative hysteresis in the behavioral dynamics of the affordance “graspable”. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 75, 1075–1091 (2013)

  66. Frank, T.D.: From systems biology to systems theory of bipolar disorder. In: Miranda, F. (ed.) Systems Theory: Perspectives, Applications and Developments, Chap. 2, pp 17–36. Nova Publ., New York (2014)

  67. Fitzpatrick, P., Carello, C., Schmidt, R.C., Corey, D.: Haptic and visual perception of an affordance for upright posture. Ecol. Psychol. 6, 265–287 (1994)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Diggle, P.J.: Time Series, a Biostatistical Introduction. The Clarendon Press, Oxford (1990)

  69. Turvey, M.T., Shaw, R.: The Primacy of Perceiving: An ecological reformulation of perception for understanding memory. In: Nilssen, L.G. (ed.) : Perspective on Memory Research: in Honor of Uppsala University’s 500th anniversary, Erlbaum, pp 167–222. Hillsdale, NJ (1979)

  70. Shaw, R., Turvey, M.T., Mace, W.: Ecological Psychology: The consequence of a commitment to realism. In: Weimer, W., Palermo, D. (eds.) : Cognition and the Symbolic Processes II, Erlbaum , pp 159–226. Hildsdale, NJ (1982)

  71. van Rooij, I.: Self-organization takes time too. Top. Cognit. Sci. 4, 63–71 (2012)

  72. Ingber, L.: Statistical mechanics of neocortical interactions. Dynamics of synaptic information. Phys. Rev. A 28, 395–416 (1983)

  73. Ingber, L.: Statistical mechanics of neocortical interactions: reaction time correlates of the g factor. Psycoloquy 10, article 068 (1999)

    Google Scholar 

  74. Wing, A.M., Kristofferson, A.B.: Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Percept. Psychophys. 14, 5–12 (1973)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Risken, H.: The Fokker-Planck Equation – Methods of Solution and Applications. Springer, Berlin (1989)

  76. Schöner, G.S., Haken, H., Kelso, J.A.S.: A stochastic theory of phase transitions in human hand movement. Biol. Cybern. 53, 247–257 (1986)

    Article  MATH  Google Scholar 

  77. Hick, W.: On the rate of gain of information. Q. J. Experiment. Psychol. 4, 11–26 (1952)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Hyman, R.: Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. J. Experiment. Psychol. 45, 188–196 (1953)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Frank, T.D.: Multistable perception in schizophrenia: a model-based analysis via coarse-grained order parameter dynamics and a comment on the 4th law. Univ. J. Psychol. 2, 231–240 (2014)

    Google Scholar 

  80. Frank, T.D.: Secondary bifurcations in a Lotka-Volterra model for n competitors with applications to action selection and compulsive behaviors. Int. J. Bif. and Chaos 24, article 1450156 (2014)

  81. Frank, T.D.: Action flow in obsessive-compulsive disorder rituals: a model based on extended synergetics and a comment on the 4th law. J. Adv. Phys. 5, 845–853 (2014)

    Google Scholar 

  82. Han, C.E., Arbib, M.A., Schweighofer, N.: Stroke rehabilitation reaches a threshold, PLoS. Comput. Biol. 4, article e1000133 (2008)

  83. Beer, R.D.: Computational and dynamical languages for autonomous agents. In: Port, R.F., van Gelder, T. (eds.) Mind as Motion, pp 121–147. MIT Press, Cambridge (1995)

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are thankful to Professor Claudia Carello and Professor Michael Turvey for various discussions supporting the present study. Vitor L.S. Profeta was supported by Grant 0991/12-0 of the Coordination of Improvement of Higher Education Personnel (CAPES).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to T. D. Frank.

Appendices

Appendix: A: Derivation of (17b) and (25b)

Recall that α r e l,c,2 is the critical control parameter value at which the transition from mode 1 to mode 2 occurs when α is gradually increased (ascending condition). From (16) it follows that an increase in α implies that the ratio λ 2/λ 1 increases. The critical condition for the ratio λ 2/λ 1 is defined by (8). Substituting (16) into (8) and replacing α r e l by α r e l,c,2, we obtain

$$ \frac{L_{0} + \beta \, \alpha_{rel,c,2}}{g} = L_{0} - \beta \, \alpha_{rel,c,2} \ . $$
(44)

Solving for α r e l,c,2 yields (17a). By analogy, the critical value α r e l,c,1 can be determined. Recall that α r e l,c,1 denotes the critical control parameter value at which the transition from mode 2 to mode 1 occurs when α is decreases gradually (descending condition). The decrease of α implies an increase of the ratio λ 1/λ 2, see (16) again. The critical condition for the ratio λ 1/λ 2 is given by (9) Substitute (16) into (9) and replacing α r e l by α r e l,c,2, we obtain

$$ \frac{L_{0} - \beta \, \alpha_{rel,c,1}}{g} = L_{0} + \beta \, \alpha_{rel,c,1} \ . $$
(45)

Solving for α r e l,c,1 yields (17b). Note that (17a) and (17b) exemplify the two control parameter principles given by (14) and (15) that hold for any dynamical system that exhibits hysteresis. In particular, using (17a) and (17b) we see explicitly that

$$ {\Delta} \alpha=\alpha_{c,2}-\alpha_{c,1}= \alpha_{rel,c,2}-\alpha_{rel,c,1}=2\alpha_{rel,c,2}=-2\alpha_{rel,c,1} $$
(46)

holds, which is (15). Consequently, we have α r e l,c,2α/2 and α r e l,c,1=−Δα/2, as stated see (17b) again.

The critical control parameter values in the case of negative hysteresis can be determined by a similar argumentation. To this end, however, we need to assume that a time scale separation holds for the variables involved in the order parameter and system parameter model. The characteristic time scales are summarized in Table 5.

Table 5 Key variables used in the model sketched in Fig. 1 and their characteristic time scales

First of all, it is assumed that order parameters ξ k evolve quickly every time a perceptual process or behavioral response is required. The L k (n) system parameters evolve slowly relative to ξ k (t). However, they evolve quickly on the time scale of the repetitively performed judgment tasks or executed behavioral responses. That is, they are considered as fast evolving variables relative to changes of the control parameter α. Consequently, at the bifurcation point as defined by the critical control parameter values α c,1 and α c,2 the system parameters L k are in good approximation at their respective stationary values.

Let us determined the critical control parameter value α r e l,c,2 for transitions from mode 1 to mode 2 when α is increased (ascending condition) provided that mode “1” has been “on” or active in several previous behavioral responses. Increasing α implies that the ratio λ 2/λ 1 increases. In (16) we replace α r e l by α r e l,c,2 such that λ k =L k β α r e l,c,2 For L k we use the corresponding stationary values s k : L 1=s 1 and L 2=s 2. That is, as mentioned above, we assume that at the transition point the system parameter dynamics is close to its fixed point. From (19) we obtain L 1=s 1=L 0h and L 2=s 2=L 0. Substituting these results into (8), we obtain

$$ \frac{L_{0} + \beta \, \alpha_{rel,c,2}}{g} = L_{0} -h \, \beta \, \alpha_{rel,c,2} \ . $$
(47)

Solving for α r e l,c,2 yields (25a). By analogy, (25b) can be derived.

Appendix : B: More than two alternatives for the special case of a set of almost indistinguishable stimuli

In the conclusions (Section 4), a generalized order parameter model involving more than two alternatives has been discussed and a heuristic argument has been developed that suggests that increasing the number of alternatives or choices will increase the response times predicted by that generalized model. This argument can be worked out in detail for a decision-making process in the special case when all order parameters evolve in exactly the same way for most of the time of the process. That is, we consider M alternatives or choices, where each alternative is described by an order parameter ξ k . Based on certain stimuli decisions have to be made (e.g., in an experiment on associative memory involving stimulus-response pairs the learned associations are tested by presenting the stimuli in an random order). The stimuli are assumed to be almost indistinguishable such that all ξ k assume that same values for most of the decision-making processes: ξ k (t)=z(t) for k=1,…,M. Note that in this context we also assume that λ k =λ holds for k=1,…,M. That is, all growth parameters are identical. Eventually, the order parameters ξ k will diverge (i.e., ξ k ξ j for all kj) due to the impact of fluctuations and only one order parameter will ”survive” (winner-takes-all dynamics, see Section 2.1). In the following calculations, we do not account for this final phase and assume that the final phase does not qualitatively affect the relationship between the number of choices M and the response times.

For ξ k (t)=z(t)>0 we need to take the interaction terms \(g \xi _{k} {\xi _{j}^{2}}\) into consideration. That is, (34) has to be supplemented with M−1 interaction terms describing the inhibitory impacts of the other patterns or choices on the pattern (choice) of consideration. Since we have ξ k (t)=z(t) for all k it follows that all interaction terms have the same form and are equal to g z 3(t). Adding M−1 terms g z 3(t) to the right-hand side of (34) and using ξ k rather than z as variable (for sake of consistency with our presentation in Section 3), we obtain

$$ \frac{\mathrm{d}}{\mathrm{d}\tau} \xi_{k} = \lambda \, \xi_{k} - g(M-1){\xi^{3}_{k}}- {\xi^{3}_{k}} = \lambda \, \xi_{k} - G {\xi^{3}_{k}} $$
(48)

with

$$ G=g(M-1)+1 >1\ . $$
(49)

Note that as long as ξ k (t)=z(t) for all k holds, the trajectories converge to an unstable saddle point with \(\xi _{k}=\sqrt {\lambda /G} \ \forall \ k\). Therefore, we assume that the trajectories ξ k converge from ξ k =ξ 0 at t=0 towards a value ξ k =ξ T at time T with ξ T >ξ 0 and \(\xi _{T}<\sqrt {\lambda /G}\). After that point the trajectories start to diverge and one of the trajectories will approach the finite value \(\xi _{k}=\sqrt {\lambda }>\sqrt {\lambda /G}\), while all others will converge to zero. Consequently, the response time RT exceeds the time point T: R T>T. The time T can be calculated just as in Section 2.1. We obtain

$$ T = \frac{1}{2\lambda} \left\{ \ln\left(\frac{{\xi^{2}_{T}}}{{\xi_{0}^{2}}}\right) + \ln\left( \frac{\lambda-G{\xi_{0}^{2}}}{\lambda-G{\xi_{T}^{2}}}\right)\right\} = \frac{1}{\lambda} \left\{ \ln\left(\frac{\xi_{T}}{\xi_{0}}\right) - \frac{1}{2} \ln\left( \frac{\lambda-G{\xi_{T}^{2}}}{\lambda-G{\xi_{0}^{2}}}\right)\right\} \ . $$
(50)

Note that for G=1 and \(\xi _{0}=D\sqrt {\lambda }\), \(\xi _{T}=\theta \sqrt {\lambda }\) we re-obtain (35). Importantly, the auxiliary parameter G increases with the number of choices M, see (49). Therefore, the question arises how does the time T depend on the factor G. Differentiating T with respect to G yields

$$ \frac{\mathrm{d}T}{\mathrm{d}G}=\frac{1}{2W} \left({\xi_{T}^{2}}-{\xi_{0}^{2}}\right)>0 $$
(51)

with \(W=(\lambda -G{\xi _{0}^{2}})(\lambda -G{\xi _{T}^{2}})>0\). We see that the duration T contributing to the response time RT increases when G increases. This implies that T increases when the number M of choices increases. As mentioned above, this calculation is a crude estimate for the response time RT. The response time includes a final phase in which ξ j ξ k holds. In fact, the assumption ξ k (t)=z(t) for t<T and ξ j ξ k for t>T should be considered as a simplified two-phases scenario that is used to derive by analytical methods an estimate for the lower bound T of the response time RT. Nevertheless, this estimate suggests that the response time RT increases as a function of M. More detailed calculations (involving simulation studies) are needed to obtain an advanced understanding of this issue but are left to the future.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Frank, T.D., Profeta, V.L.S. & Harrison, H.S. Interplay between order-parameter and system parameter dynamics: considerations on perceptual-cognitive-behavioral mode-mode transitions exhibiting positive and negative hysteresis and on response times. J Biol Phys 41, 257–292 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10867-015-9378-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10867-015-9378-z

Keywords

Navigation