Skip to main content
Log in

Concurrent and Prospective Associations Between Emotion Reactivity and Depressive Symptoms in Middle Childhood

  • Published:
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

According to the emotion context-insensitivity hypothesis (ECI: Rottenberg et al. 2005), depression is associated with emotion reactivity such that depressed individuals are less reactive to stressors. Although little is known about the relation between children’s emotion reactivity and the development of depressive symptoms, finding evidence for the ECI before the onset of major depression would suggest that emotion under-reactivity is a risk factor for depression. In this study we address outstanding questions about depression and reactivity by assessing reciprocal associations between emotion reactivity (operationalized as cardiac reactivity to stress) and depressive symptoms in middle childhood, while controlling for rumination. Forty-three children (53 % girls) between the ages of 8 and 12 participated in this longitudinal study spanning a 2-year period. At Wave 1, children completed questionnaires to assess depressive symptoms and rumination. In addition, they completed an emotional stressor task while cardiac reactivity was measured. At Wave 2, children completed a depression questionnaire as well as a performance challenge task while cardiac reactivity was measured. Results showed that cardiac under-reactivity to the emotional stressor at Wave 1 was associated concurrently and prospectively with greater depressive symptom severity. However, Wave 2 depressive symptoms were not significantly associated with Wave 2 reactivity to the performance stressor and Wave 1 depressive symptoms were marginally associated with Wave 2 reactivity. Additional research on emotion reactivity and depressive symptoms in middle childhood is needed; preliminarily, our results suggest that children who exhibit under-reactivity to sadness may benefit from interventions to reduce or prevent the development of clinically-significant depressive symptoms during middle childhood.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In this review, stimuli were classified as either positively- or negatively-valenced. Negatively-valenced stimuli included a variety of stressors, such as stress tasks, sad imagery, audio-recorded negative social interactions, and unpleasant pictures. Analyses were not conducted by type of stressor.

  2. Given that there are no “best practices” for assessing emotion reactivity (Nelson et al. 2011), we repeated all analyses using two additional measurement approaches, where change in cardiac reactivity was measured via (1) simple difference scores and (2) percent change scores. Notably, our pattern of findings was consistent across each analytical approach, underscoring the robustness of our findings. For the ease of interpretation, only the set of findings using standardized residualized coefficients (preferred by some methodologists; see Nelson et al. 2011 for further exploration of these issues) are presented here.

  3. Age was not related to depressive symptoms or to rumination at Wave 1. Children’s levels of rumination at Wave 1 were significantly different by gender, with boys reporting higher levels of rumination. Other characteristics are potentially related to physiological functioning, including fitness and anxiety. To account for individual differences in physical fitness, we repeated our analyses controlling for whether children exercised earlier in the day prior to their lab visit, and their height and weight. Controlling for these variables did not alter our findings. As suggested by an anonymous reviewer, given the high level of comorbidity between anxiety and depression, we also conducted a set of analyses controlling for anxiety at Wave 1. Our pattern of findings (not presented here) remained unchanged, although the p-value increased slightly, likely due to reduced degrees of freedom.

  4. Composite variables are treated as continuous variables in all analyses.

  5. Given our small sample size, we did not statistically test whether there were differences in patterns of association between variables among boys as compared to girls.

References

  • Abela, J. R. Z., Aydin, C., & Auerbach, R. P. (2007). Responses to depression in children: reconceptualizing the relation among response styles. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 913–927.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abela, J. R. Z., & Hankin, B. L. (2011). Rumination as a vulnerability factor to depression during the transition to adolescence: a multiwave longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 259–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, N. B., Kuppens, P., & Sheeber, L. B. (2012). Heart rate responses to parental behavior in depressed adolescents. Biological Psychology, 90, 80–87.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beesdo, K., Lau, J. Y., Guyer, A. E., McClure-Tone, E. B., Monk, C. S., Nelson, E. E., & Pine, D. S. (2009). Common and distinct amygdala-function perturbations in depressed vs anxious adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(3), 275–285.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berntson, G. G., Bigger, J. T., Eckberg, D. L., Grossman, P., Kaufmann, P. G., Malik, M., et al. (1997). Heart rate variability: origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology, 34, 623–648.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borelli, J. L. (2009). The distress vignettes paradigm: A method for probing children’s responses. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Borelli, J. L., Hilt, L.M., West, J., Gonzalez, M., & Weekes, N. (2014). School-aged children’s depressive rumination is associated with their reactivity to sadness but not fear. Journal of Clinical Adolescent and Child Psychology.

  • Borelli, J. L., Sbarra, D. A., Crowley, M. J., & Mayes, L. C. (2011). Mood symptoms and emotional responsiveness to threat in school-aged children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bylsma, L. M., Morris, B. H., & Rottenberg, J. (2008). A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 676–691.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carels, R. A., Bluemthal, J. A., & Sherwood, A. (1999). Emotional responsivity during daily life: relationship to psychosocial functioning and ambulatory blood pressure. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 36, 25–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 597–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1998). The development of depression in children and adolescents. The American Psychologist, 53(2), 221–241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. C., Fawcett, J., Salazar-Grueso, E., & Fawcett, E. (1984). Seven-month clinical outcome of anhedonic and normally hedonic depressed inpatients. American Journal of Psychology, 141, 1216–1220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, P. M., Luby, J., & Sullivan, M. W. (2008). Emotions and the development of childhood depression: bridging the gap. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 141–148.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J., & Jaser, S. S. (2004). Temperament, stress reactivity, and coping: Implications for depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33(1), 21–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craighead, W. E., Smucker, M. R., Craighead, L. W., & Ilardi, S. S. (1998). Factor analysis of the Children’s depression inventory in a community sample. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 156–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Catania, J. J. (1977). Autonomic correlates of depression and clinical improvement following electroconvulsive shock therapy. Psychophysiology, 14, 569–578.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ehring, T., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Schnulle, J., Fischer, S., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation and vulnerability to depression: spontaneous versus instructed use of emotion suppression and reappraisal. Emotion, 10(4), 563–572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Figueras, M. A., Amador-Campos, J. A., Gomez-Benito, J., & del Barrio Gandara, V. (2010). Psychometric properties of the Children’s depression inventory in community and clinical sample. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 13, 990–999.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, B. (2007). An autonomic flexibility–neurovisceral integration model of anxiety and cardiac vagal tone. Biological Psychology, 74, 185–199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentzler, A. L., Santucci, A. K., Kovacs, M., & Fox, N. A. (2009). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity predicts emotion regulation and depressive symptoms in at-risk and control children. Biological Psychology, 82, 156–163.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentzler, A. L., Wheat, A. L., Palmer, C. A., & Burwell, R. A. (2013). Children’s responses to cognitive challenge and links to self-reported rumination. Cognition and Emotion, 27(2), 305–317.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, K. E., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Gruber, J. (2013). Positive emotion dysregulation across mood disorders: how amplifying versus dampening predicts emotional reactivity and illness course. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51, 736–741.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grillon, C., Warner, V., Hille, J., Merikangas, K. R., Bruder, G. E., Tenke, C. E., & Weissman, M. M. (2005). Families at high and low risk for depression: a three-generation startle study. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 953–960.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, J., Harvey, A. G., & Johnson, S. L. (2009). Reflective and ruminative processing of positive emotional memories in bipolar disorder and healthy controls. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(8), 697–704. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.05.005.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hankin, B. L., & Abramson, L. Y. (2001). Development of gender differences in depression: an elaborated cognitive vulnerability-transactional stress theory. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 773–796.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., Mcgee, R., & Angell, K. E. (1998). Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 10, 128–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helsel, W. J., & Matson, J. L. (1984). The assessment of depression in children: the internal structure of the child depression inventory (CDI). Behavioral Research and Therapy, 22, 289–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hilt, L. M., Armstrong, J. M., & Essex, M. J. (2012). Early family context and development of adolescent ruminative style: moderation by temperament. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 916–926.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hilt, L. M., McLaughlin, K. A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). Examination of the response styles theory in a community sample of young adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 545–556.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • IBM Corp. (2013). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 22.0. Armonk: IBM Corp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, M. (1992). Children's Depression Inventory. New Ybrk: Multi-Health Systems.

  • Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous systemactivity inemotion: a review. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 394e421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: a review. Cognition and emotion, 23(2), 209–237.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCullough, M. E., Orsulak, P., Brandon, A., & Akers, L. (2007). Rumination, fear, and cortisol: an in vivo study of interpersonal transgressions. Health Psychology, 26(1), 126–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Merikangas, K. R., He, J., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., & Swendsen, J. (2010). Lifetime Prevalence of Mental Disorders in US Adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Study-Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(10), 980–989. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017.

  • Nelson, B. D., Shankman, S. A., Olino, T. M., & Klein, D. N. (2011). Defining reactivity: how methodological decisions can affect conclusions about emotional reactivity in psychopathology. Cognition & Emotion, 25(8), 1439–1459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 504–511.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1994). The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 424–443.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and post traumatic stress symptoms following a natural disaster: the 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400–424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ottaviani, C., Shapiro, D., & Fitzgerald, L. (2011). Rumination in the laboratory: what happens when you go back to everyday life? Psychophysiology, 48(4), 453–461. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01122.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Porges, S. W. (2001). The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42(2), 123–146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Romano, B. A., & Nelson, R. O. (1988). Discriminant and concurrent validity of measures of children’s depression. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 17, 255–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rood, L., Roelofs, J., Bogels, S. M., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schouten, E. (2009). The influence of emotion-focused rumination and distraction on clinical youth: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(7), 607–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rottenberg, J., Gross, J. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2005). Emotion context insensitivity in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 627–639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rottenberg, J., Kasch, K. L., Gross, J. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2002). Sadness and amusement reactivity differentially predict concurrent and prospective functioning in major depressive disorder. Emotion, 2(2), 135–146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., Zuccolotto, A. (2007). E-Prime: User’s guide, version 2.0. Psychology software tools.

  • Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Carter, C. S., Ramel, W., & Thase, M. E. (2003). Do the seconds turn into hours? Relationships between sustained pupil dilation in response to emotional information and self-reported rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 365–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. S., Dahl, R. E., Ryan, N. D., Forbes, E. E., Axelson, D. A., Birmaher, B., & Siegle, G. J. (2007). Pupillary reactivity to emotional information in child and adolescent depression: links to clinical and ecological measures. American Journal of Pyschiatry, 164(12), 1873–1880.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smiley, P., & Borelli, J. L. (2011). The impossible puzzle task: A performance challenge paradigm for school-aged children. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Smiley, P., Harder, K., Chung, S., Coffey, J., Dubon, V., Borelli, J. (2014). You’re sad? I’m angry! Children’s perceptions of parent conditional positive regard, anger expression, and task engagement. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Spasojevic, J., & Alloy, L. B. (2001). Rumination as a common mechanism relative depressive risk factors to depression. Emotion, 1, 25–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Psychopathology as an outcome of development. Development and Psychopathology, 9(2), 251–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroud, L. R., Tanofsky-Kraff, M., Wilfley, D. E., & Salovey, P. (2000). The Yale Interpersonal Stressor (YIPS): affective, physiological, and behavioral responses to a novel interpersonal rejection paradigm. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 22(3), 204–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sun, S., & Wang, S. (2014). The children’s depression inventory in worldwide child development research: A reliability generalization study. Journal of Child and Family Studies. Advance online publication.

  • Taylor, C. B. (2010). Depression, heart rate related variables and cardiovascular disease. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 78, 80–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. J., Grant, K. E., Amrhein, K., Carter, J. C., Farahmand, F., Harrison, A., Thomas, K. J., Carleton, R. A., Lugo-Hernandez, E., & Katz, B. N. (2014). The manifestation of depression in the context of urban poverty: a factor analysis of the children’s depression inventory in low-income urban youth. Psychological Assessment, 26(4), 1317–1332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, K. M., Drevets, W. C., Dahl, R. E., Ryan, N. D., Birmaher, B., Eccard, C. H., et al. (2001). Amygdala response to fearful faces in anxious and depressed children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 1057–1063.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tram, J. M., & Cole, D. A. (2006). A multimethod examination of the stability of depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 674–686.

  • Twenge, J. M., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2002). Age, gender, race, socioeconomic status, and birth cohort differences on the Children’s Depression Inventory: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 578–588.

  • Watkins, E. R. (2009). Depressive rumination and co-morbidity: evidence for brooding as a transdiagnostic process. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 27, 160–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, B., Weisz, J. R., Politano, M., Carey, M., Nelson, W. M., & Finch, A. J. (1991). Developmental differences in the factor structure of the children’s depression inventory. Psychological Assessment, 3, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weissman, M. M., Wolk, S., Goldstein, R. B., Moreau, D., Adams, P., Greenwald, S., Klier, C. M., Ryan, N. D., Dahl, R. E., & Wickramaratne, P. (1999). Depressed adolescents grown up. JAMA, 281(18), 1707–1713.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Young, E. A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2001). Effects of ruminations on the saliva cortisol response to a social stressor. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26(3), 319–329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zoccola, P. M., & Dickerson, S. S. (2012). Assessing the relationship between rumination and cortisol: a review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 73(1), 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.03.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zoccola, P. M., Quas, J. A., & Yim, I. S. (2010). Salivary cortisol responses to a psychosocial laboratory stressor and later verbal recall of the stressor: the role of trait and state rumination. Stress The International Journal on the Biology of Stress, 13(5), 435–443.

Download references

Conflict of Interest

Jennifer A. Somers, Jessica L. Borelli, Patricia A. Smiley, Jessica L. West, and Lori M. Hilt declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Experiment Participants

The protocol for this study was approved by the Institutional Review Board prior to the inception of data collection. Assent was obtained from all child participants and their primary caregivers provided informed consent.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jennifer A. Somers.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Somers, J.A., Borelli, J.L., Smiley, P.A. et al. Concurrent and Prospective Associations Between Emotion Reactivity and Depressive Symptoms in Middle Childhood. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 37, 692–704 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-015-9491-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-015-9491-0

Keywords

Navigation