Abouheif, E. 1997. Developmental genetics and homology: A hierarchical approach. Tree 12: 405–408.
Google Scholar
Allen, C., and M. Bekoff. 2007. Animal consciousness. In The Blackwell companion to consciousness, eds. M. Velmans, and S. Schneider, 58–71. Oxford: Blackwell.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Baars, B. J., and D. B. Edelman. 2012. Consciousness, biology and quantum hypotheses. Physics of Life Reviews 9 (3): 285–294.
Article
Google Scholar
Barandiaran, X. E., E. Di Paolo, and M. Rohde. 2009. Defining agency: Individuality, normativity, asymmetry, and spatio-temporality in action. Adaptive Behavior 17 (5): 367–386.
Article
Google Scholar
Barham, J. 2012. Normativity, agency, and life. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 43 (1): 92–103.
Article
Google Scholar
Bayne, T. 2013. Agency as a marker of consciousness. In Decomposing the will, eds. A. Clark, J. Kiverstein, T. Vierkant, and T. Vierkant, 160–181. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Beshkar, M. 2008. Animal consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 15 (3): 5–33.
Google Scholar
Boudry, M., M. Vlerick, and R. McKay. 2015. Can evolution get us off the hook? Evaluating the ecological defence of human rationality. Consciousness and Cognition 33: 524–535.
Article
Google Scholar
Bowmaker, J. K. 2008. Evolution of vertebrate visual pigments. Vision Research 48 (20): 2022–2041.
Article
Google Scholar
Braithwaite, V. 2010. Do fish feel pain? Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Brown, C. 2015. Fish intelligence, sentience and ethics. Animal Cognition 18 (1): 1–17.
Article
Google Scholar
Brown, C. 2016. Fish pain: An inconvenient truth. Animal Sentience 3: 32.
Google Scholar
Brown, R., H. Lau, and J. E. LeDoux. 2019. Understanding the higher-order approach to consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 23 (9): 754–768.
Article
Google Scholar
Butler, A. B., and R. M. Cotterill. 2006. Mammalian and avian neuroanatomy and the question of consciousness in birds. The Biological Bulletin 211 (2): 106–127.
Article
Google Scholar
Butler, A. B., P. R. Manger, B. I. Lindahl, and P. Århem. 2005. Evolution of the neural basis of consciousness: A bird–mammal comparison. BioEssays 27 (9): 923–936.
Article
Google Scholar
Calcott, B. 2009. Lineage explanations: Explaining how biological mechanisms change. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 60 (1): 51–78.
Article
Google Scholar
Chalmers, D. J. 1996. The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Clark, A. 2001. Visual experience and motor action: Are the bonds too tight? Philosophical Review 110 (4): 495–519.
Article
Google Scholar
Clark, A. 2007. What reaching teaches: Consciousness, control, and the inner zombie. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 58 (3): 563–594.
Article
Google Scholar
Clark, A. 2009. Perception, action, and experience: Unraveling the golden braid. Neuropsychologia 47 (6): 1460–1468.
Article
Google Scholar
Collins, C. 1997. Searle on consciousness and dualism. International Journal of Philosophical Studies 5 (1): 15–33.
Article
Google Scholar
Cook, R. G., M. A. J. Qadri, and A. M. Keller. 2015. The analysis of visual cognition in birds: Implications for evolution, mechanism, and representation. Psychology of Learning and Motivation 63: 1–38.
Article
Google Scholar
Corcoran, K. 2001. The trouble with Searle’s biological naturalism. Erkenntnis 55 (3): 307–324.
Article
Google Scholar
Cowey, A., and P. Stoerig. 1995. Blindsight in monkeys. Nature 373 (6511): 247–249.
Article
Google Scholar
Cross, F. R., N. E. Buchler, and J. M. Skotheim. 2011. Evolution of networks and sequences in eukaryotic cell cycle control. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 366 (1584): 3532–3544.
Article
Google Scholar
Currie, A. M. 2014. Venomous dinosaurs and rear-fanged snakes: homology and homoplasy characterized. Erkenntnis 79 (3): 701–727.
Article
Google Scholar
Dennett, D. 1991. Consciousness explained. Boston: Little, Brown, and Co.
Google Scholar
Dretske, F. 2006. Perception without awareness. In Perceptual experience, eds. T. S. E. Gendler, and J. E. Hawthorne, 147–189. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Edelman, D. B. 2016. Leaving the door open for fish pain: Evolutionary convergence and the utility of “just-so stories”. Animal Sentience, 2016.062.
Ereshefsky, M. 2007. Psychological categories as homologies: Lessons from ethology. Biology and Philosophy 22 (5): 659–674.
Article
Google Scholar
Ereshefsky, M. 2009. Homology: Integrating phylogeny and development. Biological Theory 4 (3): 225–229.
Article
Google Scholar
Ereshefsky, M. 2012. Homology thinking. Biology and Philosophy 27 (3): 381–400.
Article
Google Scholar
Feinberg, T. E., and J. Mallatt. 2013. The evolutionary and genetic origins of consciousness in the Cambrian period over 500 million years ago. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 667.
Article
Google Scholar
Feinberg, T. E., and J. M. Mallatt. 2016. The ancient origins of consciousness: How the brain created experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Feinberg, T. E., and J. M. Mallatt. 2018. Consciousness demystified. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Félix, M. A. 2005. An inversion in the wiring of an intercellular signal: Evolution of Wnt signaling in the nematode vulva. BioEssays: News and Reviews in Molecular, Cellular and Developmental Biology 27 (8): 765–769.
Article
Google Scholar
Flanagan, O. J. 1992. Consciousness reconsidered. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Foley, R. T., R. L. Whitwell, and M. A. Goodale. 2015. The two-visual-systems hypothesis and the perspectival features of visual experience. Consciousness and Cognition 35: 225–233.
Article
Google Scholar
Fulda, F. C. 2017. Natural agency: The case of bacterial cognition. Journal of the American Philosophical Association 3 (1): 69–90.
Article
Google Scholar
Gerkema, M. P., W. I. Davies, R. G. Foster, M. Menaker, and R. A. Hut. 2013. The nocturnal bottleneck and the evolution of activity patterns in mammals. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 280(1765), 20130508.
Ginsburg, S., and E. Jablonka. 2019. The evolution of the sensitive soul: Learning and the origins of consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Glock, H. J. 2009. Can animals act for reasons? Inquiry : a journal of medical care organization, provision and financing 52 (3): 232–254.
Article
Google Scholar
Godfrey-Smith, P. 2016a. Pain in parallel. Animal Sentience, 3 (21).
Godfrey-Smith, P. 2016b. Other minds: The octopus, the sea, and the deep origins of consciousness. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Google Scholar
Goodale, M., and D. Milner. 2005. Sight unseen: An exploration of conscious and unconscious vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Griffin, D. R., and G. B. Speck. 2004. New evidence of animal consciousness. Animal Cognition 7 (1): 5–18.
Article
Google Scholar
Haag, E. S., and J. R. True. 2018. Developmental system drift. In Evolutionary developmental biology: A reference guide, eds. L. Nuño, G. B. de la Rosa, and Müller, 1–12. Cham: Springer.
Hall, B. K. 2003. Descent with modification: The unity underlying homology and homoplasy as seen through an analysis of development and evolution. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 78 (3): 409–433.
Article
Google Scholar
Hall, B. K. 2012. Parallelism, deep homology, and evo-devo. Evolution & Development 14 (1): 29–33.
Article
Google Scholar
Hall, B. K. 2013. Homology, homoplasy, novelty, and behavior. Developmental Psychobiology 55 (1): 4–12.
Article
Google Scholar
Heesy, C. P., and M. I. Hall. 2010. The nocturnal bottleneck and the evolution of mammalian vision. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 75 (3): 195–203.
Article
Google Scholar
Hodos, W., K. A. Macko, and B. B. Bessette. 1984. Near-field acuity changes after visual system lesions in pigeons. II. Telencephalon. Behavioural Brain Research 13 (1): 15–30.
Article
Google Scholar
Hunt, D. M., L. S. Carvalho, J. A. Cowing, and W. L. Davies. 2009. Evolution and spectral tuning of visual pigments in birds and mammals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 364 (1531): 2941–2955.
Article
Google Scholar
Hacker, P. M. S. 2007. Human nature: The categorical framework. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Book
Google Scholar
Hurley, S. 2003. Animal action in the space of reasons. Mind and Language 18 (3): 231–257.
Article
Google Scholar
Jack, A. I., and T. Shallice. 2001. Introspective physicalism as an approach to the science of consciousness. Cognition 79 (1–2): 161–196.
Article
Google Scholar
James, W. 1890/1981. The principles of psychology. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Key, B. 2015. Fish do not feel pain and its implications for understanding phenomenal consciousness. Biology and Philosophy 30 (2): 149–165.
Article
Google Scholar
Key, B. 2016. Why fish do not feel pain. Animal Sentience 3: 1.
Google Scholar
Kim, J. 1995. Mental causation in Searle’s “Biological Naturalism”. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 55 (1): 189–194.
Article
Google Scholar
Knudsen, E. I. 2020. Evolution of neural processing for visual perception in vertebrates. Journal of Comparative Neurology 528 (17): 2888–2901.
Article
Google Scholar
Kozuch, B. 2015. Dislocation, not dissociation: The neuroanatomical argument against visual experience driving motor action. Mind and Language 30 (5): 572–602.
Article
Google Scholar
Lau, H., and D. Rosenthal. 2011. Empirical support for higher-order theories of conscious awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 (8): 365–373.
Article
Google Scholar
Lin, C. P., B. N. Danforth, and T. K. Wood. 2004. Molecular phylogenetics and evolution of maternal care in Membracine treehoppers. Systematic Biology 53 (3): 400–421.
Article
Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. Z. 1958. The evolution of behavior. Scientific American 199 (6): 67–78.
Article
Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. Z. 1974. Analogy as a source of knowledge. Science 185 (4147): 229–234.
Article
Google Scholar
Manzotti, R. 2016. No evidence that pain is painful neural process. Animal Sentience 3: 11.
Google Scholar
Maor, R., T. Dayan, H. Ferguson-Gow, and K. E. Jones. 2017. Temporal niche expansion in mammals from a nocturnal ancestor after dinosaur extinction. Nature Ecology and Evolution 1 (12): 1889–1895.
Article
Google Scholar
Marcel, A. 1988. “Phenomenal experience and functionalism”. In Consciousness in modern science, eds. A. Marcel, and E. Bisiach, 121–158. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Marcel, A. J. 1986. Consciousness and processing: Choosing and testing a null hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1): 40–41.
Article
Google Scholar
Marshall, J. C., and P. W. Halligan. 1988. Blindsight and insight in visuo-spatial neglect. Nature 336 (6201): 766–767.
Article
Google Scholar
Matthen, M. 2007. Defining vision: What homology thinking contributes. Biology and Philosophy 22 (5): 675–689.
Article
Google Scholar
Matthen, M. 2015. The individuation of the senses. In The Oxford handbook of philosophy of perception, ed. M. Matthen, 567–585. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
McConwell and Currie. 2017. Gouldian arguments and the sources of contingency. Biology and Philosophy 32: 243–261.
Article
Google Scholar
McDowell, J. 1994. Mind and world. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
Mestre, D. R., M. Brouchon, M. Ceccaldi, and M. Poncet. 1992. Perception of optical flow in cortical blindness: A case report. Neuropsychologia 30 (9): 783–795.
Article
Google Scholar
Mikhalevich, I., and R. Powell. 2020. Minds without spines: Evolutionarily inclusive animal ethics. Animal Sentience, 29 (1).
Milner, A. D. 2012. Is visual processing in the dorsal stream accessible to consciousness? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 279(1737), 2289–2298.
Milner, A. D., and M. A. Goodale. 2008. Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 46 (3): 774–785.
Article
Google Scholar
Müller, G. B. 2003. Homology: The evolution of morphological organization. In Origination of organismal form: Beyond the gene in developmental and evolutionary biology, eds. G. B. Müller, and S. A. Newman, 51–69. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Müller, G. B. 2010. Epigenetic innovation. In Evolution: The extended synthesis, eds. M. Pigliucci, and G. B. Müller, 307–332. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Naccache, L. 2006. Is she conscious? Science 313 (5792): 1395–1396.
Article
Google Scholar
Nelkin, N. 1995. The dissociation of phenomenal states from apperception. In Conscious experience, ed. T. Metzinger, 373–383. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.
Google Scholar
Newcomb, J. M., A. Sakurai, J. L. Lillvis, C. A. Gunaratne, and P. S. Katz. 2012. Homology and homoplasy of swimming behaviors and neural circuits in the Nudipleura (Mollusca, Gastropoda, Opisthobranchia). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109 (Supplement 1): 10669–10676.
Article
Google Scholar
Nguyen, A. P., M. L. Spetch, N. A. Crowder, I. R. Winship, P. L. Hurd, and D. R. Wylie. 2004. A dissociation of motion and spatial-pattern vision in the avian telencephalon: Implications for the evolution of “visual streams”. Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 24 (21): 4962–4970.
Article
Google Scholar
Parker, D. M., and J. D. Delius. 1980. The effects of wulst lesions on simple visual discrimination performance in the pigeon. Behavioural Processes 5 (2): 151–159.
Article
Google Scholar
Price, J. J., K. P. Johnson, and D. H. Clayton. 2004. The evolution of echolocation in swiftlets. Journal of Avian Biology 35 (2): 135–143.
Article
Google Scholar
Polger, T. 2017. Rethinking the evolution of consciousness. In The Blackwell companion to consciousness, eds. S. Schneider, and M. Velmans, 77–92 (second edn.). Oxford: Blackwell.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Powell, R. 2020. Contingency and convergence: Towards a cosmic biology of body and mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Powell, R., and N. Shea. 2014. Homology across inheritance systems. Biology & Philosophy 29 (6): 781–806.
Article
Google Scholar
Ramsey, G., and A. S. Peterson. 2012. Sameness in biology. Philosophy of Science 79 (2): 255–275.
Article
Google Scholar
Rendall, D., and A. Di Fiore. 2007. Homoplasy, homology, and the perceived special status of behavior in evolution. Journal of Human Evolution 52 (5): 504–521.
Article
Google Scholar
Robinson, W. 2007. Evolution and epiphenomenalism. Journal of Consciousness Studies 14 (11): 27–42.
Google Scholar
Robinson, Z., C. J. Maley, and G. Piccinini. 2015. Is consciousness a spandrel? Journal of the American Philosophical Association 1 (2): 365–383.
Article
Google Scholar
Rose, J. D. 2007. Anthropomorphism and “mental welfare”of fishes. Diseases of Aquatic Organisms 75 (2): 139–154.
Article
Google Scholar
Rose, J. D., R. Arlinghaus, S. J. Cooke, B. K. Diggles, W. Sawynok, E. D. Stevens, and C. D. L. Wynne. 2014. Can fish really feel pain? Fish and Fisheries 15 (1): 97–133.
Article
Google Scholar
Rosenthal, D. M. 1986. Two concepts of consciousness. Philosophical Studies 49 (3): 329–359.
Article
Google Scholar
Rosenthal, D. M. 2002. Explaining consciousness. In Philosophy of mind: Classical and contemporary readings, ed. D. J. Chalmers, 406–421. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Rosenthal, D. M. 2008. Consciousness and its function. Neuropsychologia 46 (3): 829–840.
Article
Google Scholar
Roth, V. L. 1984. On homology. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 22: 13–29.
Article
Google Scholar
Roth, V. L. 1991. Homology and hierarchies: Problems solved and unresolved. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 4: 167–194.
Article
Google Scholar
Ryle, G. 1949. The concept of mind. New York: Barnes and Noble Books.
Google Scholar
Sakurai, A., and P. S. Katz. 2017. Artificial synaptic rewiring demonstrates that distinct neural circuit configurations underlie homologous behaviors. Current Biology 27 (12): 1721–1734.e3.
Article
Google Scholar
Sakurai, A., J. M. Newcomb, J. L. Lillvis, and P. S. Katz. 2011. Different roles for homologous interneurons in species exhibiting similar rhythmic behaviors. Current Biology 21 (12): 1036–1043.
Article
Google Scholar
Schmitz, T. W., and S. C. Johnson. 2007. Relevance to self: A brief review and framework of neural systems underlying appraisal. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (4): 585–596.
Article
Google Scholar
Schütt, C., and R. Nöthiger. 2000. Structure, function and evolution of sex-determining systems in Dipteran insects. Development 127 (4): 667–677.
Article
Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. 1992. The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Seth, A. K. 2016. Why fish pain cannot and should not be ruled out. Animal Sentience, 3 (14).
Shimizu, T., and A. N. Bowers. 1999. Visual circuits of the avian telencephalon: Evolutionary implications. Behavioural Brain Research 98 (2): 183–191.
Article
Google Scholar
Shimizu, T., and S. Watanabe. 2012. The avian visual system: Overview. In How animals see the world: comparative behavior, biology, and evolution of vision, eds. O. F. Lazareva, T. Shimizu, and E. A. Wasserman, 473–482. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Shimizu, T., T. B. Patton, and S. A. Husband. 2010. Avian visual behavior and the organization of the telencephalon. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 75 (3): 204–217.
Article
Google Scholar
Shubin, N., C. Tabin, and S. Carroll. 1997. Fossils, genes and the evolution of animal limbs. Nature 388: 639–648.
Article
Google Scholar
Shubin, N., C. Tabin, and S. Carroll. 2009. Deep homology and the origins of evolutionary novelty. Nature 457: 818–823.
Article
Google Scholar
Sneddon, L. U. 2003. The evidence for pain in fish: The use of morphine as an analgesic. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 83 (2): 153–162.
Article
Google Scholar
Sneddon, L. 2011. Pain perception in fish. Journal of Consciousness Studies 18 (9–10): 209–229.
Google Scholar
Sneddon, L. U., R. W. Elwood, S. A. Adamo, and M. C. Leach. 2014. Defining and assessing animal pain. Animal Behaviour 97: 201–212.
Article
Google Scholar
Sommer, R. J. 2008. Homology and the hierarchy of biological systems. BioEssays 30 (7): 653–658.
Article
Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. 2005. The robot’s rebellion: Finding meaning in the age of Darwin. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., and R. F. West. 2000. Advancing the rationality debate. Behavioral and brain sciences 23 (5): 701–717.
Article
Google Scholar
Striedter, G. F. 2019. Variation across species and levels: Implications for model species research. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 93 (2–3): 57–69.
Article
Google Scholar
Suzuki, D. G., and S. Tanaka. 2017. A phenomenological and dynamic view of homology: Homologs as persistently reproducible modules. Biological Theory 12 (3): 169–180.
Article
Google Scholar
Thoreson, W. B., and D. M. Dacey. 2019. Diverse cell types, circuits, and mechanisms for color vision in the vertebrate retina. Physiological Reviews 99 (3): 1527–1573.
Article
Google Scholar
True, J. R., and E. S. Haag. 2001. Developmental system drift and flexibility in evolutionary trajectories. Evolution and Development 3 (2): 109–119.
Article
Google Scholar
Tye, M. 2017. Tense bees and shell-shocked crabs: Are animals conscious? New York: Oxford University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Van Gulick, R. 1989. What difference does consciousness make? Philosophical Topics 17 (1): 211–230.
Article
Google Scholar
Velmans, M. 2007. The co-evolution of matter and consciousness. Synthesis philosophica 22 (44): 273–282.
Google Scholar
Wagner, G. P. 1989. The biological homology concept. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 20: 51–69.
Article
Google Scholar
Wagner, G. P. 2014. Homology, genes, and evolutionary innovation. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Wakita, M., S. Watanabe, T. Shimizu, and L. R. Britto. 1992. Visual discrimination performance after lesions of the ventral lateral geniculate nucleus in pigeons (Columba livia). Behavioural Brain Research 51 (2): 211–215.
Article
Google Scholar
Walmsley, L. D. 2020. Lessons from a virtual slime: marginal mechanisms, minimal cognition and radical enactivism. Adaptive Behavior 28 (6): 453–464.
Article
Google Scholar
Weiskrantz, L., E. K. Warrington, M. D. Sanders, and J. Marshall. 1974. Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain 97 (4): 709–728.
Article
Google Scholar
Wylie, D. R., C. Gutierrez-Ibanez, J. M. Pakan, and A. N. Iwaniuk. 2009. The optic tectum of birds: Mapping our way to understanding visual processing. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 63 (4): 328–338.
Article
Google Scholar
Young, G. 2006. Preserving the role of conscious decision making in the initiation of intentional action. Journal of Consciousness Studies 13 (3): 51–68.
Google Scholar