Aboitiz, F. (2018). A brain for speech. Evolutionary continuity in primate and human auditory-vocal processing. Frontiers in Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00174.
Akhilesh, K., & Pappu, S. (2015). Bits and pieces: Lithic waste products and indicators of behaviour at Attirampakkham, India. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports, 4, 226–241.
Google Scholar
Alperson-Afil, N., Richter, D., & Goren-Inbar, N. (2017). Evaluating the intensity of fire at the Acheulian site of Gesher Benot Ya'aqov—spatial and thermoluminesence analyses. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188091.
Ambrose, S. H. (2010). Co-evolution of composite-tool technology, constructive memory, and language. Current Anthropology, 51(5), 135–147.
Google Scholar
Antón, S. C., Aiello, L. C., & Potts, R. (2014). Evolution of early Homo: An integrated biological perspective. Science. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1236828.
Arbib, M. A. (2018). Introducing a special issue. How the brain got language: towards a new road map. Interaction Studies. https://doi.org/10.1075/is.18011.arb.
Arthur, B. (2009). The nature of technology: what it is and how it evolves. London: Allen Lane Group.
Google Scholar
Arthur, K. W. (2018). The lives of stone tools: crafting the status, skill and identity of flintknappers. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.
Google Scholar
Ashton, N. (2016). The human occupation of Britain during the Hoxnian interglacial. Quaternary International, 409, 41–53.
Google Scholar
Aubert, M., Brumm, A., Ramli, M., Sutikna, T., Saptomo, E. W., Hakim, B., et al. (2014). Pleistocene cave art from Sulawesi, Indonesia. Nature, 514, 223–227.
Google Scholar
Aunger, R. (2010). What’s special about human technology? Cambridge Journal of Economics, 34(1), 115–123.
Google Scholar
Backwell, L., & d’Errico, F. (2005). The origin of bone tool technology and the identification of early hominid cultural traditions. In F. d’Errico & L. Backwell (Eds.), From tools to symbols. From early hominids to modern humans (pp. 238–275). Johannesburg: Witwatersrand University Press.
Google Scholar
Bahn, P., & Vertut, J. (1997). Journey through the ice age. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Google Scholar
Balezeau, F., Wilson, B., Gallardo, G., Dick, F., Hopkins, W., Anwander, A., et al. (2020). Primate auditory prototype in evolution of the arcuate fasciculus. Nature Neuroscience, 23, 611–614.
Google Scholar
Balikçi, A. (1970). The Netsilik Eskimo. Garden City: Natural History Press.
Google Scholar
Barbieri, M. (Ed.). (2008). Biosemiotics: the new biological synthesis. New York: Springer.
Google Scholar
Barham, L. (2013). From hand to handle: the first industrial revolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Barham, L., & Mitchell, P. (2008). The first Africans: African archaeology from the earliest toolmakers to most recent foragers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Barsky, D., Carbonell, E., & Ramos, R. S. (2018). Diversity and multiplicity in the Asian Acheulian. L’anthropologie, 122, 59–73.
Google Scholar
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. C. (1999). On the emergence of grammar from the lexicon. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), The emergence of language (pp. 29–80). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Google Scholar
Beck, B. B. (1980). Animal tool behavior: the use and manufacture of tools by animals. New York: Garland STPM Publishing.
Google Scholar
Bednarik, R. (1997). The origins of navigation and language. The Artefact, 20, 16–56.
Google Scholar
Bednarik, R. (1998). The “australopithecine” cobble from Makapansgat, South Africa. South African Archaeological Bulletin, 53, 4–8.
Google Scholar
Bednarik, R. (2003). A figurine from the African Acheulian. Current Anthropology, 44(3), 405–413.
Google Scholar
Bednarik, R. (2014). The beginnings of maritime travel. Advances in Anthropology, 4, 209–221.
Google Scholar
Belfer-Cohen, A., & Goren-Inbar, N. (1994). Cognition and communication in the Levantine lower Palaeolithic. World Archaeology, 26(2), 144–157.
Belitzky, S., Goren-Inbar, N., & Werker, E. (1991). A middle Pleistocene wooden plank with man-made polish. Journal of Human Evolution, 20(4), 349–353.
Google Scholar
Benito-Calvo, A., Barfod, D. N., McHenry, L. J., & de la Torre, I. (2014). The geology and chronology of the Acheulean deposits in the Mieso area (east-Central Ethiopia). Journal of Human Evolution, 76, 26–38.
Google Scholar
Bentley-Condit, V. K., & Smith, E. (2010). Animal tool use: current definitions and an updated comprehensive catalog. Behaviour, 147, 185–221.
Google Scholar
Berlant, T., & Wynn, T. (2018). First sculpture. In T. Berlant & T. Wynn (Eds.), First sculpture. Handaxe to figure stone (pp. 15–48). Dallas: Nasher Sculpture Center.
Google Scholar
Berwick, R. C., & Chomsky, N. (2016). Why only us. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1990). Language and species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2009). Adam's tongue: How humans made language, how language made humans. New York: Hall and Wang.
Bickerton, D. (2014). More than nature needs: language, mind, and evolution. Boston: Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
Biro, D. M., Haslam, M., & Rutz, C. (2013, 1630). Tool use as adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 368. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0408.
Boë, L.-J., Badin, P., Ménard, L., Captier, G., Davis, B., MacNeilage, P., et al. (2013). Anatomy and control of the developing human vocal tract: a response to Lieberman. Journal of Phonetics, 41, 379–392.
Google Scholar
Böe, L.-J., Berthommier, F., Legou, T., Captier, G., Kemp, C., Sawallis, T. R., & Becker, Y. (2017). Evidence of a vocalic proto-system in the baboon (Papio papio) suggests pre-hominin speech precursors. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169321.
Boë, L.-J., Sawallis, T. R., Fagot, J., Badin, P., Barbier, G., Captier, G., et al. (2019). Which way to the dawn of speech?: reanalyzing half a century of debates and data in light of speech science. Science Advances, 5, 1–23.
Google Scholar
Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1984). Mental map in wild chimpanzees: an analysis of hammer transports for nut cracking. Primates, 25(2), 160–170.
Google Scholar
Boivin, N., & Owoc, M. A. (Eds.). (2004). Soils stones and symbols cultural perceptions of the mineral world. London: UCL Press.
Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J., Tattersall, I., Chomsky, N., & Berwick, R. C. (2014). How could language have evolved? PLoS Biology. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001934.
Botha, R. (2010). On the soundness of inferring modern language from symbolic behaviour. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 20(3), 345–356.
Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2020). Neanderthal language. Demystifying the linguistic powers of our extinct cousins. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Brandišauskas, D. (2016). Leaving footprints in the taiga: luck, spirits and ambivalence among the Siberian Orochen reindeer herders and hunters. Oxford: Bergahn.
Google Scholar
Braun, D. R., Plummer, T., Ferraro, J. V., Ditchfield, P., & Bishop, L. C. (2009). Raw material quality and Oldowan hominin toolstone preferences: evidence from Kanjera south, Kenya. Journal of Archaeological Science, 36, 1605–1614.
Google Scholar
Brumm, A., Jensen, G. M., van den Bergh, G. D., Morwood, M. J., Kurniawan, I., Aziz, F., & Storey, M. (2010). Hominins on Flores, Indonesia, by one million years ago. Nature, 464, 748–752.
Google Scholar
Byers, M. A. (1994). Symboling and the middle-upper Palaeolithic transition: a theoretical and methodological critique. Current Anthropology, 35(4), 369–399.
Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (2007). Culture in great apes: using intricate complexity in feeding skills to trace the evolutionary origin of human technical prowess. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 362(1480), 577–585.
Google Scholar
Callaghan, T. (2020). The origins and development of a symbolic mind: the case of pictorial symbols. Interchange. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10780-020-09396-z.
Chase, P. G. (1991). Symbols and Paleolithic artifacts: Style, standardization, and the imposition of arbitrary form. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 10, 193–214.
Chase, P. G., & Dibble, H. L. (1987). Middle Paleolithic symbolism: A review of current evidence and interpretations. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 6(3), 263–296.
Chase, P. G., & Dibble, H. L. (1992). Scientific archaeology and the origins of symbolism: a reply to Bednarik. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 2(1), 43–51.
Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1955). [1975]. The logical structure of linguistic theory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Chomsky, N. (1956). Three models for the description of language. IRE Transactions on Information Theory, 2, 113–124.
Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959). On certain formal properties of grammar. Information and Control, 2(2), 137–167.
Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Chomsky, N., & Schützenberger, M. P. (1963). The algebraic theory of context free languages. In P. Braffort & D. Hirschberg (Eds.), Computer programming and formal languages (pp. 118–161). Amsterdam: North Holland.
Google Scholar
Christiansen, M. H., & Kirby, S. (2003). Language evolution: the hardest problem in science? In M. H. Christiansen & S. Kirby (Eds.), Language evolution (pp. 1–15). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Christiansen, M. H., Chater, N., & Riali, F. (2009). The biological and cultural foundations of language. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 2(3), 221–222.
Google Scholar
Clark, J. D. (2001). Kalambo falls, volume III. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2011). Supersizing the mind. Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Cole, J. (2012). The identity model: a theory to access visual display and hominin cognition with the Palaeolithic. In J. Cole & K. Ruebens (Eds.), Human origins (Vol. 1, pp. 24–40). Oxford: Archaeopress.
Google Scholar
Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. (2009). The rise of Homo sapiens. The evolution of modern thinking. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
Google Scholar
Cooper, A., & Stringer, C. (2013). Did the Denisovans cross Wallace’s line? Science, 342(6156), 321–323.
Google Scholar
Corballis, M. (2002). From hand to mouth: the origins of language. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Google Scholar
Corbey, R., Jagich, A., Vaesen, K., & Collard, M. (2016). Evolutionary Anthropology, 25(1). https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21467.
Cousins, S. D. (2014). The semiotic coevolution of mind and culture. Culture & Psychology, 20(2), 160–191.
Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., & Zuberbühler, K. (2017). Vocalizing in chimpanzees is influenced by social-cognitive processes. Science Advances, 3(11). https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.1701742.
Crompton, R., & Gowlett, J. G. (1993). Allometry and multidimensional form in Acheulean bifaces from Kilombe Kenya. Journal of Human Evolution, 25, 175–200.
Google Scholar
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1149–1157.
Google Scholar
d’Errico, F., & Stringer, C. (2011). Evolution, revolution or saltation scenario for the emergence of modern cultures? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 366, 1060–1069.
Google Scholar
Dale, R., & Lupyan, G. (2012). Understanding the origins of morphological diversity: the linguistic niche hypothesis. Advances in Complex Systems. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10780-020-09396-z10.1142/S021952591150017.
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.
Google Scholar
Davidson, I. (2002). The finished artefact fallacy: Acheulean handaxes and language origins. In A. Wray (Ed.), Transitions to language (pp. 180–203). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Davidson, I., & Noble, W. (1989). The archaeology of perception: traces of depiction and language. Current Anthropology, 30(2), 125–155.
Google Scholar
Davidson, I., & Noble, W. (1992). Why the first colonisation of the Australian region is the earliest evidence of modern human behaviour. Archaeology in Oceania, 27(3), 135–142.
Davidson, I., & Noble, W. (1993). Tools and language in human evolution. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language and cognition in human evolution (pp. 363–388). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2011). The tyranny of the discontinuous mind. NewStatesman, www.newstatesman.com/blogs/the-staggers/2011/12/issue-essay-line-dawkins
de Boer, B. (2017). Evolution of speech and evolution of language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 158–162.
Google Scholar
de la Torre, I. (2016). The origins of the Acheulean: past and present perspectives on a major transition in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 371. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0245.
de la Torre, I., & Mora, R. (2005). Technological strategies in the Lower Pleistocene at Olduvai beds I & II. Lìege: ERAUL, 112.
de la Torre, I., Mora, R., Arroyo, A., & Benito-Calvo, A. (2014). Acheulean technological behaviour in the middle Pleistocene landscape of Mieso (east-Central Ethiopia). Journal of Human Evolution, 76, 1–25.
Google Scholar
de Lumley, H., Echassoux, A., Bailon, S., Cauche, D., de Marchi, M.-P., Desclaux, E., El Guennouni, K., et al. (2004). Le sol d’occupation acheuléen de l’unité archéostratigraphique UA 25 de la grotte du Lazaret. Edisud: Alpes-Maritimes. Aix-en-Provence.
Google Scholar
de Saussure, F. (1916 [1983]). Course in general linguistics. In C. Bally & A. Sechehaye (Eds.), Translated by R. Harris. La Salle. Illinois: Open Court.
Google Scholar
Deacon, T. (1997). The symbolic species. The co-evolution of language and the brain. London: W.W. Norton & Co..
Google Scholar
Deacon, T. (2010). A role for relaxed selection in the evolution of language capacity. PNAS. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914624107.
Deacon, H. J., & Wurz, S. (2001). Middle Pleistocene populations of southern Africa and the emergence of modern behaviour. In L. Barham & K. Robson-Brown (Eds.), Africa and Asia in the middle Pleistocene (pp. 55–63). Bristol: Western Academic & Specialist Press Limited.
Google Scholar
Dediu, D., & Levinson, S. C. (2013). On the antiquity of language: the reinterpretation of Neandertal linguistic capacities and its consequences. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.
Dediu, D., Janssen, R., & Moisik, S. R. (2017). Language is not isolated from its wider environment: vocal tract influences on the evolution of speech and language. Language & Communication, 54, 9–20.
Google Scholar
Defez, A. (2013). Plato, Wittgenstein and the origins of language. In L. Perissinotto & B. R. Cámara (Eds.), Wittgenstein and Plato. Connections, comparisons and contrasts (pp. 183–195). London: Palgrave Macmillan.
Google Scholar
Delahaye, P. (2019). A semiotic methodology for animal studies. Paris: Springer Nature Switzerland AG.
Google Scholar
Dibble, H. (1987). The interpretation of middle Paleolithic scraper morphology. American Antiquity, 52(1), 109–117.
Google Scholar
Dibble, H. L., Holdaway, S. J., Lin, S. C., Braun, D. R., Douglass, M. J., Iovita, R., et al. (2016). Major fallacies surrounding stone artifacts and assemblages. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-016-9297-8.
Diez-Martína, F., Wynn, T., Sánchez-Yustosa, P., Duquea, J., Frailea, C., de Franciscoa, S., et al. (2019). A faltering origin for the Acheulean? Technological and cognitive implications from FLK West (Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania). Quaternary International, 526, 49–66.
Google Scholar
Dominguez-Rodrigo, M., Alcalá, L. L., & Luque, L. (2009). Peninj: a research project on human origins (1995–2005). Oxford: Oxbow Books.
Google Scholar
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Draper, P. (1976). Social and economic constraints on child life among the !Kung. In R. B. Lee & I. DeVore (Eds.), Kalahari hunter-gatherers: studies of the !Kung san and their neighbors (pp. 199–217). London: Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Eco, U. (1976). Peirce's notion of interpretant. Modern Language Notes, 91(6), 1457–1472.
Google Scholar
Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1940) [1979]. The Nuer: a description of the modes of livelihood and political institutions of a Nilotic people. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Everett, D. L. (1979). Aspectos da fonologia do Pirahã, unpublished Master's thesis. Universidade Estadual de Campinas: http://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/001715.
Everett, D. L. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: Another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology, 46(4), 621–646.
Google Scholar
Everett, D. L. (2008). Don’t sleep, there are snakes: life and language in the Amazonian jungle. New York: Pantheon.
Google Scholar
Everett, D. L. (2012). Language: the cultural tool. New York: Vintage Books.
Google Scholar
Everett, D. L. (2016). Dark matter of the mind: the culturally articulated unconscious. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Google Scholar
Everett, D. L. (2017). How language began: the story of humanity’s greatest invention. London: Profile Books.
Google Scholar
Everett, D. L., & Gibson, E. (2019). Review of Recursion Across Domains. In L. Amaral, M. Maia, A. Nevins, & T. Roeper (Eds.), (pp.777–790). Cambridge University Press. Language, 95(4).
Fairlie, J. E., & Barham, L. S. (2016). From chaîne opératoire to observational analysis: a pilot study of hominin tool-making events. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 26(4), 643–664.
Google Scholar
Fedorenko, E., Nieto-Castañon, A., & Kanwisher, N. (2012). Lexical and syntactic representations in the brain: An fMRI investigation of multi-voxel pattern analysis. Neuropsychologia, 50(4), 499–513.
Google Scholar
Ferraro, J. V., Plummer, T. W., Pobiner, B. L., Oliver, J. S., Bishop, L. C., Braun, D. R., et al. (2013). Earliest archaeological evidence of persistent hominin carnivory. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0062174.
Finlayson, C., Brown, K., Blasco, R., Rosell, J., Negro, J. J., Bortolotti, J. R., et al. (2012). Birds of a feather: Neanderthal exploitation of raptors and corvids. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045927.
Firchow, I. B., & Firchow, J. (1969). An abbreviated phoneme inventory. Anthropological Linguistics, 11, 271–276.
Google Scholar
Fisch, M. H. (1986). Peirce, semiotic, and pragmatism. Bloomington: Indiana University Bloomington.
Google Scholar
Fisher, S. E. (2017). Evolution of language: lessons from the genome. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(1), 34–40.
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2010). The evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2018). The biology and evolution of speech: a comparative analysis. Annual Review of Linguistics, 4, 255–279.
Google Scholar
Futrell, R., Stearns, L., Piantadosi, S. T., Everett, D. L., & Gibson, E. (2016). A corpus investigation of syntactic embedding in Pirahã. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0145289.
Galloti, R., & Mussi, M. (2017). Two Acheuleans, two humankinds: From 1.5 to 0.85 ma at Melka Kunture (upper awash, Ethiopian highlands). Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 95, 137–181.
Google Scholar
Gallotti, R., Collina, C., Raynal, J.-P., Kieffer, G., Geraads, D., & Piperno, M. (2010). The early middle Pleistocene site of Gombore II (Melka Kunture, upper awash, Ethiopia) and the issue of Acheulean bifacial shaping strategies. African Archaeological Review, 27, 291–322.
Google Scholar
Gamble, C., & Marshall, G. (2001). The shape of handaxes, the structure of the Acheulian world. In S. Milliken & J. Cook (Eds.), A very remote period indeed. Papers on the Palaeolithic presented to Derek Roe (pp. 19–27). Oxford: Oxbow Books.
Google Scholar
García-Medrano, P., Ollé, A., Ashton, N., & Roberts, M. B. (2019). The mental template in handaxe manufacture: new insights into Acheulean lithic technological behaviour at Boxgrove, Sussex, UK. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 26, 396–422.
Google Scholar
Gärdenfors, P. (2003). How Homo became sapiens. On the evolution of thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gärdenfors, P. (2004). Cooperation and the evolution of symbolic communication. In K. Oller & U. Griebel (Eds.), The evolution of communication systems (pp. 237–256). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Gärdenfors, P., & Högberg, A. (2017). The archaeology of teaching and the evolution of Homo docens. Current Anthropology, 58, 188–201.
Google Scholar
Gentilucci, M., & Corballis, M. (2006). From manual gesture to speech: a gradual transition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 949–960.
Google Scholar
Gibson, K. R. (2002). Customs and cultures in animals and humans. Anthropological Theory, 2(3), 323–339.
Google Scholar
Gil, D. (2009). How much grammar does it take to sail a boat? In G. Sampson, D. Gil, & P. Trudgill (Eds.), Language complexity as an evolving variable (pp. 19–33). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Gillespie-Lynch, K., Greenfield, P. M., Lyn, H., & Savage-Rumbaugh, S. (2014). Gestural and symbolic development among apes and humans: support for a multimodal theory of language evolution. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01228.
Goldberg, A. E. (2019). Explain me this: creativity, competition, and the partial productivity of constructions. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Google Scholar
Gómez-Olivencia, A., Carretero, J. M., Arsuaga, J. L., Rodríguez-García, L., García-González, R., & Martínez, I. (2007). Metric and morphological study of the upper cervical spine from the Sima de los Huesos site (sierra de Atapuerca, Burgos, Spain). Journal of Human Evolution, 53, 6–25.
Google Scholar
Goren-Inbar, N. (2011). Culture and cognition in the Acheulian industry: A case study from Gesher Benot Yaʿaqov. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1038–1049.
Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. Patterns of behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
Gosden, C. (2005). What do objects want? Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 12(3), 193–211.
Google Scholar
Gosden, C., & Marshall, Y. (1999). World Archaeology, 31(2), 169–178.
Gould, R. A., Koster, D. A., & Sontz, A. H. L. (1971). The lithic assemblage of the Western Desert aborigines of Australia. American Antiquity, 36(2), 149–169.
Google Scholar
Gowlett, J. A. G. (1996). Mental abilities of early Homo: elements of constraint and choice in rule systems. In P. Mellars & K. Gibson (Eds.), Modelling the early human mind (pp. 191–215). Cambridge: McDonald Institute.
Gowlett, J. A. J. (2006). The elements of design form in Acheulian bifaces: modes, modalities, rules and language. In N. Goren-Inbar & G. Sharon (Eds.), Axe age: Acheulian toolmaking from quarry to discard (pp. 203–222). London: Equinox.
Google Scholar
Gowlett, J. A. J. (2009). The longest transition or multiple revolutions? Curves and steps in the record of human origins. In P. Camps & P. R. Chauhan (Eds.), Sourcebook of Paleolithic transitions (pp. 65–78). New York: Springer.
Google Scholar
Gowlett, J. A. J. (2013). Elongation as a factor in artefacts of humans and other animals: an Acheulean example in a comparative context. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0114.
Gowlett, J. A. J. (2015). Variability in an early hominin percussive tradition: the Acheulean versus cultural variation in modern chimpanzee artefacts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0358.
Grove, M. (2016). Population density, mobility, and cultural transmission. Journal of Archaeological Science, 74, 75–84.
Google Scholar
Grüber, T., Zuberbühler, K., Clément, F., & van Schaik, C. (2015). Apes have culture but may not know that they do. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(91), 1–14.
Google Scholar
Guindon, F. (2015). Technology, material culture and the well-being of aboriginal peoples of Canada. Journal of Material Culture, 20(1), 77–97.
Google Scholar
Harmand, S., Lewis, J. E., Feibel, C. S., Lepre, C. J., Prat, S., Lenoble, A., et al. (2015). 3.3-million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature, 521, 310–315.
Google Scholar
Haspelmath, M. (2020). Human linguisticality and the building blocks of languages. Frontiers of Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03056.
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Tecumseh Fitch, W. (2002). The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569–1579.
Google Scholar
Hawkes, C. (1954). Archaeological theory and method: Some suggestions from the Old World. American Anthropologist, 56, 155–168.
Head, M. J., & Gibbard, P. L. (2005). Early-middle Pleistocene transitions: an overview and recommendation for the defining boundary. In M. J. Head & P. L. Gibbard (Eds.), Early-Middle Pleistocene transitions: The land-ocean evidence (pp. 1-18). London: Geological Society of London, Special Publication 247.
Heesen, R., Hobaiter, C., Ferrer-i-Cancho, R., & Semple, S. (2019). Linguistic laws in chimpanzee gestural communication. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.2900.
Henshilwood, C. S., & Marean, C. A. (2003). The origin of modern human behaviour: critique of the models and their test implications. Current Anthropology, 44(5), 627–651.
Google Scholar
Herries, A. I. R. (2003). Magnetostratigraphic seriation of south African hominin palaeocaves. Unpublished Ph.D thesis, geomagnetism laboratory, Department of Archaeology, University of Liverpool.
Herzlinger, G., Wynn, T., & Goren-Inbar, N. (2017). Expert cognition in the production sequence of Acheulian cleavers at Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, Israel: a lithic and cognitive analysis. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188337.
Hewlett, B. S. (2016). Social learning and innovation in hunter-gatherers. In H. Terashima & B. S. Hewlett (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers (pp. 1–15). Tokyo: Springer.
Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1986). Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. American Anthropologist, 88, 922–934.
Google Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1960). The origin of speech. Scientific American, 203, 88–111.
Google Scholar
Hodder, I. (1982). The present past. An introduction to anthropology for archaeologists. London: Batsford Books.
Google Scholar
Hodder, I. (1994). Reply to Byers “Symboling and the middle-upper Palaeolithic transition”. Current Anthropology, 35(4), 385–386.
Google Scholar
Hodder, I. (2012). Entangled. An archaeology of the relationship between humans and things. Chichester: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Hodgson, D. (2015). The symmetry of Acheulean handaxes and cognitive evolution. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports, 2, 204–208.
Google Scholar
Hoffmann, D. L., Standish, C. D., García-Diez, M., Pettitt, P. B., Milton, J. A., Zilhão, J., et al. (2018). U-Th dating of carbonate crusts reveals Neandertal origin of Iberian cave art. Science. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aap7778.
Holloway, R. (1969). Culture: a human domain. Current Anthropology, 10(2), 395–412.
Google Scholar
Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P., & Leavens, D. A. (2007). Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Animal Behaviour, 73, 281–286.
Hopkinson, T., Nowell, A., & White, M. (2013). Life histories, metapopulation ecology, and innovation in the Acheulian. PaleoAnthropology. https://doi.org/10.4207/PA.2013.ART80.
Hosfield, R., Cole, J., & McNabb, J. (2018). Less of a bird’s song than a rock ensemble. Evolutionary Anthropology, 27, 9–20.
Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. (2004). Human uniqueness, learned symbols and recursive thought. European Review, 12(4), 551–565.
Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. (2014). Origins of language. A slim guide. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Iliopoulos, A. (2016). The material dimensions of signification: rethinking the nature and emergence of semiosis in the debate on human origins. Quaternary International, 405, 111–124.
Google Scholar
Ingicco, T., van den Bergh, G. D., Jago-on, C., Bahain, J.-J., Chacón, M., Amano, G. N., et al. (2018). Earliest known hominin activity in the Philippines by 709 thousand years ago. Nature, 557, 233–237.
Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (1993). Technology, language, intelligence: a reconsideration of basic concepts. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language and cognition in human evolution (pp. 449–472). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2001). Beyond art and technology: the anthropology of skill. In B. M. Schiffer (Ed.), Anthropological perspectives on technology (pp. 17–31). Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press.
Google Scholar
Isaac, G. L. (Ed.). (1997). Koobi fora research project. Volume 5. Plio-Pleistocene archaeology. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1999). Possible stages in the evolution of the language capacity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(7), 272–279.
Jackendoff, R., & Wittenberg, E. (2014). What you can say without syntax: a hierarchy of grammatical complexity. In F. J. Newmeyer & L. B. Preston (Eds.), Measuring grammatical complexity (pp. 65–82). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Jaubert, J., Verheyden, S., Genty, D., Soulier, M., Cheng, H., Blamart, D., et al. (2016). Early Neanderthal constructions deep in Bruniquel cave in southwestern France. Nature, 534. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature18291.
Johnson, C. R., & McBrearty, S. (2010). 500,000-year-old blades from the Kapthurin, formation, Kenya. Journal of Human Evolution, 58, 193–200.
Google Scholar
Kalan, A. K., Mundry, R., & Boesch, C. (2015). Wild chimpanzees modify food call structure with respect to tree size for a particular fruit species. Animal Behaviour, 101, 1–9.
Google Scholar
Karlsson, F. (2009). Recursion and iteration. In H. van der Hulst (Ed.), Recursion and human language (pp. 43–68). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Google Scholar
Kealy, S., Louys, J., & O’Connor, S. (2016). Islands under the sea: a review of early modern human dispersal routes and migration hypotheses through Wallacea. The Journal of Island and Coastal Archaeology. https://doi.org/10.1080/15564894.2015.1119218.
Key, A. J. M. (2019). Handaxe shape variation in a relative context. Comptes Rendus Palevol, 18(5), 555–567.
Google Scholar
Killick, D. (2004). Social constructionist approaches to the study of technology. World Archaeology, 36(4), 571–578.
Google Scholar
Klein, R. (2008). Out of Africa and the evolution of human behaviour. Evolutionary Anthropology, 17(6), 267–281.
Google Scholar
Klein, R. (2017). Language and human evolution. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 43(B), 204–222.
Google Scholar
Kohn, M., & Mithen, S. (1999). Handaxes: products of sexual selection? Antiquity, 73, 518–526.
Google Scholar
Kolodny, O., & Edelman, S. (2018). The evolution of the capacity for language: The ecological context and adaptive value of a process of cognitive hijacking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0052.
Kopytoff, I. (1986). The cultural biography of things: commoditization as process. In A. Appadurai (Ed.), The social life of things: commodities in cultural perspective (pp. 64–91). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Kroll, E. (1997). Lithic and faunal distributions at eight archaeological excavations. In G. L. Isaac (Ed.), Koobi fora research project, volume 5: Plio-Pleistocene archaeology (pp. 459–543). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Google Scholar
Kuhn, S. L. (1992). On planning and curated technologies in the middle Paleolithic. Journal of Anthropological Research, 48(3), 185–214.
Google Scholar
Laitman, J. T. (1984). The anatomy of human speech. Natural History, 93, 20–27.
Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: the embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.
Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. (2017). The origins of language in teaching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1077-7.
Latour, B. (1992). Where are the missing masses? The sociology of a few mundane artifacts. In W. E. Bijker & J. Law (Eds.), Shaping technology/building society: studies in sociotechnical change (pp. 225–258). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1979). The !Kung san: men, women and work in a foraging society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Lefebvre, L., Nicolakakis, N., & Boire, D. (2002). Tools and brains in birds. Behaviour, 139, 939–973.
Lemorini, C., Plummer, T. W., Braun, D. R., Crittenden, A. N., Ditchfield, P. W., Bishop, L. C., et al. (2014). Old stones' song: use-wear experiments and analysis of the Oldowan quartz and quartzite assemblage from Kanjera south (Kenya). Journal of Human Evolution, 72, 10–25.
Google Scholar
Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1993). Gesture and speech (translated by A. B. Berger). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2017). How do hunter-gatherer children learn subsistence skills? Human Nature, 28, 367–394.
Google Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Lavi, N., Reckin, R., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2018). How do hunter-gatherer children learn social and gender norms? A meta-ethnographic review. Cross-Cultural Research, 52(2), 213–255.
Google Scholar
Li, H., Kuman, K., Lotter, M. G., Leader, G. M., & Gibbon, R. J. (2017). The Victoria West: Earliest prepared core technology in the Acheulean at canteen kopje and implications for the cognitive evolution of early hominids. Royal Society Open Science. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.170288.
Lieberman, P. (1984). The biology and evolution of language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (2007). The evolution of human speech. Its anatomical and neural bases. Current Anthropology, 48(1), 39–66.
Google Scholar
Lombao, D., Guardiola, M., & Mosquera, M. (2017). Teaching to make stone tools: new experimental evidence supporting a technological hypothesis for the origins of language. Nature: Scientific Reports. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-14322-y.
Lombard, M., & Gärdenfors, P. (2017). Tracking the evolution of causal cognition in humans. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 95, 219–234.
Google Scholar
Lupyan, G., & Dale, R. (2010). Language structure is partly determined by social structure. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0008559.
Lycett, S. J., & Gowlett, J. A. J. (2008). On questions surrounding the Acheulian “tradition”. World Archaeology, 40(3), 295–315.
Google Scholar
Lycett, S. J., Schillinger, K., Eren, M. I., von Cramon-Taubadel, N., & Mesoudi, A. (2016). Factors affecting Acheulean handaxe variation: experimental insights, microevolutionary processes, and macroevolutionary outcomes. Quaternary International. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2015.08.021.
Lyn, H., Greenfield, P. M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Gillespie-Lynch, K., & Hopkins, W. D. (2011). Nonhuman primates do declare! A comparison of declarative symbol and gesture use in two children, two bonobos, and a chimpanzee. Language and Communication, 31, 63–74.
Google Scholar
MacDonald, K. (2007). Cross-cultural comparison of learning in human hunting. Implications for life history evolution. Human Nature, 18, 386–402.
Google Scholar
MacLarnon, A., & Hewitt, G. (2004). Increased breathing control: another factor in the evolution of human language. Evolutionary Anthropology, 13(5), 181–197.
Google Scholar
Mahaney, R. A. (2014). Exploring the complexity and structure of Acheulean stoneknapping in relation to natural language. PaleoAnthropology. https://doi.org/10.4207/PA.2014.ART90.
Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind: a theory of material engagement. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute for Technology.
Google Scholar
Malinsky-Buller, A. (2016). The muddle in the middle Pleistocene: the lower–middle Paleolithic transition from the Levantine perspective. Journal of World Prehistory, 29(1), 1–78.
Google Scholar
Marlowe, F. W. (2005). Hunter-gatherers and human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20046.
Marshack, A. (1997). The Berekhat ram figurine: a late Acheulian carving from the Middle East. Antiquity, 71, 327–337.
Google Scholar
Martínez, I., Rosa, M., Arsuaga, J.-L., Jarabo, P., Quam, R., Gracia Lorenzo, C., et al. (2004). Auditory capacities in middle Pleistocene humans from the sierra de Atapuerca in Spain. PNAS. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0403595101.
Martínez, I., Arsuaga, J.-L., Quam, R., Carretero, J. M., Gracia, A., & Rodríguez, L. (2008). Human hyoid bones from the middle Pleistocene site of the Sima de los Huesos (sierra de Atapuerca, Spain). Journal of Human Evolution, 54, 118–124.
Google Scholar
Martinon-Torres, M., Xing, S., Liu, W., & Maria Bermudez de Castro, J. (2018). A "source and sink" model for East Asia? Preliminary approach through the dental evidence. Comptes Rendus Palevol, 17(1–2), 33–43.
Google Scholar
McBrearty, S., & Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution that wasn't: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 39(5), 453–563.
McCall, G. (2016). Before modern humans. New perspectives on the African stone age. London: Routledge.
Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (2010). In search of the last common ancestor: new findings on wild chimpanzees. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 365(1556), 3267–3276.
Google Scholar
McNabb, J., & Cole, J. (2015). The mirror cracked: symmetry and refinement in the Acheulean handaxe. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports, 3, 100–111.
Google Scholar
McPherron, S. P. (2000). Handaxes as a measure of the mental capabilities of early hominids. Journal of Archaeological Science, 27(8), 655–664.
Google Scholar
Medin, D. L., & Rips, L. J. (2005). Concepts and categories: memory, meaning, and metaphysics. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 37–72). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Mellars, P. (1973). The character of the middle – Upper Palaeolithic transition in south-West France. In C. Refrew (Ed.), The explanation of culture change: models in prehistory (pp. 255–276). Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.
Google Scholar
Mellars, P. (1989). Technological changes across the middle-upper Paleolithic transition: economic, social and cognitive perspectives. In P. Mellars & C. Stringer (Eds.), The human revolution, behavioural and biological perspectives in the origins of modern humans (pp. 338–365). Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press.
Google Scholar
Mellars, P. (2005). The impossible coincidence. A single-species model for the origins of modern human behavior in Europe. Evolutionary Anthropology, 14(1), 12–27.
Google Scholar
Mellars, P., & French, J. (2011). Tenfold population increase in Western Europe at the Neandertal–to–modern human transition. Science, 333(6042), 623–627.
Google Scholar
Méndez-Quintas, E., Santonja, M., Perez-Gonzalez, A., Duval, M., Demuro, M., & Arnold, L. J. (2018). First evidence of an extensive Acheulean large cutting tool accumulation in Europe from Porto Maior (Galicia, Spain). Science Reports, 8. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-21320-1.
Meyer, M. R. (2016). The spinal cord in hominin evolution. eLS. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470015902.a0027058.
Meyer, M. R., & Haeusler, M. (2015). Spinal cord evolution in early Homo. Journal of Human Evolution, 88, 43–53.
Google Scholar
Meyer, G. F., Spray, A., Fairlie, J. E., & Uomini, N. T. (2014). Inferring common cognitive mechanisms from brain blood-flow lateralization data: a new methodology for fTCD analysis. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00552.
Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind. The cognitive origins of art, religion and science. London: Thames & Hudson.
Google Scholar
Moncel, M.-H., & Schreve, D. (2016). The Acheulean in Europe: origins, evolution and dispersal. Quaternary International, 411(B), 1–8.
Google Scholar
Moncel, M. H., Arzarello, M., Boëda, É., Bonilauri, S., Chevrierc, B., Gaillarda, C., Forestiera, H., Yinghuad, L., Sémaha, F., & Zeitoune, V. (2018). Assemblages with bifacial tools in Eurasia (third part). Considerations on the bifacial phenomenon throughout Eurasia assemblages. Comptes Rendus Palevol. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crpv.2015.11.007.
Morgan, T. J. H., Uomini, N. T., Rendell, L. E., Chouinard-Thuly, L., Street, S. E., Lewis, H. M., et al. (2015). Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nature Communications. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms7029.
Müller, M. F. (1864). Lectures on the science of language. 2nd series. London: Longmans.
Google Scholar
Muller, A., Clarkson, C., & Shipton, C. (2017). Measuring behavioural and cognitive complexity in lithic technology throughout human evolution. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 48, 166–180.
Google Scholar
Murphy, E. (2015). Labels, cognomes, and cyclic computation: an ethological perspective. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00715.
Musgrave, S. M., Lonsdorf, E., Morgand, D., Prestipinoc, M., Bernstein-Kurtycze, L., Mundry, R., & Sanz, C. (2020). Teaching varies with task complexity in wild chimpanzees. PNAS, 117(2), 969–976.
Google Scholar
Newbrand, H. L. (1951). A phonemic analysis of Hawaiian, University of Hawaii Honolulu M.A. Thesis.
Noble, W., & Davidson, I. (1996). Human evolution, language and mind: a psychological and archaeological enquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Osvath, M. (2009). Spontaneous planning for future stone throwing by a male chimpanzee. Current Biology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.010.
Osvath, M., & Gärdenfors, P. (2005). Oldowan culture and the evolution of anticipatory cognition. Lund University Cognitive Science, 122, 1–16.
Google Scholar
Osvath, M., & Karvonen, E. (2012). Spontaneous innovation for future deception in a male chimpanzee. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036782.
Overmann, K. A., & Wynn, T. (2019). On tools making minds: an archaeological perspective on human cognitive evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 19, 39–58.
Google Scholar
Pedersen, J. (2012). The symbolic mind: apes, symbols, and the evolution of language. In A study of bonobos and what they may teach us about language, thought, and evolution. Lambert Academic: Publishing.
Google Scholar
Peirce, C. S. (1977). In C. Hardwick (Ed.), Semiotics and significs. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Google Scholar
Peirce, C. S. (1992). The essential Peirce volume 2: Selected philosophical writings, 1893 - 1913. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Google Scholar
Peirce, C. S. (1998). The essential Peirce: selected philosophical writings (1867–1893). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Google Scholar
Pfaffenberger, B. (1992). Social anthropology of technology. Annual Review of Anthropology, 21, 491–516.
Google Scholar
Pfaffenberger, B. (2001). Symbols do not create meaning – activities do: or, why symbolic anthropology needs the anthropology of technology. In B. M. Schiffer (Ed.), Anthropological perspectives on technology (pp. 77–86). Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press.
Google Scholar
Piantadosi, S. T., & Fedorenko, E. (2017). Infinitely productive language can arise from chance under communicative pressure. Journal of Language Evolution. https://doi.org/10.1093/jole/lzw013.
Piatelli-Palmarini, M. (2010). What is language, that it may have evolved, and what is evolution, that it may apply to language. In R. Larson, V. Déprez, & H. Yamakido (Eds.), The evolution of human language: biolinguistic perspectives (approaches to the evolution of language) (pp. 148–162). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511817755.011.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Pinch, T. J., & Bijker, W. E. (1984). The social construction of facts and artefacts: or how the sociology of science and the sociology of technology might benefit each other. Social Studies of Science, 14, 399–441.
Google Scholar
Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13(4), 707–784.
Google Scholar
Plummer, T. W., & Bishop, L. C. (2016). Oldowan hominin behavior and ecology at Kanjera South Kenya. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 94, 29–40.
Google Scholar
Pollick, A. S., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Ape gestures and language evolution. PNAS, 104(19), 8184–8189.
Google Scholar
Pons-Branchu, E., Sanchidrián, J. L., Fontugne, M., Medina-Alcaide, M. A., Quiles, A., Thil, F., & Valladas, H. (2020). U-series dating at Nerja cave reveal open system. Questioning the Neanderthal origin of Spanish rock art. Journal of Archaeological Science. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jas.2020.105120.
Potts, R. (1991). Why the Oldowan? Plio-Pleistocene toolmaking and the transport of resources. Journal of Anthropological Research, 47, 153–176.
Google Scholar
Powell, A., Shennan, S., & Thomas, M. G. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324, 1298–1301.
Google Scholar
Power, C. (2009). Sexual selection models for the emergence of symbolic communication: why they should be reversed. In R. Botha & C. Knight (Eds.), The cradle of language (pp. 257–280). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Presnyakova, D., Braun, D. R., Conard, N. J., Feibel, C., Harris, J. W. K., Pop, M., et al. (2018). Site fragmentation, hominin mobility and LCT variability reflected in the early Acheulean record of the Okote Member, at Koobi Fora, Kenya. Journal of Human Evolution. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2018.07.008.
Preucel, R. W. (2006). Archaeological semiotics. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.
Google Scholar
Preysler, J. B., Navas, C. T., & Sharon, G. (2018). Life history of a large flake biface. Quaternary Science Reviews, 190, 123–136.
Google Scholar
Pullum, G. K. (2020). Theorizing about the syntax of human language: a radical alternative to generative formalisms. Cadernos de Linguística, 1(1), 1–33.
Google Scholar
Putt, S. S., Woods, A. D., & Franciscus, R. G. (2014). The role of verbal interaction during experimental bifacial stone tool manufacture. Lithic Technology, 49(2), 96–112.
Google Scholar
Putt, S. S. J., Wijeakumar, S., & Spencer, J. P. (2019). Prefrontal cortex activation supports the emergence of early stone age toolmaking skill. NeuroImage, 199, 57–69.
Google Scholar
Queiroz, J., & Ribeiro, S. (2002). The biological substrate of icons, indexes, and symbols in animal communication: a neurosemiotic analysis of vervet monkey alarm-calls. In M. Shapiro (Ed.), The Peirce seminar papers 5 — the state of the art (pp. 69–78). Oxford: Berghahn Books.
Google Scholar
Ribeiro, S., Loula, A., de Araújo, I., Gudwin, R., & Queiroz, J. (2006). Symbols are not uniquely human. Biosystems, 90(1), 263–272.
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Google Scholar
Rodríguez, C., & Moro, C. (2008). Coming to agreement: Object use by infants and adults. In J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 89–114). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Roe, D. A. (1968). British lower and middle Palaeolithic handaxe groups. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society, 34, 1–82.
Google Scholar
Rossano, M. J. (2010). Making friends, making tools, and making symbols. Current Anthropology, 51(S1), S89–S98.
Rouse, I. (1960). The classification of artifacts in archeology. American Antiquity, 25(3), 313–323.
Google Scholar
Ruck, L. M. (2014). Manual praxis in stone tool manufacture: implications for language evolution. Brain and Language. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2014.10.003.
Ruck, L. M., & Uomini, N. T. (in press). Artefact, praxis, tool, and symbol. In A. Lock, C. Sinha, & N. Gontier (Eds.), Oxford handbook of symbolic evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Ruxton, G. D., & Wilkinson, D. M. (2012). Population trajectories for accidental versus planned colonisation of islands. Journal of Human Evolution, 63(3), 507–511.
Google Scholar
Sackett, J. R. (1982). Approaches to style in lithic archaeology. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 1, 59–122.
Google Scholar
Sackett, J. R. (1986). Isochrestism and style: a clarification. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 5(3), 266–277.
Google Scholar
Sanz, C. M., & Morgan, D. B. (2013). Ecological and social correlates of chimpanzee tool use. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0416.
Sapir, E. (1915). The Na-dene languages: a preliminary report. American Anthropologist, 17(3), 765–773.
Google Scholar
Sapir, E. (1927). Language as a form of human behavior. The English Journal, 16(6), 421–433.
Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Fields, W. M., & Spircu, T. (2004). The emergence of knapping and vocal expression embedded in a pan/Homo culture. Biology and Philosophy, 19(4), 541–575.
Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2003). Signalers and receivers in animal communication. Annual Reviews in Psychology, 54, 145–173.
Google Scholar
Sharon, G. (2008). The impact of raw material on Acheulian large flake production. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35, 1329–1344.
Google Scholar
Sharon, G. (2009). Acheulian giant-core technology. A worldwide perspective. Current Anthropology, 50(3), 335–419.
Google Scholar
Sharon, G. (2010). Large flake Acheulian. Quaternary International, 223-224, 226–233.
Google Scholar
Sharon, G., & Beaumont, P. (2006). Victoria West: a highly standardized prepared core technology. In N. Goren-Inbar & G. Sharon (Eds.), Axe age: Acheulian toolmaking from quarry to discard (pp. 181–199). London: Equinox Publishing Ltd..
Google Scholar
Shea, J. J. (2017). Occasional, obligatory, and habitual stone tool use in hominin evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 26, 200–217.
Google Scholar
Shipton, C. (2013). A million years of hominin sociality and cognition: Acheulean bifaces in the Hunsgi-Baichbal Valley, India. Oxford: Archaeopress.
Google Scholar
Shipton, C. (2016). Hierarchical organization in the Acheulean to middle Palaeolithic transition at Bhimbetka, India. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 26(4), 601–618.
Google Scholar
Shipton, C. (2018). Biface knapping skill in the east African Acheulean: progressive trends and random walks. African Archaeological Review, 35, 107–131.
Google Scholar
Shipton, C., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Before cumulative culture: the evolutionary origin of overimitation and shared intentionality. Human Nature, 26, 331–345.
Google Scholar
Shipton, C., & Nielsen, M. (2018). The acquisition of biface knapping skill in the Acheulean. In L. Di Paolo, F. Di Vincenzo, & F. De Petrillo (Eds.), Evolution of primate social cognition. Interdisciplinary evolution research. Cham: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-93776-2_18.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Shipton, C., & White, M. J. (2020). Handaxe types, colonization waves, and social norms in the British Acheulean. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jasrep.2020.102352.
Shipton, C., Clarkson, C., Pal, J. N., Jones, S. C., Roberts, R. G., Harris, C., et al. (2013). Generativity, hierarchical action and recursion in the technology of the Acheulean to middle Palaeolithic transition: a perspective from Patpara, the Son Valley, India. Journal of Human Evolution, 65(2), 93–108.
Google Scholar
Shipton, C., Clarkson, C., & Cobden, R. (2018). Were Acheulean bifaces deliberately made symmetrical? Archaeological and experimental evidence. Cambridge Archaeological Journal. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095977431800032X.
Shumaker, R. W., Kristina, R., Walkup, K. R., & Beck, B. B. (2011). Animal tool behavior: The use and manufacture of tools by animals. Revised and (updated ed.). Baltimore (Maryland): Johns Hopkins University Press.
Simanjuntak, T., Sémah, F., & Caillard, C. (2010). The Palaeolithic in Indonesia: nature and chronology. Quaternary International, 223-224, 418–421.
Google Scholar
Skibo, J. M., & Schiffer, M. (2008). People and things. A behavioral approach to material culture. New York: Springer.
Google Scholar
Slocombe, K. E., & Zuberbühler, K. (2005). Functionally referential communication in a chimpanzee. Current Biology, 15, 1779–1784.
Google Scholar
Sloutsky, V. M. (2010). From perceptual categories to concepts: what develops? Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 34, 1244–1286.
Google Scholar
Spikins, P. (2012). Goodwill hunting?: debates on the ‘meaning’ of lower palaeolithic handaxe form revisited. World Archaeology, 44(3), 378–392.
Google Scholar
Steele, J., Clegg, M., & Martelli, S. (2013). Comparative morphology of the hominin and African ape hyoid bone, a possible marker of the evolution of speech. Human Biology, 85, 639–672.
Google Scholar
Steels, L. (2005). The emergence and evolution of linguistic structure: from lexical to grammatical communication systems. Connection Science, 17, 213–230.
Google Scholar
Steels, L. (2012). Introduction. Self-organization and selection in cultural language evolution. In L. Steels (Ed.), Experiments in cultural language evolution (pp. 1–37). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Google Scholar
Stout, D. (2002). Skill and cognition in stone tool production. Current Anthropology, 43(5), 693–722.
Google Scholar
Stout, D. (2011). Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 366, 1050–1059.
Google Scholar
Stout, D., & Chaminade, T. (2012). Stone tools, language and the brain in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 367, 75–87.
Google Scholar
Stout, D., Quade, J., Semaw, S., Rogers, M. J., & Levin, N. E. (2005). Raw material selectivity of the earliest stone toolmakers at Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. Journal of Human Evolution, 48, 365–380.
Google Scholar
Stout, D., Hecht, E., Khreisheh, N., Bradley, B., & Chaminade, T. (2015). Cognitive demands of lower Paleolithic toolmaking. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121804.
Stout, D., Rogers, M. J., Jaeggi, A. V., & Semaw, S. (2019). Archaeology and the origins of human cumulative culture: a case study from the earliest Oldowan at Gona, Ethiopia. Current Anthropology. https://doi.org/10.1086/703173.
Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Terrace, H.S. (2017). In the beginning: a review of Robert C. Berwick and Noam Chomsky’s Why only us. Journal of Language Evolution, 2(2), 114–125.
Tallerman, M., & Gibson, K. R. (2012). Introduction: the evolution of language. In K. R. Gibson & M. Tallerman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution (pp. 1–33). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Tayanin, D., & Lindell, K. (2012). Hunting and fishing in a Kammu village: revisiting a classic study in southeast Asian ethnography. Copenhagen: NIAS Press.
Google Scholar
Tennie, C., Braun, D. R., Premo, L. S., & McPherron, S. (2016). The island test for cumulative culture in the Paleolithic. In M. N. Haidle, N. J. Conard, & M. Bolus (Eds.), The nature of culture (pp. 121–133). Dordrecht: Springer.
Google Scholar
Tobias, P. V. (2005). Tools and brains: which came first? In F. d’Errico & L. Backwell (Eds.), From tools to symbols: from early hominids to modern humans (pp. 82–102). Johannesburg: Witwatersrand University Press.
Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2005). Constructing a language. A usage based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Boston: Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., & Delafield-Butt, J. T. (2013). Biology of shared experience and language development: regulations for the inter-subjective life of narratives. In M. Legerstee, D. Haley, & M. Bornstein (Eds.), The infant mind: origins of the social brain (pp. 167–199). New York: Guildford Press.
Google Scholar
Tyron, C., McBrearty, S., & Texier, P.-J. (2006). Levallois lithic technology from the Kapthurin formation, Kenya: Acheulian origin and middle stone age diversity. African Archaeological Review, 22, 199–229.
Google Scholar
Uomini, N., & Meyer, G. F. (2013). Shared brain lateralization patterns in language and Acheulean stone tool production: a functional transcranial doppler ultrasound study. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0072693.
Urban, G. (1988). Ritual wailing in Amerindian Brazil. American Anthropologist, 90(2), 385–400.
Google Scholar
van den Bergh, G. D., Kaifu, Y., Kurniawan, I., Kono, R. T., Brumm, A., Setiyabudi, E., Aziz, F., & Morwood, M. J. (2016). Homo floresiensis-like fossils from the early middle Pleistocene of Flores. Nature, 534, 245–248.
Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., & Burkart, J. M. (2011). Social learning and evolution: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0304.
Villa, P., & Roebroeks, W. (2014). Neandertal demise: an archaeological analysis of the modern human superiority complex. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096424.
Vonk, J., Jett, S. E., Mosteller, K. W., & Galvan, M. (2013). Natural category discrimination in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) at three levels of abstraction. Learning & Behavior, 41, 271–284.
Google Scholar
Wadley, L. (2015). Those marvellous millennia: the middle stone age of southern Africa. Azania Archaeological Research in Africa, 50(2), 155–226.
Google Scholar
Wallace, A. R. (2009). Contributions to the theory of natural selection: a series of essays (Cambridge library collection - Darwin, evolution and genetics). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Wallis, N. J. (2013). The materiality of signs: enchainment and animacy in woodland southeastern north American pottery. American Antiquity, 78(2), 207–226.
Google Scholar
Watson, S. K., Townsend, S. W., Schel, A. M., Wilke, C., Wallace, E. K., Cheng, L., et al. (2015). Vocal learning in the functionally referential food grunts of chimpanzees. Current Biology, 25, 495–499.
Google Scholar
Watts, I., Chazan, M., & Wilkins, J. (2016). Early evidence for brilliant ritualized display: specularite use in the northern cape (South Africa) between ∼500 and ∼300 Ka. Current Anthropology, 57(3), 287–310.
Google Scholar
Welker, F., Ramos-Madrigal, J., Gutenbrunner, P., Mackie, M., Tiwary, S., Jersie-Christensen, R. R., et al. (2020). The dental proteome of Homo antecessor. Nature, 580. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2153-8.
West, D. E. (2018). The work of Peirce’s dicisign in representationalizing early deictic events. Semiotica. https://doi.org/10.1515/sem-2017-0042.
White, L. (1940). The symbol: the origin and basis of human behavior. Philosophy of Science, 7(4), 451–463.
Google Scholar
White, M. J. (1998). On the significance of Acheulean biface variability in southern Britain. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0079497X00002164.
White, M. J., & Foulds, F. W. F. (2018). Symmetry is its own reward: on the character and significance of Acheulean handaxe symmetry in the middle Pleistocene. Antiquity, 92(362), 304–319.
Google Scholar
White, R., Bosinski, G., Bourrillon, R., Clottes, J., Conkey, M. W., Rodriguez, S. C., et al. (2019). Still no archaeological evidence that Neanderthals created Iberian cave art. Journal of Human Evolution. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2019.102640.
Whiten, A. (2005). The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and humans. Nature, 437(1), 52–55.
Google Scholar
Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, B364. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0069.
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 299, 682–685.
Google Scholar
Wobst, M. H. (1978). The archaeo-athnology of hunter-gatherers or the tyranny of the ethnographic record in archaeology. American Antiquity, 43(2), 303–309.
Google Scholar
Wynn, T. (1993). Layers of thinking in tool behaviour. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language and cognition in human evolution (pp. 389–406). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Wynn, T. (2002). Archaeology and cognitive evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(3), 389–438.
Google Scholar
Wynn, T. (2017). Evolutionary cognitive archaeology. In T. Wynn & F. L. Coolidge (Eds.), Cognitive models in Palaeolithic archaeology (pp. 1–20). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Wynn, T., & Gowlett, J. A. J. (2018). The handaxe reconsidered. Evolutionary Anthropology, 28, 21–29.
Google Scholar
Wynn, T., Haidle, M. N., Lombard, M., & Coolidge, F. L. (2017). The expert cognition model in human evolutionary studies. In T. Wynn & F. L. Coolidge (Eds.), Cognitive models in Paleolithic archaeology (pp. 21–44). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Yip, M. (2002). Tone. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Zilhão, J. (2019). Tar adhesives, Neandertals, and the tyranny of the discontinuous mind. PNAS, 116(44), 21966–21968.
Google Scholar