Skip to main content
Log in

A Cross-Cultural Examination of Blatant and Subtle Dehumanization of Autistic People

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Purpose

This cross-cultural study examined various domains of dehumanization, including both blatant (viewing autistic people as animal-like, child-like, or machine-like) and subtle (denying agency and experience capabilities) dehumanization, of autistic individuals by Koreans and Americans.

Methods

A total of 404 Koreans and 229 Americans participated in an online survey, assessing blatant and subtle dehumanization, knowledge about autism, stigma toward and contact with autistic people, cultural factors, and demographic information. Robust linear mixed-effects regressions were conducted to examine the impact of the target group (autistic vs. non-autistic) and the country (South Korea vs. the US) on dehumanization. Additionally, correlations and multiple regressions were employed to identify individual variables associated with dehumanization.

Results

Both Koreans and Americans exhibited more dehumanizing attitudes towards autistic individuals than non-autistic individuals across all domains. Koreans showed greater dehumanization of autistic individuals than Americans in all domains except for the machine-like domain. Stigma toward autistic people was associated with all dehumanization domains among Koreans and with some of the domains among Americans. Individual variables associated with dehumanization varied across countries and domains. Positive contact quality frequently predicted lower dehumanization in both cultures.

Conclusions

Non-autistic individuals consistently rated autistic people as less human than non-autistic people. Future research examining how autistic characteristics or societal perceptions that influence the consideration of an autistic person’s humanness vary across cultures is needed. Implementing interventions aimed at enhancing non-autistic people’s understanding of autistic individuals’ agency and experience capabilities and promoting high-quality contact opportunities with autistic individuals may help reduce dehumanizing attitudes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We used identity-first language (e.g., ‘autistic individuals’) to align with the preference of many self-advocates in the autistic community (Bottema-Beutel et al., 2018). We recognize the ongoing discourse about language choices and acknowledge that not everyone in the autistic community may endorse identity-first language.

  2. We did not utilize repeated-measure multivariate analysis of covariance (MANOVA) because our research focus was on understanding different associations of each predictor with various dehumanization domains, rather than examining the overall effect of a variable across different domains of dehumanization. Also, we assessed the assumptions related to MANOVA and found that Levene's test, commonly used for evaluating variance homogeneity, indicated violations of the assumption of equal variances across domains. Therefore, we chose to utilize robust linear mixed-effects regression analysis, which can account for unequal variances.

  3. We did not conduct multivariate regression because initial zero-order correlations revealed that different sets of variables were correlated with different domains of dehumanization, and the included predictors varied across dehumanization domains.

  4. We conducted exploratory follow-up analyses to examine which subscales within the essentialism index are correlated with or predict dehumanization domains. There were no consistent patterns regarding which specific subscale was associated with dehumanization domains, and none of the subscale scores significantly predicted dehumanization domains.

References

  • Aberson, C. L., & Gaffney, A. M. (2008). An integrated threat model of explicit and implicit attitudes. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 808–830.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bastian, B., & Haslam. (2006). Psychological essentialism and stereotype endorsement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 228–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogardus, E. S. (1933). A social distance scale. Sociology & Social Research, 17, 265–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bostyn, D. H., Roets, A., & van Hiel, A. (2016). Right-wing attitudes and moral cognition: Are right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation related to utilitarian judgment? Personality and Individual Differences, 96, 164–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botha, M. (2021). Academic, activist, or advocate? Angry, entangled, and emerging: A critical reflection on autism knowledge production. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 727542.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Botha, M., & Cage, E. (2022). “Autism research is in crisis”: A mixed method study of researcher’s constructions of autistic people and autism research. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1050897.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Botha, M., & Frost, D. M. (2020). Extending the minority stress model to understand mental health problems experienced by the autistic population. Society and Mental Health, 10(1), 20–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bottema-Beutel, K., Kim, S. Y., & Miele, D. B. (2018). College students’ evaluations and reasoning about exclusion of students with autism and learning disability: Context and goals may matter more than contact. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(1), 307–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boysen, G. A., Isaacs, R. A., Tretter, L., & Markowski, S. (2020). Evidence for blatant dehumanization of mental illness and its relation to stigma. The Journal of Social Psychology, 160(3), 346–356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boysen, G. A., Tchaikovsky, R. L., & Delmore, E. E. (2023). Dehumanization of mental illness and the stereotype content model. Stigma and Health, 8(2), 150–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruneau, E., Szekeres, H., Kteily, N., Tropp, L. R., & Kende, A. (2020). Beyond dislike: Blatant dehumanization predicts teacher discrimination. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 23(4), 560–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, E. M., & Scofield, J. E. (2018). Methods to detect low quality data and its implication for psychological research. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 2586–2596.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cage, E., Di Monaco, J., & Newell, V. (2019). Understanding, attitudes and dehumanisation towards autistic people. Autism, 23(6), 1373–1383.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Capozza, D., Falvo, R., Boin, J., & Colledani, D. (2016). Dehumanization in medical contexts: An expanding research field. TPM-Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology, 23(4), 545–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costello, K., & Hodson, G. (2010). Exploring the roots of dehumanization: The role of animal—human similarity in promoting immigrant humanization. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13(1), 3–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costello, K., & Hodson, G. (2014). Explaining dehumanization among children: The interspecies model of prejudice. British Journal of Social Psychology, 53(1), 175–197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cumings, B. (2005). Korea’s place in the sun: A modern history (updated). Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draasima, D. (2009). Stereotypes of autism. Philosophical Transaction of the Royal Society of London. Series b, Biological Science, 364(1522), 1475–1480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N., & Waytz, A. (2010). Mind perception. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindsay (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (5th ed., pp. 498–541). Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowers, B. J. (2010). Instrumentalism and psychology: Beyond using and being used. Theory & Psychology, 20, 102–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardiner, E., & Iarocci, G. (2014). Students with autism spectrum disorder in the university context: Peer acceptance predicts intention to volunteer. Journal of Autism andDevelopmental Disorders, 44(5), 1008–1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., & De Houwer, J. (2014). Implicit measures in social and personality psychology. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., Duan, L., Almaliach, A., Ang, S., Arnadottir, J., Aycan, Z., Boehnke, K., Boski, P., Cabecinhas, R., Chan, D., Chhokar, J., D’Amato, A., Ferrer, M., & Fischlmayr, C. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332(6033), 1100–1104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gillespie-Lynch, K., Daou, N., Sanchez-Ruiz, M., Kapp, S. K., Obeid, R., Brooks, P. J., Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (2022). If you want to develop an effective autism training, ask autistic students. Autism, 26(5), 1082–1094.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gillespie-Lynch, K., Daou, N., Sanchez-Ruiz, M., Kapp, S. K., Obeid, R., Brooks, P. J., Someki, F., Silton, N., & Abi-Habib, R. (2019). Factors underlying cross-cultural differences in stigma toward autism among college students in Lebanon and the United States. Autism, 23(8), 1993–2006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goff, P. A., Eberhardt, J. L., Williams, M. J., & Jackson, M. C. (2008). Not yet human: Implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contemporary consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(2), 292–306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1990). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity (3rd ed.). Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, H. M., Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Dimensions of mind perception. Science, 315, 619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grinker, R. R., & Cho, K. (2013). Border children: Interpreting autism spectrum disorder in South Korea. Journal of the Society for Psychological Anthropology, 41(1), 46–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, N. (2006). Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(3), 252–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, N. (2021). The social psychology of dehumanization. Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, N., & Loughnan, S. (2014). Dehumanization and infrahumanization. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 399–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hinshaw, S. P., & Chiccetti, D. (2000). Stigma and mental disorder: Conceptions of illness, public attitudes, personal disclosure, and social policy. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 555–598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holtgraves, T. (2004). Social desirability and self-reports: Testing models of socially desirable responding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(2), 161–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holtz, P., & Wagner, W. (2009). Essentialism and attribution of monstrosity in racist discourse: Right-wing internet postings about Africans and Jews. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 19(6), 411–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, A. J., Weikum, B. A., & Dyck, H. L. (2011). Psychological essentialism and its association with stigmatization. Personality and Individual Differences, 59(1), 95–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Islam, M. R., & Hewstone, M. (1993). Dimensions of contact as predictors of intergroup anxiety, perceived out-group variability, and out-group attitude: An integrative model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 700–710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, J. C., Gelfand, M. J., & Ember, C. R. (2020). The cultural evolutionary trade-off of ritualistic synchrony. Proceedings of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 287, 20201036.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, T. D., & Joshi, A. (2016). Dark clouds or silver linings? A stigma threat perspective on the implications of an autism diagnosis for workplace well-being. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101(3), 430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, S. C. (2022). Hey look, I’m (not) on TV: autistic people reflect on autism portrayals in entertainment media. Disability & Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S. Y., & Gillespie-Lynch, K. (2022). Do autistic people’s support needs and non-autistic people’s support for the neurodiversity movement contribute to heightened autism stigma in South Korea vs. the U.S.? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Advance Online Publication.

  • Kim, S. Y., Cheon, J. E., Gillespie-Lynch, K., Grandits, J., & Kim, Y.-H. (2022). Explicit stigma and implicit biases toward autism in South Korea versus the United States. Autism, 27(5), 1492–1507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S. Y., Cheon, J. E., Gillespie-Lynch, K., & Kim, Y. H. (2021). Is autism stigma higher in South Korea than the United States? Examining cultural tightness, intergroup bias, and concerns about heredity as contributors to heightened autism stigma. Autism, 26(2), 460–472.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S. Y., Song, D.-Y., Bottema-Beutel, K., Gillespie-Lynch, K., & Cage, E. (2023). A systematic review and meta-analysis of associations between primarily non-autistic people’s characteristics and attitudes toward autistic people. Autism Research, 16(2), 441–457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim-Rupnow, W. (2005). Disability and Korean culture. In J. H. Stone (Ed.), Culture and disability: Providing culturally competent services (pp. 115–138). SAGE.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Krzyzanowski, D. J., Howell, A. J., & Passmore, H.-A. (2019). Predictors and causes of the use of noun-based mental disorder labels. Stigma and Health, 4, 86–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kteily, N. S., & Bruneau, E. (2017). Darker demons of our nature: The need to (re)focus attention on blatant forms of dehumanization. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 487–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kteily, N. S., Bruneau, E., Waytz, A., & Cotterill, S. (2015). The ascent of man: Theoretical and empirical evidence for blatant dehumanization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 901–931.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kteily, N., Hodson, G., & Bruneau, E. (2016). They see us as less than human: Metadehumanization predicts intergroup conflict via reciprocal dehumanization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, 343–370.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landry, A. P., Ihm, E., Protzko, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2022). Essentially subhuman: Psychological essentialism facilitates dehumanization. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peach Psychology, 28(2), 167–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leyens, J. P., Cortes, B., Demoulin, S., Dovidio, J. F., Fiske, S. T., Gaunt, R., Paladino, M.-P., Rodriguez-Perez, A., Rodriguez-Torres, R., & Vaes, J. (2003). Emotional prejudice, essentialism, and nationalism. The 2002 Tajfel Lecture. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33(6), 703–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leyens, J. P., Rodriguez, A. P., Rodriguez, R. T., Gaunt, R., Paladino, M. P., Vaes, J., & Demoulin, S. (2001). Psychological essentialism and the differential attribution of uniquely human emotions to in-groups and out-groups. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 395–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. C. (2001). Conceptualizing stigma. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 363–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martinez, A. G., Piff, P. K., Mendoza-Denton, R., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2011). The power of a label: Mental illness diagnoses, ascribed humanity, and social rejection. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30, 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milton, D. (2012). On the ontological status of autism: The ‘double empathy problem.’ Disability & Society, 27(6), 883–887.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obeid, R., Daou, N., DeNigris, D., Shane-Simpson, C., Brooks, P. J., & Gillespie-Lynch, K. (2015). A cross-cultural comparison of knowledge and stigma associated with autism spectrum disorder among college students in Lebanon and the United States. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(11), 3520–3536.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Over, H. (2020). Seven challenges for the dehumanization hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620902133

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical and assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Park, J., Haslam, J., & Kashima, Y. (2011). Relational to the core: Lay theories of humanness in Australia, Japan, and Korea. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(5), 774–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, L. R., Monteith, M. J., & South, S. C. (2020). Dehumanization, prejudice, and social policy beliefs concerning people with developmental disabilities. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 23(2), 262–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pepitone, A., & DiNubile, M. (1976). Contrast effects in judgments of crime severity and the punishment of criminal violators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33(4), 448–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pesko, C. D., Lei, R. F., Kunst, J. R., & Bruneau, E. (2021). Blatant dehumanization in the mind’s eye: Prevalent even among those who explicitly reject it? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 150(6), 1115–1131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pettigrew, T. F. (2008). Future directions for intergroup contact theory and research. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 32(3), 187–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasset, P., Montalan, B., Mauny, N., Boudemadi, V., & Mange, J. (2022). Not all ‘intouchable’: Variations in humanness perceptions between physical and mental disability. International Review of Social Psychology, 35(1), 7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, L., & Levy, S. R. (2010). The colorblind, multicultural, and polycultural ideological approaches to improving intergroup attitudes and relations. Social Issues and Policy Review, 4(1), 215–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., & Mescher, K. (2012). Of animals and objects: Men’s implicit dehumanization of women and likelihood of sexual aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(6), 734–746.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Someki, F., Silton, N., Abi-Habib, R., Gillespie-Lynch, K., Daou, N., Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., Kapp, S. K., Obeid, R., Brooks, P. J., Someki, F., Silton, N., & Abi-Habib, R. (2019). Factors underlying cross-cultural differences in stigma toward autism among college students in Lebanon and the United States. Autism, 23(8), 1993–2006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W. L. (1987). Cross-disciplinary perspectives on autism. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 12(4), 615–630.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. C. (1989). Self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. C., & Gelfland, M. J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 118–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • UCLA Advanced Research Computing Statistical Methods and Data Analytics. (n.d.). Regression with Stata Chapter 2 - Regression Diagnostic. Retrieved April 25, 2022, from https://stats.oarc.ucla.edu/stata/webbooks/reg/chapter2/stata-webbooksregressionwith-statachapter-2-regressiondiagnostics/

  • Utych, S. M. (2017). How dehumanization influences attitudes toward immigrants. Political Research Quarterly, 71(2), 440–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaes, J., Leyens, J.-P., Paladino, M. P., & Miranda, M. P. (2012). We are human, they are not: Driving forces behind outgroup dehumanization and humanization of the ingroup. European Review of Social Psychology, 23(1), 64–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, X., Chen, H., Shi, J., & Chen, Z. (2022). Threatened humanity in a tight world: Cultural tightness results in self-objectification. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 25(8), 2003–2020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wasserstein, R. L., Schirm, A. L., Lazar, N. A., Kuffner, T. A., & Walker, S. G. (2019). Why are P-values controversial? The American Statistician, 73(1), 1–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waytz, A., & Schroeder, J. (2014). Overlooking others: Dehumanization by commission and omission. TPM-Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology, 21(3), 251–266.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Yonsei Signature Cluster Program of 2023 (2023-22-0013).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation, data collection and analysis were performed by SYK and JEC. The first draft of the manuscript was written by SYK, and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Young-Hoon Kim.

Ethics declarations

Competing Interests

The authors have no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (XLSX 132 kb)

Supplementary file2 (DOCX 24 kb)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kim, S.Y., Cheon, J.E. & Kim, YH. A Cross-Cultural Examination of Blatant and Subtle Dehumanization of Autistic People. J Autism Dev Disord (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-023-06217-x

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-023-06217-x

Keywords

Navigation