Skip to main content
Log in

Does the Neurotypical Human Have a ‘Theory of Mind’?

  • Letter to the Editor
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Notes

  1. For the sake of simplicity, I treat these as mutually exclusive, as they are generally understood in relevant debates (although for options with key elements of both, see Ereshefsky, 1992; Haueis, 2021).

  2. I use the phrase ‘autistic people’, as is the preference of some autistic people, although I acknowledge that some people with autism prefer the phrase ‘people with autism’.

References

  • Abell, F., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cognitive Development, 15(1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. A. (2012). What is “theory of mind”? Concepts, cognitive processes, and individual differences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(5), 825–839.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2000a). The evolution of a theory of mind. In M. Corballis & S. E. G. Lea (Eds.), The descent of mind: Psychological perspectives on hominid evolution (pp. 261–277). Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2000b). Theory of mind and autism: A review. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23, 169–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Autism: The empathizing-systemizing (E-S) theory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156(1), 68–80. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04467.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beaudoin, C., Leblanc, É., Gagner, C., & Beauchamp, M. H. (2020). Systematic review and inventory of theory of mind measures for young children. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2905–2905. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02905

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bermúdez, J. L. (2020). Cognitive science: An introduction to the science of the mind (3rd ed.). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloechle, J., Huber, S., Klein, E., Bahnmueller, J., Moeller, K., & Rennig, J. (2018). Neuro-cognitive mechanisms of global Gestalt perception in visual quantification. NeuroImage, 181, 359–369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.026

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human. William Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crary, A. (2019). The horrific history of comparisons between cognitive disability and animality (and how to move past it). In L. Gruen & F. Probyn-Rapsey (Eds.), Animaladies: Gender, animals, and madness (pp. 117–136). Bloomsbury Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1976). Conditions of personhood. In A. O. Rorty (Ed.), The identities of persons. University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 568–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ereshefsky, M. (1992). Eliminative pluralism. Philosophy of Science, 59(4), 671–690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1999). Theory of mind and self-consciousness: What is it like to be autistic? Mind & Language, 14(1), 82–89. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, S. N., Mclanachan, A., & Critchley, H. D. (2017). Interoceptive training for anxiety management in autism: Aligning dimension of interoceptive experience. Psychosomatic Med., 79, A100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, S. N., Tiley, C., O’Keeffe, S., Harrison, N., & Seth, A. (2016). Discrepancies between dimensions of interoception in autism: Implications for emotion and anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 114, 117–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A., & Yergeau, M. (2019). Empirical failures of the claim that autistic people lack a theory of mind. Archives of Scientific Psychology, 7(1), 102–118.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gori, S., Molteni, M., & Facoetti, A. (2016). Visual illusions: An interesting tool to investigate developmental dyslexia and autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 175. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00175

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2020). Annual research review: Looking back to look forward-changes in the concept of autism and implications for future research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(3), 218–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harman, G. (1978). Studying the Chimpanzee’s theory of mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 576–577. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076743

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haueis, P. (2021). The death of the cortical column? Patchwork structure and conceptual retirement in neuroscientific practice. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A, 85, 101–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2014). Submentalizing: I am not really reading your mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 131–143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Higashida, N., Yoshida, K. A., & Mitchell, D. (2016). The reason I jump: The inner voice of a thirteen-year-old boy with autism. Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huberle, E., & Karnath, H. O. (2012). The role of temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) in global Gestalt perception. Brain Structure & Function, 217(3), 735–746. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-011-0369-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jack, A. I., & Robbins, P. (2012). The phenomenal stance revisited. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 3(3), 383–403. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-012-0104-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karvelis, P., Seitz, A. R., Lawrie, S. M., & Seriès, P. (2018). Autistic traits, but not schizotypy, predict increased weighting of sensory information in Bayesian visual integration. eLife, 7, e34115.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lurz, R. (2011). Mindreading animals: The debate over what animals know about other minds. MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Neto, C. (2020). When imprecision is a good thing, or how imprecise concepts facilitate integration in biology. Biology and Philosophy, 35(6), 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ogawa, S., Iriguchi, M., Lee, Y.-A., Yoshikawa, S., & Goto, Y. (2019). Atypical social rank recognition in autism spectrum disorder. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 15657. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-52211-8

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Penn, D. C., & Povinelli, D. J. (2007). On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a “theory of mind.” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 362, 731–744.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2003). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? The Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rennig, J., Bilalic, M., Huberle, E., Karnath, H. O., & Himmelbach, M. (2013). The temporo-parietal junction contributes to global gestalt perception-evidence from studies in chess experts. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 513. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00513

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, P., & Jack, A. I. (2006). The phenomenal stance. Philosophical Studies, 127(1), 59–85. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098-005-1730-x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, C. E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2017). Sensory perception in autism. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(11), 671–684. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.112

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R. (2009). Theory of mind (neural basis). In W. P. Banks (Ed.), Encyclopedia of consciousness (pp. 401–409). Elsevier, Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R. (2010). The right temporo-parietal junction: A specific brain region for thinking about thoughts. In E. Margolis, R. Samuels, & S. P. Stich (Eds.), Handbook of theory of mind (pp. 1–35). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaafsma, S. M., Pfaff, D. W., Spunt, R. P., & Adolphs, R. (2015). Deconstructing and reconstructing theory of mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(2), 65–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.007

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schuwerk, T., Schurz, M., Muller, F., Rupprecht, R., & Sommer, M. (2017). The rTPJ’s overarching cognitive function in networks for attention and theory of mind. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 157–168. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw163

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, H., & Vickers, P. (2017). Conceptual fragmentation and the rise of eliminativism. European Journal for Philosophy of Science, 7(1), 17–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walter, E., Dassonville, P., & Bochslet, T. M. (2009). A specific autistic trait that modulates visuospatial illusion susceptibility. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(2), 339–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yergeau, M., & Huebner, B. (2017). Minding theory of mind. Journal of Social Philosophy, 48(3), 273–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The author confirms sole responsibility for the conception and preparation of this manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joe Gough.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gough, J. Does the Neurotypical Human Have a ‘Theory of Mind’?. J Autism Dev Disord 53, 853–857 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05381-2

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05381-2

Navigation