Skip to main content
Log in

Can Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Learn New Vocabulary From Linguistic Context?

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated whether children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) can learn vocabulary from linguistic context. Thirty-five children with ASD (18 with age-appropriate structural language; 17 with language impairment [ALI]) and 29 typically developing peers were taught 20 Science words. Half were presented in linguistic context from which meaning could be inferred, whilst half were accompanied by an explicit definition. Children with ASD were able to learn from context. Condition did not influence phonological learning, but receptive semantic knowledge was greatest in the context condition, and expressive semantic knowledge greatest in the definitional condition. The ALI group learnt less than their peers. This suggests that at least some vocabulary should be taught explicitly, and children with ALI may need additional tuition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, C., Clarke, E., & Haynes, R. (2009). Inference and sentence comprehension in children with specific or pragmatic language impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44(3), 301–318. doi:10.1080/13682820802051788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychological Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-V). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Armbruster, B., Lehr, F., & Osborn, J. (2010). Put reading first: The research building blocks for teaching children to read. In R. Adler (Ed.), NICHD (3rd ed.). Rockville: NICHD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D., & Adams, C. (1992). Comprehension problems in children with specific language impairment: Literal and inferential meaning. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35(1), 119–129. doi:10.1044/jshr.3501.119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Autism, primary pragmatic difficulties, and specific language impairment: can we distinguish them using psycholinguistic markers? Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45(8), 515–524. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2003.tb00951.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, H., Weighall, A., Henderson, L., & Gaskell, G. (2012). Enhanced recognition and recall of new words in 7- and 12-year-olds following a period of offline consolidation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(1), 56–72. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.11.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Lemmon, K. (2004). Individual differences in the inference of word meanings from context: The influence of reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(4), 671–681. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741–748. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00770.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crais, E., & Chapman, R. (1987). Story recall and inferencing skills in language/learning-disabled and nondisabled children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52(1), 50–55. doi:10.1044/jshd.5201.50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Department for Education. (2015). Special educational needs in England: January 2015 Statistics: Special educational needs.

  • Dockrell, J., Messer, D., George, R., & Ralli, A. (2003). Beyond naming patterns in children with WFDs - definitions for nouns and verbs. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16(2–3), 191–211. doi:10.1016/S0911-6044(02)00012-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dodwell, K., & Bavin, E. (2008). Children with specific language impairment: an investigation of their narratives and memory. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(2), 201–218. doi:10.1080/13682820701366147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis Weismer, S. (1985). Constructive comprehension abilities exhibited by language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28(2), 175–184. doi:10.1044/jshr.2802.175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feinstein, L., & Duckworth, K. (2006). Development in the early years: Its importance for school performance and adult outcomes. Wider Benefits of Learning Research Report (Vol. 20). London: Center for Research on the Wider Benefits of Learning, Institute of Education

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, M. (1990). Receptive one-word picture vocabulary test (revised): Manual. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodlin-Jones, B., Tang, K., Liu, J., & Thomas, F. (2008). Sleep patterns in preschool-age children with autism, developmental delay, and typical development. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 930–938. doi:10.1097/CHI.ObO13e3181799f7c.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L., Powell, A., Gaskell, G., & Norbury, C. (2014). Learning and consolidation of new vocabulary in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Science, 17(6), 858–871. doi:10.1111/desc.12169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L., Weighall, A., Brown, H., & Gaskell, G. (2012). Consolidation of vocabulary is associated with sleep in children. Developmental Science, 15(5), 674–687. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01172.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L., Weighall, A., & Gaskell, G. (2013). Learning new vocabulary during childhood: Effects of semantic training on lexical consolidation and integration. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(3), 572–592. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J., Stein, M., & Wysocki, K. (1984). Learning vocabulary through reading. American Educational Research Journal, 21(4), 767–787. doi:10.3102/00028312021004767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of central coherence theory: linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome: is local coherence impaired? Cognition, 71(2), 149–185. doi:10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00022-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kan, P., & Windsor, J. (2010). Word learning in children with primary language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(3), 739–756. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0248).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karasinski, C., & Weismer, S. (2010). Comprehension of inferences in discourse processing by adolescents with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 1268–1279. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2009/09-0006).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kjelgaard, M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2–3), 287–308. doi:10.1080/01690960042000058.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lindgren, K., Folstein, S., Tomblin, J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment and their first-degree relatives. Autism Research, 2(1), 22–38. doi:10.1002/aur.63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Loucas, T., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Chandler, S., Meldrum, D., & Baird, G. (2008). Autistic symptomatology and language ability in autism spectrum disorder and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(11), 1184–1192. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01951.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R., & Norbury, C. (2014). Orthography facilitates vocabulary learning for children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(7), 1317–1334. doi:10.1080/17470218.2013.859714.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R., & Norbury, C. (2015). Making inferences from text: It’s vocabulary that matters. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 1224–1232. doi:10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0330.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, K., Newman, R., Reilly, R., & Capone, N. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 998–1014. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2002/081).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, K., Rost, G., Arenas, R., Farris-Trimble, A., & Stiles, D. (2013). Children with ASD can use gaze in support of word recognition and learning. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 54(7), 745–753. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mengoni, S., Nash, H., & Hulme, C. (2013). The benefit of orthographic support for oral vocabulary learning in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Child Language, 40(1), 221–243. doi:10.1017/S0305000912000396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, P., Farkas, G., Hillemeier, M., Hammer, C., & Maczuga, S. (2015). 24-month-old children with larger oral vocabularies display greater academic and behavioral functioning at kindergarten entry. Child Development, 86(5), 1351–1370. doi:10.1111/cdev.12398.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W., & Townsend, D. (2012). Words as tools: Learning academic vocabulary as language acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 47(1), 91–108. doi:10.1002/RRQ.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nash, H., & Snowling, M. (2006). Teaching new words to children with poor existing vocabulary knowledge: a controlled evaluation of the definition and context methods. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(3), 335–354. doi:10.1080/13682820600602295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nash, M., & Donaldson, M. (2005). Word learning in children with vocabulary deficits. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(2), 439–458. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2005/030).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C., & Bishop, D. (2002). Inferential processing and story recall in children with communication problems: A comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 37(3), 227–251. doi:10.1080/13682820210136269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K. (2010). Sound before meaning: Word learning in autistic disorders. Neuropsychologia, 48(14), 4012–4019. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C., & Nation, K. (2011). Understanding variability in reading comprehension in adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Interactions with language status and decoding skill. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(3), 191–210. doi:10.1080/10888431003623553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potts, G., John, M., & Kirson, D. (1989). Incorporating new information into existing world knowledge. Cognitive Psychology, 21(3), 303–333. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(89)90011-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richdale, A. (1999). Sleep problems in autism: prevalence, cause, and intervention. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 41(1), 60–66. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.1999.tb00012.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riches, N., Loucas, T., Charman, T., Simonoff, E., & Baird, G. (2010). Sentence repetition in children with specific language impairment and autism: A study of linguistic factors affecting recall. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 45, 47–60. doi:10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ricketts, J., Bishop, D. V., & Nation, K. (2009). Orthographic facilitation in oral vocabulary acquisition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(10), 1948–1966. doi:10.1080/17470210802696104.

  • Ricketts, J., Dockrell, J., Patel, N., Charman, T., & Lindsay, G. (2015). Do children with specific language impairment and autism spectrum disorders benefit from the presence of orthography when learning new spoken words? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 134(0), 43–61. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.01.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saldaña, D., & Frith, U. (2007). Do readers with autism make bridging inferences from world knowledge? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96(4), 310–319. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2006.11.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (CELF-4 UK). London: Harcourt Assessment.

  • Simcock, G., & DeLoache, J. (2006). Get the picture? The effects of iconicity on toddlers’ reenactment from picture books. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1352–1357. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simcock, G., & DeLoache, J. (2008). The effect of repetition on infants’ imitation from picture books varying in iconicity. Infancy, 13(6), 687–697. doi:10.1080/15250000802459102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, S., & Fairbanks, M. (1986). The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model-based meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 56(1), 72–110. doi:10.2307/1170287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanborn, M., & de Glopper, K. (1999). Incidental word learning while reading: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 69(3), 261–285. doi:10.3102/00346543069003261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamminen, J., Payne, J., Stickgold, R., Wamsley, E., & Gaskell, M. G. (2010). Sleep spindle activity is associated with the integration of new memories and existing knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(43), 14356–14360. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3028-10.2010.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tare, M., Chiong, C., Ganea, P., & DeLoache, J. (2010). Less is more: How manipulative features affect children’s learning from picture books. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 395–400. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2010.06.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scales of intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assesment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G., Sears, L., & Allard, A. (2004). Sleep problems in children with autism. Journal of Sleep Research, 13(3), 265–268. doi:10.3102/00346543069003261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all of the families who took part in the study and the schools who supported the research. We would also like to thank the Experimental Psychology Society who provided an Undergraduate Research Bursary and we are grateful to Emily Crosby and Rhiannon Daffurn for their assistance with data collection.

Author contributions

CFN contributed to the design and coordination of the study, and helped to draft the manuscript. RL contributed to the design and coordination of the study, data collection and analyses, and drafted the manuscript. LT contributed to data collection and analyses, and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rebecca Lucas.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual parents/guardians of the children included in this study, and assent was obtained from all individual children in this study.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 3.

Table 3 Stimuli characteristics

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lucas, R., Thomas, L. & Norbury, C.F. Can Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Learn New Vocabulary From Linguistic Context?. J Autism Dev Disord 47, 2205–2216 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3151-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3151-z

Keywords

Navigation