Skip to main content
Log in

An Investigation of the “Jumping to Conclusions” Data-Gathering Bias and Paranoid Thoughts in Asperger Syndrome

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The existence of a data-gathering bias, in the form of jumping to conclusions, and links to paranoid ideation was investigated in Asperger syndrome (AS). People with AS (N = 30) were compared to a neurotypical control group (N = 30) on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes and the Beads tasks, with self-report measures of depression, general anxiety, social anxiety, self-consciousness and paranoid ideation. The AS group performed less well than the control group on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Task with regard to accuracy but responded more quickly and tended to make decisions on the basis of less evidence on the Beads Task with 50 % demonstrating a clear ‘jumping to conclusions bias’, whereas none of the control group showed such a bias. Depression and general anxiety were associated with paranoid ideation but not data-gathering style, which was contrary to expectation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Bayesian probability relies on Bayes' theorem, which has been used to explain attitude formation and related behaviour as it provides ‘a mathematical rule for deciding how prior opinion or choices should optimally be modified in the light of new evidence’ (Reber 1995).

References

  • Abell, F., & Hare, D. J. (2005). An experimental investigation of the phenomenology of delusional beliefs in people with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 9(5), 515–531.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, M. H., Lincoln, A. J., & Kaufman, A. S. (1991). Sequential and simultaneous processing abilities of high-functioning autistic and language-impaired children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21(4), 502–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arora, M., Praharaj, S. K., Sarkhel, S., & Sinha, V. K. (2011). Asperger disorder in adults. Southern Medical Journal, 104(4), 264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Attwood, T. (2008). The complete guide to Asperger’s syndrome. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. E., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001a). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 241–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. E., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001b). The autism spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, V., Halligan, P. W., & Ellis, H. D. (2006). Explaining delusions: A cognitive perspective. Trends in cognitive sciences, 10(5), 219–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bentall, R. P., Corcoran, R., Howard, R., Blackwood, N., & Kinderman, P. (2001). Persecutory delusions: A review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(8), 1143–1192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bentall, R. P., Kinderman, P., & Kaney, S. (1994). The self, attributional processes and abnormal beliefs: Towards a model of persecutory delusions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(3), 331–341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bentall, R. P., & Swarbrick, R. (2003). The best laid schemas of paranoid patients: Autonomy, sociotropy and need for closure. Psychology and Psychotherapy, 76(2), 163–171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blackshaw, A. J., Kinderman, P., Hare, D. J., & Hatton, C. (2001). Theory of mind, causal attribution and paranoia in Asperger syndrome. Autism, 5(2), 147–163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bramham, J., Ambery, F., Young, S., Morris, R., Russell, A., Xenitidis, K., et al. (2009). Executive functioning differences between adults with attention deficit strategy formation hyperactivity disorder and autistic spectrum disorder in initiation, planning and strategy formation. Autism, 13(3), 245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broome, M. R., Johns, L. C., Valli, I., Woolley, J. B., Tabraham, P., Brett, C., et al. (2007). Delusion formation and reasoning biases in those at clinical high risk for psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 191, 38–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, D., Baxter, M., Perry, D., & Prasher, V. (1999). The diagnosis of affective and psychotic disorders in adults with autism: Seven case reports. Autism, 3(2), 149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, D., Littlejohns, C. S., Corbett, J. A., & Joseph, S. (1989). Pervasive developmental disorders and psychoses in adult life. British Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 692–699.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colbert, S. M., & Peters, E. R. (2002). Need for closure and jumping-to-conclusions in delusion-prone individuals. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190(1), 27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Constantine, Corbett., Rocke, Hendren., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Research, 166(2–3), 210–222.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, C. R., Bollini, A. M., Graham, B. G., Keefe, R. S. E., Schiffman, S. S., & McEvoy, J. P. (2002). Sensory acuity and reasoning in delusional disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 43(3), 175–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corden, B., Chilvers, R., & Skuse, D. (2008). Avoidance of emotionally-arousing stimuli predicts social-perceptual impairment in Asperger’s syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 46(1), 137–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craig, J. S., Hatton, C., Craig, F. B., & Bentall, R. P. (2004). Persecutory beliefs, attributions and theory of mind: Comparison of patients with paranoid delusions, Asperger’s syndrome and healthy controls. Schizophrenia Research, 69(1), 29–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darr, G. C., & Worden, F. G. (1951). Case report twenty-eight years after an infantile autistic disorder. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 21(3), 559–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Docter, M., & Naqvi, S. (2010). Asperger’s syndrome: A guide to helping your child thrive at home and at school. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dossetor, D. R. (2007). ‘All that glitters is not gold’: Misdiagnosis of psychosis in pervasive developmental disorders—a case series. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 12(4), 537–548.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dudley, R. E., John, C. H., Young, A. W., & Over, D. E. (1997a). The effect of self-referent material on the reasoning of people with delusions. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36(4), 575–584.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dudley, R. E., John, C. H., Young, A. W., & Over, D. E. (1997b). Normal and abnormal reasoning in people with delusions. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36(2), 243–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dudley, R. E., & Over, D. E. (2003). People with delusions jump to conclusions: A theoretical account of research findings on the reasoning of people with delusions. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 10(5), 263–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fear, C. F., & Healy, D. (1997). Probabilistic reasoning in obsessive-compulsive and delusional disorders. Psychological Medicine, 27(01), 199–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenigstein, A., & Vanable, P. A. (1992). Paranoia and self-consciousness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(1), 129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fine, C., Gardner, M., Craigie, J., & Gold, I. (2007). Hopping, skipping or jumping to conclusions? Clarifying the role of the JTC bias in delusions. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 12(1), 46–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzgerald, M., & Corvin, A. (2001). Diagnosis and differential diagnosis of Asperger syndrome. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 7(4), 310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (2004). Emanuel Miller lecture: Confusions and controversies about Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(4), 672–686.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garety, P. A., & Freeman, D. (1999). Cognitive approaches to delusions: A critical review of theories and evidence. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38(2), 113–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garety, P. A., Freeman, D., Jolley, S., Dunn, G., Bebbington, P. E., Fowler, D. G., et al. (2005). Reasoning, emotions, and delusional conviction in psychosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(3), 373–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garety, P. A., & Hemsley, D. R. (1994). Delusions: Investigations into the psychology of delusional reasoning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garety, P. A., Hemsley, D. R., & Wessely, S. (1991). Reasoning in deluded schizophrenic and paranoid patients. Biases in performance on a probabilistic inference task. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 179(4), 194–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, C., Freeman, D., Kuipers, E., Bebbington, P., Fowler, D., Dunn, G., et al. (2008). Measuring ideas of persecution and social reference: The Green et al. paranoid thought scales (GPTS). Psychological Medicine, 38(01), 101–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gweon, H., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Schulz, L. E. (2010). Infants consider both the sample and the sampling process in inductive generalization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(20), 9066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haddock, G., McCarron, J., Tarrier, N., & Faragher, E. (1999). Scales to measure dimensions of hallucinations and delusions: The psychotic symptom rating scales (PSYRATS). Psychological Medicine, 29, 879–889.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, R., Chelune, G., Talley, J., Kay, G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huq, S. F., Garety, P. A., & Hemsley, D. R. (1988). Probabilistic judgements in deluded and non-deluded subjects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40(4), 801–812.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kapur, S. (2003). Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(1), 13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linney, Y. M., Peters, E. R., & Ayton, P. (1998). Reasoning biases in delusion-prone individuals. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37(3), 285–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Menon, M., Pomarol-Clotet, E., McKenna, P. J., & McCarthy, R. A. (2006). Probabilistic reasoning in schizophrenia: A comparison of the performance of deluded and nondeluded schizophrenic patients and exploration of possible cognitive underpinnings. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 11(6), 521–536.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Menon, M., Woodward, T. S., Pomarol-Clotet, E., McKenna, P. J., & McCarthy, R. (2005). Heightened stimulus salience renders deluded schizophrenics less susceptible to the ‘famous names illusion’. Schizophrenia Research, 80(2–3), 369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meraj, U., & Hare, D. J. (2004). Role of daily life stressing mediating anxiety and delusions of grandeur in people with Asperger Syndrome. Paper presented at the Poster presented at 34th Annual Meeting of the European Association for Cognitive and Behavioural Psychotherapy

  • Moritz, S., & Woodward, T. S. (2005). Jumping to conclusions in delusional and non delusional schizophrenic patients. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(2), 193–207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, E. R., & Garety, P. (2006). Cognitive functioning in delusions: A longitudinal analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(4), 481–514.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, E. R., Joseph, S. A., & Garety, P. A. (1999). Measurement of delusional ideation in the normal population: introducing the PDI (Peters et al. delusions inventory). Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25(3), 553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, E. R., Thornton, P., Siskou, L., Linney, Y., & MacCabe, J. (2006). Specificity and origins of the” jump-to-conclusions” bias in deluded patients. Schizophrenia Research, 81, 129–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, L. D., & Edwards, W. (1966). Conservatism in a simple probability inference task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(3), 346.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raja, M., & Azzoni, A. (2001). Asperger’s disorder in the emergency psychiatric setting. General Hospital Psychiatry, 23(5), 285–293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raja, M., & Azzoni, A. (2007). Thought disorder in Asperger syndrome and schizophrenia: Issues in the differential diagnosis. A series of case reports. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 10(4-3), 944–952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1995). The penguin dictionary of psychology (2nd ed.). Harmsworth: Penguin Books.

  • Sasson, N., Tsuchiya, N., Hurley, R., Couture, S. M., Penn, D. L., Adolphs, R., et al. (2007). Orienting to social stimuli differentiates social cognitive impairment in autism and schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia, 45(11), 2580–2588.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, S. D., Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Spatial working memory deficits in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 605–612.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G., Bremner, R., Bond, S., & Rich, S. (1989). A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(2), 213–225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dael, F., Versmissen, D., Janssen, I., Myin-Germeys, I., van Os, J., & Krabbendam, L. (2006). Data gathering: Biased in psychosis? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(2), 341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warman, D. M., Lysaker, P. H., Martin, J. M., Davis, L., & Haudenschield, S. L. (2007). Jumping to conclusions and the continuum of delusional beliefs. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(6), 1255–1269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

  • Wing, L. (1981). Asperger’s syndrome: A clinical account. Psychological Medicine, 11(1), 115–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L. (1996). The autistic spectrum. A guide for parents and professionals. London: Constable.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodbury-Smith, M. R., Boyd, K., & Szatmari, P. (2010). Autism spectrum disorders, schizophrenia and diagnostic confusion. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 35(5), 360.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodbury-Smith, M. R., Robinson, J., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. E. (2005). Screening adults for Asperger Syndrome using the AQ: A preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical practice. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3), 331–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F., & Garcia, V. (2008). Intuitive statistics by 8-month-old infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(13), 5012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, H. F., & Bentall, R. P. (1995). Hypothesis testing in patients with persecutory delusions: Comparison with depressed and normal subjects. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34(3), 353–369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Young, H. F., & Bentall, R. P. (1997). Probabilistic reasoning in deluded, depressed and normal subjects: Effects of task difficulty and meaningful versus non-meaningful material. Psychological Medicine, 27(2), 455–465.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

The authors would like to thank the participants.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dougal Julian Hare.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Jänsch, C., Hare, D.J. An Investigation of the “Jumping to Conclusions” Data-Gathering Bias and Paranoid Thoughts in Asperger Syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 44, 111–119 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1855-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1855-2

Keywords

Navigation