Skip to main content
Log in

(Re-)conceptualisation in Asperger’s Syndrome and Typical Individuals with Varying Degrees of Autistic-like Traits

  • Original paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The abilities to form new concepts from scratch (conceptualisation), and to flexibly switch from one concept to another (re-conceptualisation), were investigated in adults with Asperger’s Syndrome and in typically-developed adults with low and high autism spectrum quotients. In consecutively presented morphs, containing increasing percentages of animate or inanimate objects, the emerging objects had to be identified. The abilities to conceptualise and reconceptualise became increasingly impaired with increasing autistic(-like) traits. Across both tasks, all groups recognised animate objects quicker than inanimate objects. However, this ‘animate advantage’ was differently affected by the two tasks. In the Reconceptualisation task, the ‘animate advantage’ gradually disappeared with increasing autistic(-like) traits, whereas in the Conceptualisation task it remained present.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adolphs, R. (1999). Social cognition and the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 469–479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 267–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Almeida, R. A., Dickinson, J. E., Maybery, M. T., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2010). A new step towards understanding embedded figures test performance in the autism spectrum: The radial frequency search task. Neuropsychologia, 48, 374–381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). The empathising system: A revision of the 1994 model of the mindreading system. In B. Ellis & D. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind. Guilford: Guilford Publications Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., & Swettenham, J. (1997). Theory of mind in autism: Its relationship to executive functioning and central coherence. Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, S., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger’s Syndrome/high functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayliss, A. P., Pellegrino, G., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Sex differences in eye gaze and symbolic cueing of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 631–650.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Gaze and arrow cueing of attention reveals individual differences along the autism-spectrum as a function of target context. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 95–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchamp, M. S., Lee, K. E., Haxby, J. V., & Martin, A. (2002). Parallel visual motions processing streams for manipulable objects and human movements. Neuron, 34, 149–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J., Frith, U., Smith, N., Abel, F., & Cipolotti, L. (2001). Fractionation of visual memory: Agency detection and its impairment in autism. Neuropsychologia, 40, 108–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S. J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 561–567.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 843–859.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowler, D. M., & Thommen, E. (2000). Attribution of mechanical and social causality to animated displays by children with autism. Autism, 4, 147–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brothers, L. (1990). The social brain: A project for integrating primate behavior and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts in Neuroscience, 1, 27–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. (1967). A study of thinking. New York: Science Editions.

  • Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger Syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cesaroni, L., & Garber, M. (1991). Exploring the experience of autism through first-hand accounts. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 201–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cipolotti, L., Robinson, G., Blair, J., & Frith, U. (1999). Fractionation of visual memory: Evidence from a case with multiple neurodevelopmental impairments. Neuropsychologia, 37, 455–465.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A. J., et al. (1994). Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebella patients. Behavioural Neuroscience, 108, 848–865.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Winter, J., & Wagemans, J. (2004). Contour-based object identification and segmentation: Stimuli, norms and data, and software tools. Behaviour Research Methods Instruments & Computers, 36, 604–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). The delis–kaplan executive function system. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downing, P. E., Bray, D., Rogers, J., & Childs, C. (2004). Bodies capture attention when nothing is expected. Cognition, 93, 27–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6, 178–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eylen, L. V., Boets, B., Steyaert, J., Evers, K., Wagemans, J., & Noens, I. (2011). Cognitive flexibility in autism spectrum disorder: Explaining the inconsistencies? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1390–1401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (2001). Mind blindness and the brain in autism. Neuron, 32, 969–979.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds: A biological basis. Science, 286(5445), 1692–1695.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., Morton, J., & Leslie, A. M. (1991). The cognitive basis of a biological disorder—autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 14, 433–438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fuggetta, G., Rizzo, S., Pobric, G., Lavidor, M., & Walsh, V. (2009). Functional representation of living and nonliving domains across the cerebral hemispheres: A combined event-related potential/transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 403–414.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., & Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 74–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. (2006). Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology and neuroscience of mindreading. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in pictures. New York: Doubleday Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grinter, E. J., Maybery, M. T., Van Beek, P., Pellicano, E., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2009). Global visual processing and self-rated autistic-like traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1278–1290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E., & Bird, C. M. (2006). Executive processes in Asperger Syndrome: Patterns of performance in a multiple case series. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2822–2835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, P. R. (1987). The autistic child’s recognition of age and sex related characteristics of people. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 63–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hock, H. S., Kelso, J. A. S., & Schoner, G. (1993). Bistability and hysteresis in the organization of apparent motion patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 63–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., Cath, D. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): A study in Dutch population and patient groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1555–1566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horng, L. I. (2003). An adaptive smoothing approach for fitting digital planar curves with line segments and circular arcs. Pattern Recognition Letters, 24, 565–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C. H., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children’s difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. Developmental Psychology, 29, 10–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jellema, T., Lorteije, J. A. M., Van Rijn, S., Van ‘t Wout, M., De Haan, E. H. F., Van Engeland, H., et al. (2009). Involuntary processing of social cues is compromised in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 2, 192–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jellema, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Coding of visible and hidden objects. Attention and Performance, 19, 356–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jellema, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Neural basis for the perception of goal-directed actions. In A. Easton & N. J. Emery (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of social behaviour (pp. 81–112). New York: Psychology Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C. R., & Rakison, D. H. (2006). Early categorization of animate/inanimate concepts in young children with autism. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 18, 73–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaland, N., Smith, L., & Mortensen, E. L. (2008). Brief report: Cognitive flexibility and focused attention in children and adolescents with aspergers syndrome or high functioning autism as measured on the computerised version of the Wisconsin card sorting test. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 38, 1161–1165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L., & Lesser, L. I. (1958). Early infantile autism. Paediatrics Clinic of North America, 5, 711–730.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleinhans, N., Akshoomoff, N., & Delis, D. C. (2005). Executive functions in autism and asperger’s disorder. Flexibility, fluency, and inhibition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 379–401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, L., & Dawson, G. (2001). Prototype formation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 111–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1115–1122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2002). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review, Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Meyer, J., & Goldstein, G. (2002). Abstract reasoning in autism: A disassociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology, 16, 327–334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 171–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2001). Developmental origin of the animate–inanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 209–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. (1998). Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M. D., Pennington, B. P., & Rogers, S. J. (2006). The perception of animacy in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 983–992.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition of childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 8, 139–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger Syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. G., & Van der Wart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, M. E., Watson, J., Allcock, A. J., & Yaqoob, T. (2009). Autistic traits predict performance on the block design. Autism, 13, 133–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., et al. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and non-autistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 747–753.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van ‘t Wout, M., Van Rijn, S., Jellema, T., Kahn, R. S., & Aleman, A. (2009). Deficits in implicit attention to social signals in schizophrenia and high risk groups: Behavioural evidence from a new illusion. PLoS ONE, 4(e5581), 1–9.

  • Verbeke, E., Peeters, W., Kerkhof, I., Bijttebier, P., Steyaert, J., & Wagemans, J. (2005). Lack of motivation to share intentions: Primary deficit in autism? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 718–719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verstijnen, I. M., & Wagemans, J. (2004). Ambiguous figures: Living versus nonliving objects. Perception, 33, 531–546.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T., & Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 135–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–854.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III: Wechsler adult intelligence scale–third edition: Administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheatly, T., Milleville, S. C., & Martin, A. (2007). Understanding animate agents: Distinct roles for the social network and mirror system. Psychological Science, 18, 469–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., Smith, R., et al. (2006). Predicting autism spectrum quotient (AQ) from the systemizing quotient-revised (SQ-R) and empathy quotient (EQ). Brain Research, 1079, 47–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD–10 classification of diseases clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: WHO.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Sven Panis and Bart Machielsen for computing the compactness and homogeneity scores for the silhouettes used in this study. This research was supported by the AHRC.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tjeerd Jellema.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Burnett, H.G., Jellema, T. (Re-)conceptualisation in Asperger’s Syndrome and Typical Individuals with Varying Degrees of Autistic-like Traits. J Autism Dev Disord 43, 211–223 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1567-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1567-z

Keywords

Navigation