Skip to main content
Log in

Implicit Social Learning in Relation to Autistic-Like Traits

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigated if variation in autistic traits in the typically-developed population (using the Autism-spectrum Quotient, AQ) influenced implicit learning of social information. In the learning phase, participants repeatedly observed two identities whose gaze and expression conveyed either a pro- or antisocial disposition. These identities were then employed in a gaze-cueing paradigm. Participants made speeded responses to a peripheral target that was spatially pre-cued by a non-predictive gaze direction. The low AQ group (n = 50) showed a smaller gaze-cueing effect for the antisocial than for the prosocial identity. The high AQ group (n = 48) showed equivalent gaze-cueing for both identities. Others’ intentions/dispositions can be learned implicitly and affect subsequent responses to their behavior. This ability is impaired with increasing levels of autistic traits.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. As we employed a median split to create the low and high AQ groups, the mean AQ score for the high AQ group was low compared to the threshold of 32 for clinical levels of autistic traits (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001a). The data was therefore re-analyzed with the High AQ group defined as those in the upper quartile of AQ scores, with an AQ score of 19 or more (n = 24, M = 21.3, SD = 1.9). However, the results following this manipulation were no different from those using the median split (300 ms SOA: t(23) = 0.427, p = .673; 800 ms SOA: t(23) = 0.772, p = .448).

References

  • Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Perceived gaze direction and the processing of facial displays of emotion. Psychological Science, 14, 644–647.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion, 5, 3–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: Mass MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). The empathizing system: A revision of the 1994 model of the mindreading system. In B. Ellis & D. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 468–493). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 29, 407–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001a). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Spong, A., Scahill, V., & Lawson, J. (2001b). Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 5, 47–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001c). The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Gaze and arrow cueing of attention reveals individual differences along the autism spectrum as a function of target context. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 95–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2006). Predictive gaze cues and personality judgments: Should eye trust you? Psychological Science, 17, 514–520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayliss, A. P., di Pellegrino, G., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Sex differences in eye gaze and symbolic cueing of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A Human Experimental Psychology, 58, 631–650.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayliss, A. P., Griffiths, D., & Tipper, S. P. (2009). Predictive gaze cues affect face evaluations: The effect of facial emotion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 1072–1084.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, W. M., & Moore, C. (2002). Smile asymmetries and reputation as reliable indicators of likelihood to cooperate: An evolutionary analysis. In S. P. Shohov (Ed.), Advances in psychology research (Vol. 11, pp. 19–36). Hauppauge: Nova Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chawarska, K., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003). Automatic attention cueing through eye movement in 2-year-old children with autism. Child Development, 74, 1108–1122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic traits in the general population: A twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 719–726.

    Google Scholar 

  • D′Entremont, B., & Yazbek, A. (2007). Imitation of intentional and accidental actions by children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1665–1678.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deaner, R. O., Shepherd, S. V., & Platt, M. L. (2007). Familiarity accentuates gaze cuing in women but not men. Biology Letters, 3, 64–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6, 509–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (2003). Abrupt onsets and gaze direction cues trigger independent reflexive attentional effects. Cognition, 87, B1–B10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friesen, C. K., Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A. (2004). Attentional effects of counterpredictive gaze and arrow cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 30, 319–329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frischen, A., & Tipper, S. P. (2006). Long-term gaze cueing effects: Evidence for retrieval of prior states of attention from memory. Visual Cognition, 14, 351–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J., & Rutherford, M. D. (2007). The “reading the mind in the voice” test-revised: A study of complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1096–1106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grinter, E. J., Maybery, M. T., Van Beek, P. L., Pellicano, E., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2009). Global visual processing and self-rated autistic like traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1278–1290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 216–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E. (2007). Looking at you or looking elsewhere: The influence of head orientation on the signal value of emotional facial expressions. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 137–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, M., & Jellema, T. (2011). Resolving ambiguous behavioural intentions by means of involuntary prioritization of gaze processing. Emotion, 11, 681–686.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, M., Liu, C. H., & Jellema, T. (2009). Anticipating intentional actions: The effect of gaze direction on the judgment of head rotation. Cognition, 112, 423–434.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, M., Burnett, H. G., & Jellema, T. (2011). Anticipation of intentional actions in high- functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1410-y.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurst, R. M., Mitchell, J. T., Kimbrel, N. A., Kwapil, T. K., & Nelson-Gray, R. O. (2007). Examination of the reliability and factor structure of the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1938–1949.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jellema, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Coding of visible and hidden objects. Attention and Performance, 19, 356–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jellema, T., Lorteije, J., van Rijn, S., van t’ Wout, M., de Haan, E., van Engeland, H., et al. (2009). Involuntary interpretation of social cues is compromised in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 2, 192–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson, M., Mecklinger, A., & Treese, A. C. (2004). Recognition memory for emotional and neutral faces: An event related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1840–1853.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., Conway, C. A., & Feinberg, D. R. (2006). Integrating gaze direction and expression in preferences for attractive faces. Psychological Science, 17, 588–591.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kemner, C., Schuller, A. M., & van Engeland, H. (2006). Electrocortical reflections of face and gaze processing in children with pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 1063–1072.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, G., & Kingstone, A. (2009). Look away! Eyes and arrows engage occulomotor responses automatically. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 71, 314–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kylliainen, A., & Hietanen, J. K. (2004). Attention orienting by another’s gaze direction in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 435–444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langton, S. R. H., & Bruce, V. (1999). Reflexive visual orienting in response to the social attention of others. Visual Cognition, 6, 541–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Intuition: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 109–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D., Gaunt, R., Gilbert, D. T., & Trope, Y. (2002). Reflexion and reflection: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 199–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombardo, M. V., Barnes, J. L., Wheelwright, S. J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2007). Self referential cognition and empathy in autism. PLoS ONE, 2, e883.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mehu, M., Grammer, K., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007a). Smiles when sharing. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 415–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mehu, M., Little, A. C., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007b). Duchenne smiles and the perception of generosity and sociability in faces. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 5, 133–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Penny, S. (2008). Sensitivity to eye gaze in autism: Is it normal? Is it automatic? Is it social? Development and Psychopathology, 20, 79–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., O′Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 765–775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redcay, E. (2008). The superior temporal sulcus performs a common function for social and speech perception: Implications for the emergence of autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 123–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, P., Lowe, C., & Everett, R. (2011). Perceptual learning and perceptual search are altered in male university students with higher Autism Quotient scores. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 732–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ristic, J., Mottron, L., Friesen, C. K., Iarocci, G., Burack, J. A., & Kingstone, A. (2005). Eyes are special but not for everyone: The case of autism. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 715–718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman, T., Garnham, W., & Rideout, P. (2001). Social understanding in autism: Eye gaze as a measure of core insights. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 1083–1094.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Kaiser, S., Wehrle, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2007). Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 470–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2006). Integrating automatic and controlled processes into neurocognitive models of social cognition. Brain Research, 1079, 86–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scharlemann, J. P. W., Eckel, C. C., Kacelnik, A., & Wilson, R. K. (2001). The value of a smile: Game theory with a human face. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22, 617–640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, K. L., & Cohn, J. F. (2001). Human facial expressions as adaptations: Evolutionary questions in facial expression research. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 44, 3–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Dairoku, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2004). Reflexive orienting in response to eye gaze and an arrow in children with and without autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 445–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, M. E., & Austin, E. J. (2009). The structure of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from a student sample in Scotland. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 224–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suda, M., Takei, Y., Aoyama, Y., Narita, K., Sakurai, N., Fukuda, M., et al. (2011). Autistic traits and brain activation during face-to-face conversations in typically developed adults. PLoS ONE, 6, e20021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, A., & Crewther, D. P. (2010). Magnocellular visual evoked potential delay with high Autism-Spectrum Quotient yields a neural mechanism for altered perception. Brain, 133, 2089–2097.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swettenham, J., Condie, S., Campbell, R., Milne, E., & Coleman, M. (2003). Does the perception of moving eyes trigger reflexive visual orienting in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 358, 325–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tipples, J. (2005). Orienting to eye gaze and face processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 31, 843–856.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tipples, J. (2008). Orienting to counterpredictive gaze and arrow cues. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 77–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Dijk, E., van Kleef, G. A., Steinel, W., & van Beest, I. (2008). A social functional approach to emotions in bargaining: When communicating anger pays and when it backfires. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 600–614.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vanneste, S., Verplaetse, J., Van Hiel, A., & Braeckman, J. (2007). Attention bias toward noncooperative people: A dot probe classification study in cheating detection. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 272–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplaetse, J., Vanneste, S., & Braeckman, J. (2007). You can judge a book by its cover: The sequel. A kernel of truth in predictive cheating detection. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 260–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vlamings, P. H. J. M., Stauder, J. E. A., van Son, I. A. M., & Mottron, L. (2005). Atypical visual orienting to gaze- and arrow-cues in adults with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 267–277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Von dem Hagen, A. H., Nummenmaa, L., Yu, R., Engell, A. D., Ewbank, M. P., & Calder, A. J. (2011). Autism spectrum traits in the typical population predict structure and function in the posterior superior temporal sulcus. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 493–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodbury-Smith, M. R., Robinson, J., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). Screening adults for Asperger syndrome using the AQ: A preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical practice. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 331–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamagishi, T., Tanida, S., Mashima, R., Shimoma, E., & Kanazawa, S. (2003). You can judge a book by its cover: Evidence that cheaters may look different from cooperators. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 290–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zalla, T., Sav, A. M., Stopin, A., Ahade, S., & Leboyer, M. (2009). Faux pas detection and intentional action in Asperger syndrome: A replication in a French sample. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 373–382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the 80th Anniversary University of Hull Studentship Scheme and by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO, No. 446-08-003).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthew Hudson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hudson, M., Nijboer, T.C.W. & Jellema, T. Implicit Social Learning in Relation to Autistic-Like Traits. J Autism Dev Disord 42, 2534–2545 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1510-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1510-3

Keywords

Navigation