Skip to main content
Log in

Reasoning on the Basis of Fantasy Content: Two Studies with High-Functioning Autistic Adolescents

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Reasoning about problems with empirically false content can be hard, as the inferences that people draw are heavily influenced by their background knowledge. However, presenting empirically false premises in a fantasy context helps children and adolescents to disregard their beliefs, and to reason on the basis of the premises. The aim of the present experiments was to see if high-functioning adolescents with autism are able to utilize fantasy context to the same extent as typically developing adolescents when they reason about empirically false premises. The results indicate that problems with engaging in pretence in autism persist into adolescence, and this hinders the ability of autistic individuals to disregard their beliefs when empirical knowledge is irrelevant.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Here we would like to note that our study included contrary-to-fact materials, rather than counterfactual materials, which is an important distinction. Whereas contrary-to-fact materials refer to empirically false fantasy content (e.g., flying pigs), counterfactual materials refer to alternatives to past events which, although they are not true, could be true (e.g., in the Sally-Anne task, the marble could be in the basket, rather than in the box). Although some classic studies in the literature (most notably, studies by Dias and Harris, and Scott and colleagues) used the two terms interchangeably, a growing body of evidence from recent studies suggests that the developmental trajectories of the two abilities differ (e.g., Beck et al. 2006; Beck and Guthrie 2011).

  2. Given that the proportion of males and females was unequal in the two groups, we have compared our male and female participants in each sample across all the measures that we used In order to check if this could affect our results regarding differences between the autistic and control groups. We have found no significant gender differences across any of the tasks (ts < 1.25, ps > .22).

  3. The availability of each participant was determined by external circumstances, and it did not reflect their individual characteristics.

  4. Given that the proportion of males and females was unequal in the two groups (similarly to Experiment 1), we have compared our male and female participants in each sample across all the measures that we used. We have found no significant gender differences across any of the tasks (ts < 1.36, ps > .18).

References

  • Adolphs, R. (1999). The human amygdala and emotion. The Neuroscientist, 5, 125–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: The case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285–298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Allen, J., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Can autism be detected at 18 months? The needle, the haystack, and the CHAT. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 839–843.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Scahill, V., Lawson, J., & Spong, A. (2001). Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 5, 47–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, S. R., & Guthrie, C. (2011). Almost thinking counterfactually: Children’s understanding of close counterfactuals. Child Development, 82, 1189–1198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, S. R., Robinson, E. J., Carroll, D. J., & Apperly, I. A. (2006). Children’s thinking about counterfactuals and future hypotheticals as possibilities. Child Development, 77, 413–426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Binnie, L., & Williams, J. (2003). Intuitive psychology and physics among children with autism and typically developing children. Autism, 7, 173–193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., & Berthollier, N. (2004). Source memory in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 533–542.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. M. J., Espino, O., & Santamaria, C. (1999). Counterexamples and the suppression of inferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 347–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daniel, D., & Klaczynski, P. A. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in conditional reasoning: Effects of logic instructions and alternative antecedents. Child Development, 77, 339–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Gernsbacher, M. A., & Mottron, L. (2007). The level and nature of autistic intelligence. Psychological Science, 18, 657–662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Martino, B., Harrison, N., Knafo, S., Bird, G., & Dolan, R. (2008). Explaining enhanced logical consistency during decision making in autism. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 10746–10750.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Neys, W. (2006). Dual processing in reasoning: Two systems but one reasoner. Psychological Science, 17, 428–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dias, M. G., & Harris, P. L. (1988). The effect of make-believe play on deductive reasoning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 207–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dias, M. G., & Harris, P. L. (1990). The influence of the imagination on reasoning by young children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 305–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. St. B. T., Barston, J. L., & Pollard, P. (1983). On the conflict between logic and belief in syllogistic reasoning. Memory and Cognition, 11, 295–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gauffroy, C., & Barrouillet, P. (2009). Heuristic and analytic processes in mental models for conditional: An integrative developmental theory. Developmental Review, 29, 249–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Handley, S., Capon, A., Beveridge, M., Dennis, I., & Evans, J. St. B. T. (2004). Working memory, inhibitory control, and the development of children’s reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 10, 175–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Handley, S. J., Newstead, S. E., & Trippas, D. (2011). Logic, beliefs and instruction: A test of the default interventionist account of belief bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 28–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. E. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of Relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1994). Autism: An introduction to psychological theory. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843–855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. E. (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Reading homographs in context. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld, L., Bartmess, E., White, S., & Frith, U. (2007). Can autistic children predict behavior by social stereotypes? Current Biology, 17, 451–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Honey, E., Leekam, S., Turner, M., & McConachie, H. (2007). Repetitive behavior and play in typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1107–1115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C., & Conn, C. (2011). The development of pretend play in autism. In A. Pellegrini (Ed.), Oxford handbook of the development of play (pp. 308–321). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of central coherence theory: Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome—is local coherence impaired? Cognition, 71, 149–185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Linguistic processing in high-functioning adultswith autism or Asperger syndrome: Can global coherence be achieved? A further test of central coherence theory. Psychological Medicine, 30, 1169–1187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kokis, J., Macpherson, R., Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). Heuristic and analytic processing: Age trends and associations with cognitive ability and cognitive styles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 26–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LaPointe, L. B., & Engle, R. W. (1990). Simple and complex word spans as measures of working memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 1118–1133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leevers, H. J., & Harris, P. L. (1999). Persisting effects of instruction on young children’ssyllogistic reasoning with incongruent and abstract premises. Thinking and Reasoning, 2, 145–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leevers, H. J., & Harris, P. L. (2000). Counterfactual syllogistic reasoning in normal 4-year-olds, children with learning disabilities, and children with autism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76, 64–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretence and representation: The origins of ‘‘theory of mind’.’. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lopez, B., & Leekam, S. R. (2003). Do children with autism fail to process information in context? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44, 285–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markovits, H. (1995). Conditional reasoning with false premises: Fantasy and information retrieval. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markovits, H., & Lortie Forgues, H. (2011). Conditional reasoning with false premises facilitates the transition between familiar and abstract reasoning. Child Development, 82, 646–660.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 311–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCrory, E., Henry, L. A., & Happé, F. (2007). Eye-witness memory and suggestibility in children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 482–489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McKenzie, R., Evans, J. St. B. T., & Handley, S. J. (2010). Conditional reasoning in autism: Activation and integration of knowledge and belief. Developmental Psychology, 46, 391–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McKenzie, R., Evans, J. St. B. T., & Handley, S. J. (2011). Autism and performance on the suppression task: Reasoning, context and complexity. Thinking and Reasoning, 17, 182–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., & Ahn, J. (2004). Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: The effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 115, 123–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S, (1996), Control of mental processes. In V. Bruce (Ed.), Unsolved Mysteries of the Mind: Tutorial Essays in Cognition (pp. 93–148). Hove, E. Sussex: Erlbaum (UK).

  • Morsanyi, K., & Handley, S. J. (2008). How smart do you need to be to get it wrong? The role of cognitive capacity in the development of heuristic-based judgment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 18–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morsanyi, K., & Holyoak, K. J. (2010). Analogical reasoning ability in autistic and typically-developing children. Developmental Science, 13, 578–587.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morsanyi, K., Handley, S. J., & Evans, J. S. B. T. (2010). Decontextualised minds: Adolescents with autism are less susceptible to the conjunction fallacy than typically developing adolescents. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1378–1388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pexman, P. M., Rostad, K. R., McMorris, C. A., Climie, E. A., Stowkowy, J., & Glenwright, M. R. (2011). Processing of ironic language in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1097–1112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pijnacker, J., Geurts, B., van Lambalgen, M., Kan, C. C., Buitelaar, J. K., & Hagoort, P. (2009). Defeasible reasoning in high-functioning adults with autism: Evidence for impaired exception-handling. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2940–2951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pijnacker, J., Geurts, B., van Lambalgen, M., Buitelaar, J., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Exceptions and anomalies: An ERP study on context sensitivity in autism. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2940–2951.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, S., & Markovits, H. (1998). Conditional reasoning, causality, and the structure of semantic memory: Strength of association as a predictive factor for content effects. Cognition, 68, B93–B101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Manual for Raven’s advanced progressive matrices (1998th ed.). Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sattler, J. M. (2001). Assessment of children: Cognitive applications (4th ed.). San Diego, CA: Jerome M. Sattler.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, F. J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1996). Logical, analogical and psychological reasoning in autism: A test of the Cosmides theory. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 235–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, F. J., Baron-Cohen, S., & Leslie, A. M. (1999). If pigs could fly: A test of counterfactual reasoning and pretence in children with autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 349–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simoneau, M., & Markovits, H. (2003). Reasoning with premises that are not empirically true: Evidence for the role of inhibition and retrieval. Developmental Psychology, 39, 964–975.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skolnick, D., & Bloom, P. (2006). What does Batman think about SpongeBob? Children’s understanding of the fantasy/fantasy distinction. Cognition, 101, B9–B18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (2003). The fundamental computational biases of human cognition: Heuristics that (sometimes) impair decision making and problem solving. In J. E. Davidson & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of problem solving (pp. 291–342). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, V. A. (1996). Reasoning from false premises: The role of soundness in making logical deductions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 315–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vadeboncoeur, I., & Markovits, H. (1999). The effect of instructions and information retrieval on accepting the premises in a conditional reasoning task. Psychology, 5, 97–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Venet, M., & Markovits, H. (2001). Understanding uncertainty with abstract conditional premises. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47, 74–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verschueren, N., Schaeken, W., & d’Ydewalle, G. (2005). Everyday conditional reasoning: A working memory-dependent tradeoff between counterexample and likelihood use. Memory and Cognition, 33, 107–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children–third edition (WISC-III). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 9, 11–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Jonathan S.B.T. Evans for helpful discussions, and Becky McKenzie for her help with recruiting participants, and collecting data. The authors would also like to thank all the schools which participated in the study: Ridgeway School, Plymstock School, Tamarside Community College, Tavistock College and Callington Community College. This research was conducted as part of the first author’s PhD research at the University of Plymouth (PhD supervisors: Simon J. Handley and Jonathan S.B.T. Evans). The writing of this paper was supported by an ESRC Post-Doctoral Fellowship (ES/1038071/1) to K.M.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kinga Morsanyi.

Appendix

Appendix

Syllogisms used in Experiment 1 (correct responses are marked with *)

  • 1. All nurses are sabs.

  • All sabs are caring.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all nurses are caring.

  • Yes* No, not necessarily

  • 2. All politicians are mids.

  • All mids are shy.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all politicians are shy.

  • Yes* No, not necessarily

  • 3. All hals are firemen.

  • All brave people are hals.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all firemen are brave.

  • Yes No, not necessarily*

  • 4. All eagles are fals.

  • All fals are predators.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all eagles are predators.

  • Yes* No, not necessarily

  • 5. All ice creams are vons.

  • All vons are hot.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all ice creams are hot.

  • Yes* No, not necessarily

  • 6. All heavy animals are dufs.

  • All dufs are elephants.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all elephants are heavy.

  • Yes No, not necessarily*

  • 7. All gofs are slow.

  • All cheetahs are gofs.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all cheetahs are slow.

  • Yes* No, not necessarily

  • 8. All funny people are bocs.

  • All bocs are comedians.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all comedians are funny.

  • Yes No, not necessarily*

  • 9. All puds are foolish.

  • All doctors are puds.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all doctors are foolish.

  • Yes* No, not necessarily

  • 10. All raks are clever.

  • All police dogs are raks.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all police dogs are clever.

  • Yes* No, not necessarily

  • 11. All vums are glasses.

  • All unbreakable things are vums.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all glasses are unbreakable.

  • Yes No, not necessarily*

  • 12. All baks are healthy.

  • All aerobics instructors are baks.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all aerobics instructors are healthy.

  • Yes* No, not necessarily.

  • 13. All quiet people are curs.

  • All curs are salesmen.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all salesmen are quiet.

  • Yes No, not necessarily*

  • 14. All black plants are mibs.

  • All mibs are flowers.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all flowers are black.

  • Yes No, not necessarily*

  • 15. All jops are aeroplane pilots.

  • All drinkers are jops.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all aeroplane pilots are drinkers.

  • Yes No, not necessarily*

  • 16. All embs are aeroplanes.

  • All fast vehicles are embs.

  • Therefore, in this village

  • all aeroplanes are fast.

  • Yes No, not necessarily*

Syllogisms used in Experiment 2 (correct responses are marked with *)

  • 1. On this planet

  • all furry animals have long ears.

  • Rabbits are furry animals.

  • On this planet rabbits have long ears.

  • Yes* No Not certain

  • 2. On this planet

  • fruits need water to grow.

  • Vegetables need water to grow.

  • On this planet vegetables are fruits.

  • Yes No Not certain*

  • 3. On this planet

  • trees have red trunks.

  • Oaks do not have red trunks.

  • On this planet oaks are not trees.

  • Yes* No Not certain

  • 4. On this planet fish eat nuts.

  • Squirrels do not eat nuts.

  • On this planet squirrels are not fish.

  • Yes* No Not certain

  • 5. On this planet turtles live in little ponds in the mountains.

  • Sharks are not turtles.

  • On this planet sharks don’t live in little ponds in the mountains.

  • Yes No Not certain*

  • 6. On this planet

  • robins have two legs.

  • Doves are not robins.

  • On this planet doves do not have two legs.

  • Yes No Not certain*

  • 7. On this planet

  • flowers have thorns on them.

  • Daffodils are flowers.

  • On this planet daffodils have thorns on them.

  • Yes* No Not certain

  • 8. On this planet all poisonous animals live in the jungle.

  • Cobras live in the jungle.

  • On this planet cobras are poisonous animals.

  • Yes No Not certain*

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Morsanyi, K., Handley, S.J. Reasoning on the Basis of Fantasy Content: Two Studies with High-Functioning Autistic Adolescents. J Autism Dev Disord 42, 2297–2311 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1477-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1477-0

Keywords

Navigation