Skip to main content
Log in

Dyadic and Triadic Behaviours in Infancy as Precursors to Later Social Responsiveness in Young Children with Autistic Disorder

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The relationship between dyadic (eye contact and affect) and triadic (joint attention) behaviours in infancy, and social responsiveness at pre-school age, was investigated in 36 children with Autistic Disorder. Measures of eye contact and affect, and joint attention, including requesting behaviours, were obtained retrospectively via parental interviews and home videos from 0- to- 24-months of age. Concurrent measures (3–5 years) included social responsiveness to another’s distress and need for help. Early dyadic behaviours observed in home videos, but not as reported by parents, were associated with later social responsiveness. Many triadic behaviours (from both parent-reports and home video) were also associated with social responsiveness at follow-up. The results are consistent with the view that early dyadic and triadic behaviours, particularly sharing attention, are important for the development of later social responsiveness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For example, initiating joint attention was not asked for the 0-5 month age range since these behaviours do not typically emerge until the end of the first year of life.

  2. Given the small subject numbers in the study, the decision was made to report on only those correlations with an r value >.4 along with p values <.05. Bonferroni correction was not applied since this would inflate Type II error to unacceptable levels.

References

  • Adrien, J. L., Perrot, A., Sauvage, D., Leddet, I., Larmanade, C., Hameury, L., et al. (1992). Early symptoms in autism from family home movies: Evaluation and comparison between 1st and 2nd year of life using I.B.S.E. Scale. Acta Paedopsychiatrica: International Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(2), 71–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th text revised ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D. A. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 131–158). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baranek, G. T. (1999). Autism during infancy: A retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor and social behaviors at 9–12 months of age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(3), 213–224. doi:10.1023/A:1023080005650.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). Cognition and communication from 9–13 months: correlational findings. In E. Bates, L. Benigni, I. Bretherton, L. Camaioni, & V. Volterra (Eds.), The emergence of symbols (pp. 69–140). NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernabei, P., Camaioni, L., & Levi, G. (1998). An evaluation of early development in children with autism and pervasive developmental disorders from home movies: Preliminary findings. Autism, 2, 243–258. doi:10.1177/1362361398023003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calder, A., Lawrence, A., Keane, J., Scott, S., Owen, A., Christoffels, I., & Young, A. (2002). Reading the mind from eye gaze. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1129–1138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15(4), 481–498. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00037-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1998). An experimental investigation of social-cognitive abilities in infants with autism: Clinical implications. Infant Mental Health Journal, 19(2), 260–275. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199822)19:2<260::AID-IMHJ12>3.0.CO;2-W.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clifford, S., & Dissanayake, C. (2008). The early development of joint attention in infants with autistic disorder using home video observations and parental interview. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 791–805. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0444-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, K., Nacewicz, B., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H., et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 519–526.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Carver, L., Meltzoff, A. N., Panagiotides, H., & McPartland, J. (2002). Neural correlates of face recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development. Child Development, 73, 700–717.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., et al. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–283. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dissanayake, C., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1996). Long-term stability of individual differences in the emotional responsiveness of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 37(4), 461–467. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01427.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (2000). Development of children’s knowledge about the mental world. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 15–23. doi:10.1080/016502500383421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1986). The autistic child’s appraisal of expressions of emotion: A further study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 27, 671–680. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1986.tb00191.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1989). On sharing experiences. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 197–203. doi:10.1017/S0954579400000390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, P. (2002). The cradle of thought. Hampshire, England: Macmillan Education Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988a). What’s in a face? The case of autism. The British Journal of Psychology, 79, 441–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988b). Emotion recognition in autism: Coordinating faces and voices. Psychological Medicine, 18, 911–923.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S., & Ramsden, C. (2006). Dyadic orienting and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 185–197. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0054-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., Di Lavore, P., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule-WPS Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

  • Loveland, K., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Pearson, D., Brelsford, K., Ortegon, J., & Chen, R. (1994). Imitation and expression of facial affect in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 433–444. doi:10.1017/S0954579400006039.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maestro, S., Casella, C., Milone, A., Muratori, F., & Espasa, F. P. (1999). Study of the onset of autism through home movies. Psychopathology, 32(6), 292–300. doi:10.1159/000029102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malle, B. (2001). Folk explanations of intentional action. In B. Malle, L. Moses, & J. Louis (Eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 265–286). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

  • Meltzoff, A. N., Gopnik, A., & Repacholi, B. M. (1999). Toddlers’ understanding of intentions, desires, and emotions: Explorations of the dark ages. In P. D. Zelazo, J. W. Astington, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control (pp. 17–41). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Eribaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P. (1995). Joint attention and social-emotional approach behavior in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 7(1), 63–82. doi:10.1017/S0954579400006349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P. (2003). The neural basis of social impairments in autism: The role of the dorsal medial-frontal cortex and anterior cingulate system. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 44(6), 793–809. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Crowson, M. (1997). Joint attention and early social communication: Implications for research on intervention with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 653–676. doi:10.1023/A:1025802832021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Hogan, A. (1994). Intersubjectivity, joint attention, and autistic developmental pathology. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Disorders and dysfunctions of the self. Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology (Vol. 5, pp. 1–30). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

  • Mundy, P., Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. (1992). Nonverbal communication, affective sharing, and intersubjectivity. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 377–381. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(92)80006-G.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Sheinkopf, S. J. (1998). Early communication skill and developmental disorders. In J. Burack, R. Hodapp, & E. Zigler (Eds.), Handbook of mental retardation and development (pp. 183–207). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 115–128. doi:10.1007/BF02206861.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint attention, developmental level, and symptom presentation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 389–401. doi:10.1017/S0954579400006003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noldus. (2000). The Observer 4.0 [Computer software]. Wageningen, The Netherlands: Noldus Information Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257. doi:10.1007/BF02172225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. (2002). Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 239–251. doi:10.1017/S0954579402002031.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schopler, E., Reichler, J., & Renner, B. (1998). The childhood autism rating scale (CARS). CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seibert, J., Hogan, A., & Mundy, P. (1982). Assessing interactional competencies: The Early Social-Communication Scales. Infant Mental Health Journal, 3, 244–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M. D., Kasari, C., Kwon, J., & Yirmiya, N. (1992). Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Development, 63, 796–807. doi:10.2307/1131234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(1), v–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., & Rochat, P. (1999). Developmental link between dyadic and triadic social competence in infancy. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17(4), 551–562. doi:10.1348/026151099165474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Travis, L., & Sigman, M. (2001). Communicative intentions and symbols in autism: Examining a case of altered development. In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 279–308). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Travis, L., Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (2001). Links between social understanding and social behavior in verbally able children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(2), 119–130. doi:10.1023/A:1010705912731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 321–347). London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol (pp. 183–299). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N. (2000). Brief report: Recognition of autism spectrum disorder before one year of age: A retrospective study based on home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(2), 157–162. doi:10.1023/A:1005463707029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the families who willingly gave their time to participate in this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sally Clifford.

Additional information

This research formed part of the first author’s doctoral dissertation, which was conducted under the supervision of the second author at La Trobe University.

Appendix

Appendix

Operational Definitions and Coding Criteria for Video Study Variables

1. Eye contact: The infant looks directly into the person’s face/eyes; here it is clear that the infant is looking at a person, and not the camera.

*Coding example for quality.

0 = infant shows age and contextually appropriate level of eye-contact with other persons that is clear, flexible, socially modulated and of appropriate duration.

1 = infant shows a slight deficiency in eye-contact with other persons, but does engage in some eye-contact (i.e. eye-contact occurs but may be fleeting and of short duration, or may sometimes consist of empty, lifeless or vacant stares;

2 = infant shows a moderate deficiency in eye-contact with other persons, rarely engaging in eye-contact and with extremely short duration, or may often consist of empty, lifeless or vacant stares, or somewhat poorly modulated eye contact;

3 = infant never engages in eye-contact or continuously stares vacantly and lifelessly, or displays poorly modulated eye contact in interactions.

2. Responds to name call: Infant looks directly at person calling them. If there is repeated calling, code a new look after 3 sec has elapsed.

3. Social smile: The infant smiles at a person while looking at them (initiating the smile); the infant responds to the smile of a caregiver by returning a smile immediately after the caregiver initiated that smile (reciprocal).

4. Shared positive affect: The infant demonstrates at least one of the following behaviours (in addition to a social smile): laughs, giggles, shows joy, happiness, facial enthusiasm, elevation, excitement; and combines these with some eye contact all while in close proximity to another person’s face. The infant wants to share enjoyment and excitement with the caregiver and directs toward the caregiver.

5. Joint attention gaze switching (checking/looking behaviour: sharing through eye contact): Infant looks at another person’s face in the presence of something interesting (possibly while holding/activating a toy) and then looks back at that object/event (in a sense ‘checking’ that the person has seen the object/event; Infant looks to caregiver to within 2 sec of a toy ceasing and then back to toy.

6. Initiating joint attention (Proto-declarative pointing/showing/giving/pushes toward for sharing, not to obtain or to request the removal of a toy): The infant points at a (proximal or distal) object in order to direct the caregiver’s attention to the object to share interest in the object; brings an object/hands object to a person or extends arm in the direction of the person’s face to show the object (not associated with need for help).

7. Responding joint attention (gaze monitoring and point following): The infant follows the caregiver’s point, gaze or head turn by moving their own head and focus or turning in the same direction in which the caregiver is looking, pointing, or showing interest (attention to a common focus). The caregiver may be vocalising too (e.g. “look”).

8. Social referencing: Infant looks at another person’s face in the presence of something ambiguous/threatening for information (and then may look back at that object/event).

9. Initiate requests (proto-imperatives): The infant points or extends arm and hand toward a desired object which aids the infant in obtaining the object (is part of a request for something out of reach; is often accompanied by vocalisation); infant gives object/pushes object toward caregiver in order to obtain help “do it again” or to “get rid” of something they do not want.

10. Responds to requests: The infant responds to the request of another (verbal or gestural) by, for example, giving an object to another person when they request it with an open palm; coming to ‘sit down’ when signal is given; labelling.

Note: The Early Social Communication Scales, Seibert et al. (1982) and the ADOS-G Module 1/2 (Lord et al.1999) were used in the development of these operational definitions.

Parent Interview Summary

Parents to answer in terms of this scale and interviewer to record answer:

Eye contact (All questions asked for: 0–6; –12; 12–18; and 18–24 months).

1. While in close proximity to your child (e.g. when sitting with your child on your lap facing you, while sitting close together at the table, at bath time) did your child gaze into your eyes?

2. When you try to get your child’s attention, did you ever feel that your child avoids looking directly at you?

3. Was it difficult to get eye contact initially with your child? (i.e. to ‘catch’ your child’s eye).

4. Was your child’s eye contact ever unpredictable?

5. Did your child seem to display abnormal eye contact, that is, the QUALITY seems to be different (e.g. an empty or lifeless expression, fleeting or piercing looks).

Eye contact and affect (All questions asked for: 0–6; 6–12; 12–18; and 18–24 months).

6. Did your child participate in peek-a-boo games? (i.e. a hiding behind a cloth and then being surprised in an anticipatory manner).

7. Did your child combine smiling and eye contact in interactions with you? (e.g. look at you smiling, make eye contact with you and smile at the same time).

Affect (All questions asked for: 0–6; 6–12; 12–18; and 18–24 months unless stated).

8. Does your child smile during interactions with you while playing with another object or sharing an event (e.g. smile at you when a mechanical toy is operating, or when an animal is near)?

(Interviewer Note: not asked for the first 6 months, as this behaviour is not generally shown during this age).

9. While close to your child (i.e. face to face with eye contact) how often did your child INITIATE smiles with you (i.e. smile spontaneously, first)?

10. Did your child smile IN RESPONSE to your smile?

11. Did your child use his/her emotions appropriately? (e.g. cry when sad; smile and laugh when happy; show a fearful face when scared etc.).

Joint attention (All questions asked for: 6–12; 12–18; and 18–24 months unless stated).

12. Did your child look at things you pointed at (follow your point)?

13. Did your child look at things you look at (follow your gaze)?

14. How often did your child use his/her index finger to point to indicate INTEREST in something (or extend his/her arm and hand to indicate like an approximation of a point)?

15. Did your child ever bring objects over to you, to SHOW you something?

(Interviewer Note: not asked for the first 12 months, as this behaviour is not generally shown during this age).

16. Did your child ever bring objects over to you, to GIVE you something?

(Interviewer Note: not asked for the first 12 months, as this behaviour is not generally shown during this age).

17. Did your child try and attract your attention to his/her own activity by looking back and forth between you and the object (and possibly vocalising)? (Demonstrate behaviour).

18. Did your child look at your face to check your reaction when faced with something unfamiliar? (e.g. a new toy, a stranger).

19. Does your child share your affect when faced with something unfamiliar (e.g. if you look uncertain, your child looks uncertain)?

Requesting (All questions asked for: 6–12; 12–18; and 18–24 months).

20. Did your child ever use his/her index finger to point to ASK for something (or extend his/her arm and hand to ASK)?

21. Did your infant ever BRING you something in attempt to request something (e.g. a toy to be activated or a box to be opened, often accompanied with a vocalisation such as a whine)?

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Clifford, S., Dissanayake, C. Dyadic and Triadic Behaviours in Infancy as Precursors to Later Social Responsiveness in Young Children with Autistic Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 39, 1369–1380 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0748-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0748-x

Keywords

Navigation