Skip to main content
Log in

Generativity Abilities Predict Communication Deficits but not Repetitive Behaviors in Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) often demonstrate impaired generativity that is thought to mediate repetitive behaviors in autism (Turner in J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 40(6):839–849, 1999a). The present study evaluated generativity in children with and without ASD via the use-of-objects task (Turner in J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 40(2):189–201, 1999b) and an Animals Fluency Task (Lezak in Neuropsychological assessment. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 1995). Groups differed significantly on two of four metrics from the Animals Fluency Task and two of seven metrics from the Use of Objects task. In the ASD sample, no significant relations were found between generativity and repetitive behaviors. Significant relations were found, however, between performance on the Animals Fluency Task and communication symptoms. Results replicate reports of generativity deficits in ASD and suggest that impaired generativity may reflect communication deficits that are characteristic of the disorder.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ambery, F. Z., Russell, A. J., Perry, K., Morris, R., & Murphy, D. G. (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 10(6), 551–564. doi:10.1177/1362361306068507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. Task Force on DSM-1V. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV (4th ed.). Washington, DC.

  • Ardila, A., Pineda, D., & Rosselli, M. (2000). Correlation between intelligence test scores and executive function measures. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15(1), 31–36. doi:10.1016/S0887-6177(98)00159-0.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arffa, S. (2007). The relationship of intelligence to executive function and non-executive function measures in a sample of average, above average, and gifted youth. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(8), 969–978. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnard, L., Muldoon, K., Hasan, R., O’Brien, G., & Stewart, M. (2008). Profiling executive dysfunction in adults with autism and comorbid learning disability. Autism, 12(2), 125–141. doi:10.1177/1362361307088486.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benton, A. C., & Hamsher, K. D. (1989). Multilingual aphasia examination. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, H., & Barch, D. (1995). The categorization of thought disorder. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24(5), 349–376. doi:10.1007/BF02144565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. (1998). Development of the children’s communication checklist (CCC): A method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(6), 879–891. doi:10.1017/S0021963098002832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V., & Norbury, C. F. (2005). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology. 1: Generativity. Autism, 9(1), 7–27. doi:10.1177/1362361305049027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bodfish, J.W., Symons, F.W., & Lewis, M.H. (1999). The repetitive behavior scale-revised. Western Carolina Center Research Reports (unpublished).

  • Constantino, J. N., Davis, S. A., Todd, R. D., Schindler, M. K., Gross, M. M., Brophy, S. L., et al. (2003). Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: Comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(4), 427–433. doi:10.1023/A:1025014929212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1970). Studies in pattern detection in normal and autistic children. II. Reproduction and production of color sequences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 10(1), 120–135. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(70)90049-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1996). Cognitive explanations of autism. Acta Paediatrica (Oslo, Norway). Supplement, 416, 63–68. doi:10.1111/j.1651-2227.1996.tb14280.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., & Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 74–82. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, R. (1981). Wisconsin card sorting test manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 26–32. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Humes, G. E., Welsh, M. C., Retzlaff, P., & Cookson, N. (1997). Towers of Hanoi and London: Reliability and validity of two executive function tests. Assessment, 4, 249–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinhans, N., Akshoomoff, N., & Delis, D. C. (2005). Executive functions in autism and Asperger’s disorder: Flexibility, fluency, and inhibition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(3), 379–401. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2703_5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lam, K. S., & Aman, M. G. (2007). The Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised: Independent validation in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 37(5), 855–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview—revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685. doi:10.1007/BF02172145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1997). Neuropsychological functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W., & Weiskrantz, L. (1998). The prefrontal cortex: Executive and cognitive functions. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roid, G. H., & Miller, L. J. (1997). Leiter international performance scale-revised. Woodale, IL: Stoelting Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1988). Neuropsychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism, residual state. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10(2), 201–221. doi:10.1080/01688638808408236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1990). Neuropsychological divergence of high-level autism and severe dyslexia. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(2), 155–168. doi:10.1007/BF02284715.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. (1997). Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M. D., & Rogers, S. J. (2003). Cognitive underpinnings of pretend play in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(3), 289–302. doi:10.1023/A:1024406601334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, M., Ozonoff, S., Carter, C., & Caplan, R. (2008). Formal thought disorder and the autism spectrum: Relationship with symptoms, executive control, and anxiety. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 38(8), 1474–1484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. T., & Knight, R. T. (2002). Principles of frontal lobe function. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M. A. (1997). Towards an executive dysfunction account of repetitive behaviour in autism. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder (pp. 57–100). Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M. A. (1999a). Annotation: Repetitive behaviour in autism: A review of psychological research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(6), 839–849. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M. A. (1999b). Generating novel ideas: Fluency performance in high-functioning and learning disabled individuals with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(2), 189–201. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by R01 MH073402 (Bodfish). G. Dichter was supported by NIH/NCRR K12 RR023248 and NIMH K23 MH081285. G. Dichter, K. S. L. Lam, and L. M. Turner-Brown were supported by NICHD T32-HD40127. Assistance for this study was provided by the Subject Registry Core of the UNC Neurodevelopmental Disorders Research Center (P30 HD03110).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gabriel S. Dichter.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dichter, G.S., Lam, K.S.L., Turner-Brown, L.M. et al. Generativity Abilities Predict Communication Deficits but not Repetitive Behaviors in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 39, 1298–1304 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0742-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0742-3

Keywords

Navigation