Skip to main content
Log in

Goal-Directed Action Representation in Autism

  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

The aim of the present study was to investigate the ability of individuals with autism to represent goal-directed behavioural actions. We compared the performance of subjects with autism (n=16), mentally retarded subjects (n=14) and normal healthy subjects (n=15) in a sequencing task consisted in arranging pictures of single events in their appropriate order so as to make coherent stories. Three types of actions were presented: (a) actions on objects; (b) actions on objects in a broader spatio-temporal context; (c) interactive actions. Autistic subjects were impaired in arranging sequences of actions on objects, as compared to both control groups. This impairment might contribute to explaining both executive deficits and difficulties in understanding others’ behaviour in autism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 4.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatry Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders 4th Edition, Washington, DC American Psychiatry Association DSM-IV-TR (Text Revision)

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin D. A., Baird J. A. (2001). Discerning intentions in dynamic human action Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(4):171–178

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin D. A., Baird J. A., Saylor M. M., Clark M. A. (2001). Infants parse dynamic action Child Development 72(3):708–717

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S. (1995). Mindblindness. An essay on autism and theory of mind Cambridge MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S., Leslie A. M., Frith U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21:37–46

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S., Leslie A., Frith U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioural and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4:113–125

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartak L., Rutter M., Cox A. (1975). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific development receptive language disorder. I. The children British Journal of Psychiatry 126:127–145

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boddaert N., Zilbovicius M. (2002). Functional neuroimaging and childhood autism Pediatric Radiology 32(1):1–7

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter M., Pennington B. F., Rogers S. J. (2001). Understanding of others’ intentions in children with autism Journal of Autism and Development Disorders 31:589–599

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne E., Hesselink J. R., Jernigan T. L., Yeung-Courchesne R. (1987). Abnormal neuroanatomy in a nonretarded person with autism Archives of Neurology 44(3):335–341

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra G., Gergely G. (1998). The teleological origins of mentalistic action explanations: a developmental hypothesis Developmental Science 1(2):255–259

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curcio F., Piserchia E. A. (1978). Pantomimic representation in psychotic children Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia 8(2):181–189

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Defeyter M. A., German T. P. (2003) Acquiring an understanding of design: evidence from children’s insight problem solving Cognition 89:133–155

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeMeyer M. K., Alpern G. D., Barton S., DeMyer W. E., Churchill D. W., Hingten J. N., Bryson C. Q., Pontius W., Kimberlin C. (1972). Imitation in autistic, early schizophrenic and non-psychotic subnormal children Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia 2:264–287

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dickerson Mayes S., Calhoun S. L. (2003) Analysis of WISC-III, Stanford-Binet:IV, and Academic Achievement Test Scores in Children with Autism Journal of Autism Developmental Disorder 3:329–341

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan J. (1986). Disorganization of behaviour after frontal lobe damage Cognitive Neuropsychology 3(3):271–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma Blackwell Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith U. (2001). Mind blindness and the brain in autism Neuron 32(6):969–979

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster J. M. (1980). The prefrontal cortex. anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe New York Raven Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergely G., Csibra G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naive theory of rational action Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:287–292

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely G., Nadasdy Z., Csibra G., Biro S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age Cognition 56:165–93

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • German T. P., Defeyter M. A. (2000) Immunity to functional fixedness in young children Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7:707–712

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grafman J. (1995). Similarities and distinctions among current models of prefrontal cortical functions Annals of the NewYork Academy of Sciences 769:337–368

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammes J. G. W., Langdell T. (1981). Precursors of symbol formation and childhood autism Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 11:331–346

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism Child Development 66:843–855

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé F., Frith U. (1996). The neuropsychology of autism Brain 119:1377–1400

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes C., Russell J. (1993). Autistic children’s difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: its implications fot theories of autism Developmental Psychology 29(3):498–510

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes C., Russell J., Robbins T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism Neuropsychologia 32(4):477–492

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen D. (2004). Are children “intuitive theists”? Reasoning about purpose and design in nature Psychological Science 15(5):295–301

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen D. (1999). Function, goals and intention: children’s teleological reasoning about objects Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3:461–468

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmkuhl G., Poeck K. (1981). A disturbance in the conceptual organisation of actions in patients with ideational apraxia Cortex 17(1):153–158

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie A. M. (1987). Pretence and Representation: The Origins of “Theory of Mind” Psychological Review 94(4):412–426

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Losche G. (1990). Sensorimotor and action development in autistic children from infancy to early childhood Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 31(5):749–761

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luria A. R. 1966 Higher cortical functions in man New York Basic Books

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children Developmental Psychology 31:838–850

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muller R. A., Behen M. E., Rothermel R. D., Chugani D. C., Muzik O., Mangner T. J., Chugani H. T. (1999). Brain mapping of language and auditory perception in high-functioning autistic adults: a PET study Journal of Autism Developmental Disorder 29:19–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newtson D. (1973). Attribution and the unit of perception of ongoing behavior Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 28:28–38

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohnishi T., Matsuda H., Hashimoto T., Kunihiro T., Nishikawa M., Uema T., Sasaki M. (2000). Abnormal regional cerebral blood flow in childhood autism Brain 123:1838–1844

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff S., Pennington B. F., Rogers S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to Theory of mind Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 32:1081–1105

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff S., Rogers S. J., Pennington B. F. (1991). Asperger’s syndrome evidence of an empirical distinction from high-functioning autism Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 32:1107–1122

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington B. F., Ozonoff S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 37:51–87

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perner J., Frith U., Leslie A. M., Leekman S. R. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child’s theory of mind: knowledge, belief, and communication Child Development 60:688–700

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prior M. R., Dahlstrom B., Squire T. L. (1990). Autistic children’s knowledge of thinking and feeling states in other people Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 31:587–601

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell J., Saltmarsh R., Hill E. (1999). What do executive factors contribute to the failure on false belief tasks by children with autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 40:859–868

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Searle J. R. (1983) Intentionality: an essay in the phylosophy of mind Cambridge University Press Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Shallice T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences 298:199–209

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice T., Burgess N. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man Brain 114:727–741

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel D. J., Minshew N. J., Goldstein G. (1996). Wechsler IQ profiles in diagnosis of high-functioning autism Journal of Autism Developmental Disorder 26:389–406

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sirigu A., Zalla T., Pillon B., Grafman J., Agid Y., Dubois B. (1995). Selective impairments in managerial knowledge in patients with pre-frontal cortex lesions Cortex 31:301–316

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sirigu A., Zalla T., Pillon B., Grafman J., Agid Y., Dubois B. (1996). Encoding of sequence and boundaries of scripts following Pre-frontal Lesions Cortex 32:297–310

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith I. M., Bryson S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: a critical review Psychological Bulletin 116: 259–273

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M., Barton M. (1994) Learning words in nonostensive context Developemental Psychology 30:639–650

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner M. (1999). Annotation: repetitive behaviour in autism: a review of psychological research Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 40:839–849

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind Cambridge, MA MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman H. M., Woolley J. D., (1990) From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: the early development of everyday psychology Cognition 35:245–275

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward A. L. (1998) Infant selectivity encode the goal object of an actor’s reach Cognition 69:1–34

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zacks J. M., Tversky B. (2001). Event structure in perception and conception Psychological Bulletin 127(1):3–21

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zalla T., Labruyère N., Georgieff N. (2001a). Goal-directed action representation in autism: preliminary results Eight Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society New York, USA

    Google Scholar 

  • Zalla T., Plassiart C., Pillon B., Grafman J., Sirigu A. (2001b). Action planning in a virtual context after prefrontal cortex damage Neuropsychologia 39:759–770

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zalla T., Pradat-Diehl P., Sirigu A. (2003) Perception of Action Boundaries in Patients with Frontal Lobe Damage Neuropsychologia 41:1619–1627

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zilbovicius M., Garreau B., Samson Y., Remy P., Barthélémy C., Syrota A., Lelord G. (1995). Delayed maturation of the frontal cortex in childhood autism American Journal of Psychiatry 152:248–252

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a grant of the Fondation de France (2000–2002). We are grateful to Prof. Hochmann for permission to include in this study autistic patients under his care at the ITTAC, and F. Ferretti and the two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on the previous version of the manuscript. Preliminary results of this study were presented in part at scientific meetings (Zalla, Labruyère, & Georgieff, 2001a).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tiziana Zalla.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Zalla, T., Labruyere, N. & Georgieff, N. Goal-Directed Action Representation in Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 36, 527–540 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0092-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0092-3

Keywords

Navigation