Introduction

Stress, and stress-related behaviour, is a problem in aquaculture as it results in lowered productivity and reduced welfare of farmed fish (The EFSA Journal 2009). The general stress response of teleost fish is well studied (Wendelaar Bonga 1997), and in aquaculture the stress is often related to aggressive behaviour (Bergqvist and Gunnarsson 2011). Thus, research concerning aggression and understanding processes involved in aggression is necessary. In recent years, the neuropeptides arginine-vasopressin (AVP) in mammals, or the non-mammalian homologue arginine-vasotocin (AVT), and corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) have been suggested as factors modulating agonistic behaviour and aggression in vertebrates, including teleost fish.

The AVP/AVT is involved in the stress response as an adrenocorticotropin (ACTH) secretagogue in the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis in mammals, and in the hypothalamic-pituitary-interrenal (HPI) axis in teleost fish (Fryer et al. 1985; Gillies et al. 1982; Tonon et al. 1986). Moreover, several behaviours, including aggressive behaviour, seem to be modulated by the AVP/AVT system [see reviews by Balment et al. (2006), Godwin and Thompson (2012), and Goodson and Bass (2001)]. The effects of AVP are mediated by the V1A, V1B and V2 receptors (Balment et al. 2006; Godwin and Thompson 2012; Manning et al. 2008), and recently the homologues of these receptors have been cloned in teleost fish (Konno et al. 2010; Lema 2010). The V1A receptor and its non-mammalian homologue, the AVT receptor (AVT-R), are involved in the behavioural responses to stress (Castagna et al. 1998; Goodson and Bass 2001).

Corticotropin-releasing factor is also involved in the stress response as an ACTH secretagogue in the HPA/HPI axis (Gillies et al. 1982; Vale et al. 1981). Furthermore, the CRF system is an important mediator of several behavioural stress responses (see reviews by Bale and Vale (2004), Heinrichs and Koob (2004), Lowry and Moore (2006)), as well as aggression in vertebrates [see review by Backström and Winberg (2013)]. The effects of CRF are mediated by at least two different receptors, the CRF receptor 1 (CRF-R1) and the CRF receptor 2 (CRF-R2) [see reviews by Smagin et al. (2001) and Heinrichs and Koob (2004)].

Based on this background, we performed a study to evaluate the effects of aggression on AVT and CRF systems. To study the effects of social dominance on the mRNA expression of the AVT and CRF systems in teleost fish, we used juvenile rainbow trout Oncorhynchus mykiss (Walbaum, 1792) which are territorial and form strong dominance hierarchies (Chiszar et al. 1975; McIntyre et al. 1979). Previous studies have reported that aggression is attenuated by exogenous AVT (Backström and Winberg 2009) and CRF (Backström et al. 2011a) in rainbow trout. Similarly, mRNA levels of CRF have been reported to be upregulated in subordinate individuals (Bernier et al. 2008; Doyon et al. 2003). However, the effects of aggressive behaviour and dominance relationships upon mRNA expressions of AVT, AVT-R, CRF-R1 and CRF-R2 at the same time have not been examined in this species. Based on the studies using exogenous AVT, we hypothesized that mRNA expression of AVT and/or AVT-R would be upregulated in subordinate individuals. Similarly, our hypothesis concerning the CRF system was an upregulation of mRNA expression of CRF, CRF-R1 and/or CRF-R2 in subordinates based on the exogenous effect of CRF.

Here, we report the effects of 5 days inter-individual interactions in quadruples, thus establishing dominance–subordinance relationship, upon mRNA expression of AVT, CRF and their respective receptors. Furthermore, aggressive and foraging behaviours during the 5 days of interactions were monitored, and plasma cortisol levels were quantified to evaluate stress levels.

Materials and methods

Experimental animals

The experiments were performed on juvenile rainbow trout weighing 83.0 ± 15.2 g (mean ± SD, N = 27). Prior to the experiment, fish were kept indoors in a 1-m3 holding tank at the Evolutionary Biology Centre, Uppsala University, at a rearing density of approximately 0.02 kg/L for at least 1 week prior to the experiment. The holding tank was continuously supplied with aerated (> 90% O2 saturation) Uppsala tap water (pH 7.6, HCO3 5.2 mM, Ca2+ 2.8 mM, Mg2+ 0.4 mM) at 8–11 °C and the light/dark regime was continuously and automatically adjusted to latitude 51°N conditions. Fish were hand-fed with commercial trout pellets (EWOS ST40, Ewos AS, Bergen, Norway) at 1–2% of body mass per day.

Experimental protocol—dominance hierarchy

Juvenile rainbow trout randomly selected from the holding tank were lightly anaesthetized with ethyl-4-amino benzoate (0.25 g/L), weight matched in quadruples (deviation in weight less than 15%), tagged by either no, dorsal, ventral or dorsal and ventral cut in the caudal fin. Fish were socially isolated for 1 week in individual compartments, created by removable dark PVC walls in experimental aquaria (250 L), and continuously supplied with aerated Uppsala tap water (oxygen saturation > 90%, 0.8 L/min, 8–11 °C). Individual compartments were equally sized at 62.5 L (250 × 500 × 500 mm). The light/dark regimen was 12-h light/12-h dark (light on at 08.00 and light off at 20.00 h), light provided by a 30-W Lumilux daylight fluorescent tube placed 40 cm above the water surface of each aquarium. During social isolation, fish were fed commercial trout pellets (EWOS ST40) corresponding to 1% of their body mass per day. At the end of isolation, all individuals ate the entire ration.

Following isolation, the PVC walls were removed and a total of 8 quadruples were allowed to interact. Also, a PVC tube (length 25–27 cm, outer diameter 7.6 cm, inner diameter 6.7 cm, weighted down with a 50-mL Falcon tube filled with gravel) was put in each aquarium with the purpose to act as a hiding place. Interaction was continuous over 5 days and fish were fed the sum of earlier rations at a specific place of the aquaria. Agonistic interactions were observed in all quadruples for three different 5-min periods per day, the first in the morning 10.30–11.10, the second in the afternoon prior to feeding 15.10–15.50 and the third in the afternoon post feeding 15.50–16.30. During each 5-min period, the number of attacks performed per individual, as well as individuals receiving these attacks, was counted, and the number of times spent in or in the vicinity of the hiding place for every fish and an estimation of social rank based on general behaviour and demeanour were noted. The estimation of rank was mainly based on attacks performed; thus, the one performing most attacks in one aquarium was the alpha, the one with the second most attacks was the beta, and so on for gamma and delta. Furthermore, attacks received were also used to distinguish when two individuals had a similar number of performed attacks. Five individual fish were too badly injured by the interactions, and were removed from the aquarium and further analysis. During feeding, every individual eating got a score of 1 per day leading to a maximum possible score of 5. After 5 days, the fish were netted in estimated ranking order (alpha, beta, gamma and delta) and sacrificed using ethyl-4-amino benzoate (0.5 g/L). The fish were weighed and blood collected through the caudal vasculature with a heparinized syringe. Thereafter, the spinal cord was cut, the brain collected and sex was noted. The blood was spun at 16,000 g for 10 min at + 4 °C and the plasma collected, and the brain was wrapped in aluminium foil and frozen on liquid nitrogen. All samples were stored at − 80 °C until analysis.

Physiological analysis

Plasma was analysed for cortisol using a commercial enzyme linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) kit (product # 402,710, Neogen Corporation, Lexington, USA, delivered by Skafte Medlab, Onsala, Sweden) as described previously (Backström and Winberg 2009). In short, plasma samples were extracted in ethyl acetate and the organic phase evaporated and dissolved in buffer provided in the kit. The plates included in the kit were exposed to the samples and standards, the enzyme-conjugate and substrate. Prior to reading, 1 N HCl was added to stop enzyme reaction. Absorbance was read at 450 nm. All the samples were assayed in duplicates in one batch and the detection limit was 0.4 ng cortisol/mL, and an intra-assay coefficient of variation of 1–2.18%.

Quantitative PCR (qPCR) was used for analysing mRNA expression as described previously (Backström et al. 2011b). In short, RNA was extracted from brain samples using QIAzol Reagent (QIAGEN), DNAse-treated (DNA-free, Ambion, Austin, USA). The resulting RNA was checked for quality and cDNA was prepared using Stratascript (Stratagene, San Diego, USA). Primers for AVT, AVT-R, CRF, CRF-R1, CRF-R2 and elongation factor 1α (EF1α) as reference gene were used in the qPCR. The primers were 18–22 nucleotides long, melting point around 60 °C and products of 70–110 bp (see Table 1). The primer specificity was tested and efficiencies for primers were 80–100%.

Table 1 Primer design for quantitative PCR

Statistical analysis

All data were tested for normality using Shapiro–Wilk normality test, and if possible transformed to fit normality. Following this process, data was tested using Kruskal–Wallis test (non-parametric data), Welch two-sample one-tailed or two-tailed t-test (parametric data) (see “Results” section for motivation of not using the four ranks further). The free software R for statistical computing (R Core Team 2020) in the integrated development environment RStudio (RStudio Team 2019) was used for all statistical analyses. Fin clips (tagging) (Kruskal–Wallis test, P = 0.9118), sex (Kruskal–Wallis test, P = 0.2892) and weight before (two-tailed t-test, P = 0.6101) or after (two-tailed t-test, P = 0.7629) the experiment had no effects on social rank. Data are presented as mean ± s.e.m. if not stated otherwise. The methodology of this study was approved by the Uppsala Animal Research Ethical Committee.

Results

Dominance hierarchy

Generally, interactions started directly after removing compartment walls with all four fish involved in performing and receiving attacks. Already at the second 5-min period during the first day of interactions, dominance started to be stabilized with one easily identified alpha individual. However, beta, gamma and delta individuals were not so easily distinguished. When dominance was stabilized, the alpha individual was never usurped. During the study, it was noted that the hiding place was not commonly used, and therefore it was excluded from further analysis. Behavioural analysis of social interactions showed several differences between social ranks (Table 2). Dominant individuals had a higher feeding rate (Kruskal–Wallis test, P < 0.001), performed more attacks in the morning (Kruskal–Wallis test, P < 0.001) and performed more attacks in the afternoon prior to (Kruskal–Wallis test, P < 0.001) and post (Kruskal–Wallis test, P < 0.001) feeding. Dominant individuals also received fewer attacks in the morning (two-tailed t-test, P = 0.004), in the afternoon prior to (Kruskal–Wallis test, P < 0.001) and post feeding (Kruskal–Wallis test, P = 0.026). These differences were however only between alpha and the other ranks combined with no internal differences between beta, gamma and delta individuals. Therefore, these and the following analyses are divided into dominants, being alpha individuals, and subordinates, i.e. the beta, gamma and delta individuals taken together.

Table 2 The total number of attacks performed and received at three different 5-min periods and the total days of feeding during 5 days of social interacting in juvenile rainbow trout

After 5 days of social interaction, plasma cortisol levels were higher in subordinate compared to dominant individuals (Kruskal–Wallis test, P = 0.003, Fig. 1). Furthermore, mRNA levels of AVT-R (one-tailed t-test, P = 0.018, Fig. 2) and CRF (one-tailed t-test, P = 0.033, Fig. 2) were higher in subordinate compared to dominant individuals. However, there were no significant differences between social ranks in CRF-R1 (one-tailed t-test, P = 0.929, Fig. 2), CRF-R2 (one-tailed t-test, P = 0.293, Fig. 2) or AVT (one-tailed t-test, P = 0.261, Fig. 2) brain mRNA levels.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The effect of social status on plasma cortisol levels (ng/ml). Values are mean ± S.E.M and * indicates difference between dominant (N = 8) and subordinate individuals (N = 19) (P < 0.05, Kruskal–Wallis test)

Fig. 2
figure 2

The effect of social status on relative concentrations of AVT, AVT-R, CRF, CRF-R1 and CRF-R2 mRNA (arbitrary values). Values are mean ± S.E.M and * indicates difference between dominant and subordinate individuals (P < 0.05, Welch one-tailed t-test)

Discussion

Dominance behaviour and the stress axis

A clear dominance hierarchy was established in all groups of fish. Similar to several earlier studies, dominants showed higher food intake and were more aggressive than subordinates [see reviews by da Silva et al. (2021), Fulenwider et al. (2021), Goymann and Wingfield (2004), Sloman and Armstrong (2002), Winberg and Nilsson (1993)]. Furthermore, social subordination induced the typical plasma glucocorticoid elevation seen in previous studies (see reviews by Blanchard et al. (2001), da Silva et al. (2021), Sloman and Armstrong (2002)), and Summers and Winberg (2006)).

Arginine vasopressin/vasotocin and social behaviour

Arginine vasopressin and the non-mammalian homologue AVT affect social behaviour in vertebrates (Goodson and Bass 2001). Aggression seems to be modulated differently by AVP/AVT depending upon social system in all vertebrates. For instance, AVT stimulates aggression in colonial species (Goodson and Adkins-Regan 1999) and inhibits aggression in territorial species (Goodson 1998) of birds. Similarly, AVP shows divergent effects on aggressive pattern depending on social system in voles of the genera Microtus (Young et al. 1997). There is also evidence for diverging AVT effects depending on individual social status. In teleost fish for instance, AVT increases aggressive behaviour in non-territorial bluehead wrasse (Thalassoma bifasciatum) males (Semsar et al. 2001) and decreases aggressive behaviour in territorial Amargosa River pupfish (Cyprinodon nevadensis amargosae) males (Lema and Nevitt 2004a). Thus, the AVP/AVT effects on aggressive behaviour seem to be evolutionary conserved within specific social systems.

Earlier, we reported an attenuation of aggression by exogenous AVT (Backström and Winberg 2009). Therefore, we predicted an upregulation of AVT expression in brain of subordinate individuals, but no difference was observed between dominants and subordinates in this study. The reason for these non-conclusive results could depend on the use of whole brain for analyses. Several earlier studies show differences in AVP/AVT neurons of specific areas in the brain. AVP/AVT neurons are typically found in the magnocellular and parvocellular cells of the preoptic area-anterior hypothalamus (POA-AH) in vertebrates (Goodson and Bass 2001). In mammals, there is evidence that variation in AVP expression in the POA-AH relates to aggressive behaviour. For instance, experimentally induced changes in AVP seem to affect aggression in Syrian hamsters (Mesocritus auratus) (Ferris and Delville 1994; Ferris et al. 1986; Ferris and Potegal 1988) and in male prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) (Gobrogge et al. 2009). Furthermore, AVP distribution differs in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) between two closely related species of the mouse genera Peromyscus with divergent aggressive behaviour (Bester-Meredith et al. 1999), and in rats (Rattus norvegicus) AVP release in the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) differed between active and passive intruders (Ebner et al. 2005). Similar patterns of association between hypothalamic AVT and aggression are also apparent in teleost fish. For instance, in zebrafish (Danio rerio), dominant individuals exhibit AVT-immunoreactive cells in magnocellular POA, whereas subordinate individuals exhibit AVT-immunoreactive cells in parvocellular POA (Larson et al. 2006). Furthermore, the territorial multiband butterflyfish (Chaetodon multicintus) has larger AVT immunoreactive somata in the POA and higher AVT fibre densities in parts of the telencephalon than the non-territorial milletseed butterflyfish (Chaetodon miliaris) (Dewan et al. 2008) and similar findings have been reported concerning different populations with diverging aggressiveness of the Death Valley pupfish (Cyprinodon nevadensis) (Lema and Nevitt 2004b). Similarly, in an African cichlid species (Astatotilapia burtoni), territorial males exhibit higher levels of AVT mRNA expression in the posterior POA (gigantocellular nucleus) than non-territorial males whereas in the anterior POA (parvocellular nucleus) AVT mRNA expression is lower in territorial males than that in non-territorial males (Greenwood et al. 2008). The AVT distribution is well studied in rainbow trout and the POA is dense with AVT neurons (Saito et al. 2004). Thus, even though our results did not establish a divergence in AVT mRNA expression between dominant and subordinate individuals, these earlier reports indicate that AVT expression could be affected in specific areas of the brain. The expression in these areas, specifically the parvocellular and magnocellular POA, could cancel each other out and therefore not be measurable in whole-brain samples.

In mammals, the behavioural effects of AVP, including aggression, across both social system and social status, seem to be mediated by the V1A receptor. For instance, in Microtus voles with different social systems, the brain distribution of V1A receptor also differs and apparently gives this divergence in social system (Young et al. 1997), and male mice (Mus musculus) transfected with the Microtus vole V1A receptor display similar behavioural effects (Young et al. 1999). In male Syrian hamsters, V1A receptor was upregulated in dominants (Cooper et al. 2005). Similarly, studies using the V1A receptor antagonist Manning compound in teleost fish report behavioural effects. For example, Manning compound decreases courtship and territorial defence in territorial males of bluehead wrasse (Thalassoma bifasciatum) (Semsar et al. 2001), and decreases aggression in male beaugregory damselfish (Stegastes leucostictus) (Santangelo and Bass 2006). Some indications of Manning compound affecting behaviour in rainbow trout have also been reported. Fish receiving Manning compound prolonged time for dyadic fights when becoming subordinate (Backström and Winberg 2009). Thus, our results that social stress upregulates transcript levels of AVT-R in subordinates fit well with the hypothesis that aggression in territorial animals is attenuated by AVP/AVT.

Corticotropin-releasing factor, and stress related behaviour

Similar to AVP/AVT, CRF is involved in several behavioural stress responses (Lowry and Moore 2006; Smagin et al. 2001). Among the behaviours affected during stress are feeding and appetite (Bernier 2006; De Pedro et al. 1993; Koob and Heinrichs 1999), aggressive behaviour (Elkabir et al. 1990; Mele et al. 1987) and anxiety-related behaviour (Lowry and Moore 2006; Risbrough and Stein 2006). Furthermore, these responses are usually associated with specific brain areas. For instance, several stressors induce CRF expression in the hypothalamus (Imaki et al. 1991) and PVN of rats (Aubry et al. 1999; Harbuz et al. 1993; Imaki et al. 1991). These patterns are also apparent in teleost fish. For instance, hyperosmotic stress induces CRF expression 24 h post stress in the hypothalamus and the POA of rainbow trout (Craig et al. 2005). Furthermore, repeated chasing stress, isolation stress for at least 24 h and confinement stress for at least 4 h elevated CRF expression in the POA of rainbow trout (Doyon et al. 2005). Interestingly, CRF expression in the POA of rainbow trout was elevated after subordination stress following 72 h of interactions in dyads (Doyon et al. 2003), whereas in Astatotilapia burtoni territorial males have higher CRF expression in brain compared to non-territorial males after 4 weeks of social interaction (Chen and Fernald 2008). In our study, CRF was upregulated in subordinate compared to dominant individuals, following the earlier studies as well as our hypothesis.

Aggressive behaviour has been reported to be affected by CRF. For instance, CRF reduced aggression in mice (Mele et al. 1987), whereas low doses of CRF increase aggression and high doses of CRF reduce aggression in rats (Elkabir et al. 1990). In mice, maternal aggression is also reduced by CRF (Gammie et al. 2005) and the two CRF-related peptides Urocortin 1 and Urocortin 3 (D'Anna et al. 2005). Furthermore, the use of CRF antagonists modulates aggressive behaviour. For instance, the CRF-R1 antagonist SSR125543A delayed latency to attack intruders in Syrian hamsters (Farrokhi et al. 2004) and the CRF-R1 antagonist antalarmin reduces defensive posture in socially defeated mice (Robison et al. 2004). In mice selected for aggression, CRF mRNA expression was higher in the less aggressive line 24 h post swim stress (Veenema et al. 2003). Thus, CRF seems to be involved in aggressive behavioural modulation in mammals but not in a consistent pattern. However, the higher CRF expression in subordinates in this study together with an earlier report concerning CRF attenuating aggression (Backström et al. 2011a) supports the hypothesis, although questioned (Carpenter et al. 2009) that CRF reduces aggression and dominance behaviour in rainbow trout.

Corticotropin-releasing factor has at least two different receptors mediating its effects, namely CRF-R1 and CRF-R2 (Bale and Vale 2004; Flik et al. 2006). CRF-R1 is probably involved in the HPA/HPI axis activity and is found in the pituitary gland in teleost fish (Flik et al. 2006). Several studies report that CRF-R1 is involved in stress responses. For instance, CRF-R1-deficient mice have depleted stress response (Bale et al. 2002). In crucian carp (Carassius carassius), intraperitoneal injection of antalarmin reduces plasma cortisol during skin extract exposure (Lastein et al. 2008). Huising et al. (2004) reported that CRF-R1 is expressed in the hypothalamus and the pituitary gland, and also showed that CRF-R1 is downregulated after 24 h of restraint stress in the pituitary gland of common carp (Cyprinius carpio). Since CRF-R1 seems to be tightly linked to plasma cortisol, differences in expression could be apparent between social ranks. However, in our study, no differences were seen in expression of CRF-R1 mRNA.

On the other hand, CRF-R2 has been connected to the behavioural responses of stress (Flik et al. 2006). Among these behaviours are anxiety-related behaviours, although not exclusively linked to CRF-R2 (Rotzinger et al. 2010). Recently, an anxiety-like behaviour evoked by CRF has been reported in rainbow trout (Carpenter et al. 2007). We also observed a similar head-shake behaviour after administration of both CRF and UI and suggested that the head-shake was mediated by the CRF-R2 (Backström et al. 2011a). In the present study, CRF-R2 expression was not different between dominant and subordinate individuals.

Conclusions

In conclusion, dominance/subordinance relationship affects AVT-R and CRF expression as well as plasma cortisol. Thus, the apparent attenuating effect on aggression by AVT could be mediated by AVT-R. Furthermore, the attenuating effect of aggression by the CRF system could be mediated by CRF. Finally, more studies are needed to elucidate mRNA expression of AVT, CRF and their respective receptors in specific areas of the brain, and the most promising area seems to be the POA.