Skip to main content
Log in

Inversion in mathematical thinking and learning

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Inversion is a fundamental relational building block both within mathematics as the study of structures and within people’s physical and social experience, linked to many other key elements such as equilibrium, invariance, reversal, compensation, symmetry, and balance. Within purely formal arithmetic, the inverse relationships between addition and subtraction, and multiplication and division, have important implications in relation to flexible and efficient computation, and for the assessment of students’ conceptual understanding. It is suggested that the extensive research on arithmetic should be extended to take account of numerical domains beyond the natural numbers and of the difficulties students have in extending the meanings of operations to those of more general domains. When the range of situations modelled by the arithmetical operations is considered, the complexity of inverse relationships between operations, and the variability in the forms that these relationships take, become much greater. Finally, some comments are offered on the divergent goals and preoccupations of cognitive psychologists and mathematics educators as illuminated by research in this area.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Berliner, D. C. (2002). Educational research: The hardest science of all. Educational Researcher, 31(8), 18–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, J. H., & Guy, R. K. (1996). The book of numbers. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • De Corte, E., Greer, B., & Verschaffel, L. (1996). Mathematics teaching and learning. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 491–549). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Corte, E., & Verschaffel, L. (1987). The effect of semantic structure on first graders’ strategies for solving addition and subtraction word problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 18, 363–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Morgan, A. (1910). Study and difficulties of mathematics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press [Originally 1831].

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E., Deri, M., Nello, M. S., & Marino, M. S. (1995). The role of implicit models in solving verbal problems in multiplication and division. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 16(1), 3–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freudenthal, H. (1983). Didactical phenomenology of mathematical structures. Dordrecht: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freudenthal, H. (1991). Revisiting mathematics education. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuson, K. C. (1992). Research on whole number addition and subtraction. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 243–275). New York: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, B. (1992). Multiplication and division as models of situations. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 276–295). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, B. (1994). Extending the meaning of multiplication and division. In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of multiplicative reasoning in the learning of mathematics (pp. 61–85). Albany: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, B. (2009). Helping children develop mathematically. Human Development, 52, 148–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, B., & Harel, G. (1998). The role of isomorphisms in mathematical cognition. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 17(1), 5–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, B., & Mangan, C. (1986). Understanding multiplication and division: From 10-year-olds to student teachers. In L. Burton & C. Hoyles (Eds.), Proceedings of the tenth international conference for the psychology of mathematics education (pp. 25–30). London: London Institute of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, B., & Verschaffel, L. (1990). Introduction to the special issue on mathematics as a proving ground for information-processing theories. International Journal for Educational Research, 14, 3–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harel, G., Behr, M., Post, T., & Lesh, R. (1994). The impact of the number type on the solution of multiplication and division problems: Further considerations. In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of multiplicative reasoning in the learning of mathematics (pp. 363–384). Albany: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kilpatrick, J. (1981). The reasonable ineffectiveness of research in mathematics education. For the Learning of Mathematics, 2(2), 22–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Nunez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maher, C. A., Powell, A. B., & Uptegrove, E. B. (2010). Combinatorics and reasoning: Representing, justifying, and building isomorphisms. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1969). The child’s conception of number. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul [Originally 1941].

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Stern, E. (1998). Conscious and unconscious strategy discoveries: A microgenetic analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 127, 377–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Urton, G. (1997). The social life of numbers: A Quechua ontology of numbers and philosophy of arithmetic. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. (1996). Assessment and realistic mathematics education. Utrecht: Utrecht University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vergnaud, G. (2009). The theory of conceptual fields. Human Development, 52, 83–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1982). Productive thinking. Chicago: University of Chicago Press [Originally 1945].

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brian Greer.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Greer, B. Inversion in mathematical thinking and learning. Educ Stud Math 79, 429–438 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-011-9317-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-011-9317-2

Keywords

Navigation