Skip to main content
Log in

Situational Moral Disengagement: Can the Effects of Self-Interest be Mitigated?

  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Self-interest has long been recognized as a powerful human motive. Yet, much remains to be understood about the thinking behind self-interested pursuits. Drawing from multiple literatures, we propose that situations high in opportunity for self-interested gain trigger a type of moral cognition called moral disengagement that allows the individual to more easily disengage internalized moral standards. We also theorize two countervailing forces—situational harm to others and dispositional conscientiousness—that may weaken the effects of personal gain on morally disengaged reasoning. We test our hypotheses in two studies using qualitative and quantitative data and complementary research methods and design. We demonstrate that when personal gain incentives are relatively moderate, reminders of harm to others can reduce the likelihood that employees will morally disengage. Furthermore, when strong personal gain incentives are present in a situation, highly conscientious individuals are less apt than their counterparts to engage in morally disengaged reasoning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Raters assessed (on a five-point Likert scale), “the extent to which you agree/disagree that the scenario represents a behavior that involves harm to others.” Harm ratings ranged from 2.60 to 4.47 across the scenarios; ICC(2) was .95 (Bartko 1976; LeBreton and Senter 2008).

  2. Two participants’ data were removed from the analysis because they indicated they were suspicious about the true purpose of the “work opportunity”. Two additional participants’ data were removed from the analysis for unruly behavior, suggesting that they were not treating the task or the paperwork responsibly.

  3. We selected a rate of $1.25/page after conducting pilot sessions at two universities not involved in the Study 2 experiment. We determined (by paying a generous flat rate and exhorting students to do their best for 15 min) that at a rate of $1.25/page, most students doing the job carefully (i.e., accurately finding the errors) would be able to earn $10 or more for just 30 min of work. Thus, even the baseline personal gain condition was quite generous considering the worker population in this task.

  4. To determine an appropriate level for the enhanced personal gain bonus condition, a pilot study was conducted at a private liberal arts college in the Midwest. The 68 pilot participants were recruited from three courses offered in management and accounting, and ranged from sophomores to seniors. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three personal gain conditions—no bonus beyond piece rate, $5 bonus, and $10 bonus. Based on analysis—e.g., changes in behavior in response to the different personal gain conditions—and information gleaned from debriefs, it was decided that a $10 bonus condition (essentially doubling the amount of money that could be earned) significantly enhanced recognition of opportunity for personal gain without evoking high suspicion.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashforth, B. E., & Anand, V. (2003). The normalization of corruption in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 25, 1–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 364–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., & Zsolnai, L. (2000). Corporate transgressions through moral disengagement. Journal of Human Values, 6(1), 57–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., & Regalia, C. (2001). Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms governing transgressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 125–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrick, M., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The big five personality dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personality Psychology, 44, 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrick, M., Mount, M. K., & Judge, T. (2001). Personality and performance at the beginning of the new millennium: What do we know and where do we go next? International Journal of Selection Assessment, 9, 9–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartko, J. J. (1976). On various interclass correlation reliability coefficients. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 762–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Lishner, D. A., Carpenter, A., Dulin, L., Harjusola-Webb, S., Stocks, E. L., et al. (2003). “…As you would have them to unto you”: Does imagining yourself in the other’s place stimulate moral action? Personality and Social Psychology, 29(9), 1190–1201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, T. E. (1998). Integrity in organizations: Beyond honesty and conscientiousness. Academy of Management Review, 23(1), 154–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, C. M., Ones, D. S., & Sackett, P. R. (2007). Interpersonal deviance, organizational deviance, and their common correlates: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(2), 410–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bersoff, D. M. (1999). Explaining unethical behavior among people motivated to act prosocially. Journal of Moral Education, 28(4), 413–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cawley, M. J., I. I. I., Martin, J. E., & Johnson, J. A. (2000). A virtues approach to personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 997–1013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Claybourn, M. (2011). Relationships between moral disengagement, work characteristics, and workplace harassment. Journal of Business Ethics, 100, 283–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J. (2001). Motivating cognitive change: The self-standards model of dissonance. In J. P. Forgas, K. D. Williams, & S. C. Wheeler (Eds.), The social mind: Cognitive and motivational aspects of interpersonal behavior (pp. 72–91). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cressey, D. R. (1953). A study in the social psychology of embezzlement: Other people’s money. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cropanzano, R., Stein, J., & Goldman, B. M. (2007). Self-interest. In J. Bailey (Ed.), Handbook of organizational and managerial wisdom. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Del Barrio, V., Aluja, A., & Garcia, L. F. (2004). Relationship between empathy and the big five personality traits in a sample of Spanish adolescents. Social Behavior and Personality, 32(7), 677–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Detert, J. R., Treviño, L. K., & Sweitzer, V. L. (2008). Moral disengagement in ethical decision making: A study of antecedents and outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(2), 374–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ditto, P. H., Pizarro, D. A., & Tannenbaum, D. (2009). Motivated moral reasoning. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 50, 307–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, M. K., Aquino, K., Tepper, B. J., Reed, A., & O’Leary-Kelly, A. M. (2005). Moral disengagement and social identification: When does being similar result in harm doing? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Academy of Management.

  • Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 665–697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Funder, D. C., & Fast, L. A. (2010). Personality in social psychology. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 668–697). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan, C. (1977). In a different voice: Women’s conceptions of the self and morality. Harvard Educational Review, 49, 431–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gioia, D. A. (1992). Pinto fires and personal ethics: A script analysis of missed opportunities. Journal of Business Ethics, 11(5–6), 379–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glovin, D., Kishan, S., Hurtado, P., & Burton, K. (2011, February 9). SAC Ex-portfolio managers accused in trading probe. Bloomberg BusinessWeek.

  • Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five-factor models. In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. De Fruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 7–28). Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J. W., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 366–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grover, S. L., & Hui, C. (1994). The influence of role conflict and self-interest on lying in organizations. Journal of Business Ethics, 13(4), 295–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., & Kesebir, S. (2010). Morality. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., Vol. 2). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinrichs, K. T., Wang, L., Hinrichs, A. T., & Romero, E. J. (2012). Moral disengagement through displacement of responsibility: The role of leadership beliefs. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42(1), 62–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. Academy of Management Review, 16(2), 366–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, T. M., & Ryan, L. V. (1997). The link between ethical judgment and action in organizations: A moral approbation approach. Organization Science, 8(6), 663–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelman, H. C., & Hamilton, V. L. (1989). Crimes of obedience. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kish-Gephart, J. J., Harrison, D. A., & Treviño, L. K. (2010). Bad apples, bad cases, and bad barrels: Meta-analytic evidence about sources of unethical decisions at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Belanger, J. J., Chen, X., Kopetz, C., Pierro, A., & Mannetti, L. (2012). The energetics of motivated cognition: A force-field analysis. Psychological Review, 119(1), 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kutner, M. H., Nachtsheim, C. J., Neter, J., & Li, W. (2005). Applied linear statistical models (5th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeBreton, J. M., & Senter, J. L. (2008). Answers to 20 questions about interrater reliability and interrater agreement. Organizational Research Methods, 11(4), 815–852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lodi-Smith, J., & Roberts, B. W. (2007). Social investment and personality: A meta-analysis of the relationship of personality traits to investment in work, family, religion, and volunteerism. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 68–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maruna, S., & Copes, H. (2005). Excuses, excuses: What have we learned from five decades of neutralization research? Crime and Justice, 32, 221–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P. (2009). The moral personality. In D. Narvaez & D. K. Lapsley (Eds.), Personality, identity, and character: Explorations in moral psychology (pp. 11–29). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60(2), 175–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGrath, J. E. (1982). Dilemmatics: The study of research choices. In J. E. McGrath, J. Martin, & R. A. Kulka (Eds.), Judgment calls in research (pp. 69–102). Beverly Hills: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLean, B., & Nocera, J. (2010). All the devils are here: The hidden history of the financial crisis. New York: Penguin Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. (1999). The norm of self-interest. American Psychologist, 54(12), 1053–1060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C. (2008). Moral disengagement in processes of organizational corruption. Journal of Business Ethics, 80, 129–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. A., & Lowenstein, G. (2004). Self-interest, automaticity, and the psychology of conflict of interest. Social Justice Research, 17(2), 189–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C., Detert, J. R., Treviño, L. K., Baker, V. L., & Mayer, D. M. (2012). Why employees do bad things: Moral disengagement and unethical organizational behavior. Personnel Psychology, 65(1), 1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nocera, J. (2011, March 25). In prison for taking a liar loan. New York Times.

  • Ressler, P., & Mitchell, M. (2011). Conversations with Wall Street: The inside story of the financial Armageddon and how to prevent the next one. New York: FastPencil Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and theory. New York: Praeger Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, B. W., & Hogan, R. (2001). Personality psychology in the workplace. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schweitzer, M. E., Ordonez, L., & Douma, B. (2004). Goal setting as a motivator of unethical behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 47(3), 422–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sen, A. K. (1977). Rational fools: A critique of the behavioral foundations of economic theory. Philosophy & Public Affairs, 6(4), 317–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shu, L. L., Gino, F., & Bazerman, M. H. (2009). Dishonest deed, clear conscience: Self-preservation through moral disengagement and motivated forgetting. Harvard Business School Working Paper Series, no. 09-078.

  • Smith, A. (2011, February 28). Madoff says his victims were ‘greedy’. CNNMoney.

  • Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: A theory of delinquency. American Sociological Review, 22, 664–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 271–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenbrunsel, A., & Messick, D. M. (1999). Sanctioning systems, decision frames, and cooperation. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44(4), 684–707.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenbrunsel, A., & Smith-Crowe, K. (2008). Ethical decision making: Where we’ve been and where we’re going. Academy of Management Annals, 2(1), 545–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treviño, L. K. (1986). Ethical decision making in organizations: A person–situation interactionist model. Academy of Management Review, 11(3), 601–617.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsang, J. (2002). Moral rationalization and the integration of situational factors and psychological processes in immoral behavior. Review of General Psychology, 6(1), 25–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walumbwa, F. O., & Schaubroeck, J. (2009). Leader personality traits and employee voice behavior: Mediating roles of ethical leadership and work group psychological safety. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(5), 1275–1286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L., & Murnighan, J. K. (2011). On greed. Academy of Management Annals, 5(1), 279–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We extend our thanks to Vikas Anand, Mike Brown, Dan Chiaburu, David Harrison, Nate Petitt, and the members of the ORG seminar at Penn State for their feedback on earlier drafts.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jennifer Kish-Gephart.

Appendix

Appendix

Study 2 Proofreading Activity: Screens One and Two

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kish-Gephart, J., Detert, J., Treviño, L.K. et al. Situational Moral Disengagement: Can the Effects of Self-Interest be Mitigated?. J Bus Ethics 125, 267–285 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-013-1909-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-013-1909-6

Keywords

Navigation