Skip to main content
Log in

Using EEG to Explore How rTMS Produces Its Effects on Behavior

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Brain Topography Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A commonly held view is that, when delivered during the performance of a task, repetitive TMS (rTMS) influences behavior by producing transient “virtual lesions” in targeted tissue. However, findings of rTMS-related improvements in performance are difficult to reconcile with this assumption. With regard to the mechanism whereby rTMS influences concurrent task performance, a combined rTMS/EEG study conducted in our lab has revealed a complex set of relations between rTMS, EEG activity, and behavioral performance, with the effects of rTMS on power in the alpha band and on alpha:gamma phase synchrony each predicting its effect on behavior. These findings suggest that rTMS influences performance by biasing endogenous task-related oscillatory dynamics, rather than creating a “virtual lesion”. To further differentiate these two alternatives, in the present study we compared the effects of 10 Hz rTMS on neural activity with the results of an experiment in which rTMS was replaced with 10 Hz luminance flicker. We reasoned that 10 Hz flicker would produce widespread entrainment of neural activity to the flicker frequency, and comparison of these EEG results with those from the rTMS study would shed light on whether the latter also reflected entrainment to an exogenous stimulus. Results revealed pronounced evidence for “entrainment noise” produced by 10 Hz flicker—increased oscillatory power and inter-trial coherence (ITC) at the driving frequency, and increased alpha:gamma phase synchronization—that were nonetheless largely uncorrelated with behavior. This contrasts markedly with 10-Hz rTMS, for which the only evidence for stimulation-induced noise, elevated ITC at 30 Hz, differed qualitatively from the flicker results. Simultaneous recording of the EEG thus offers an important means of directly testing assumptions about how rTMS exerts its effects on behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In this context, we use “noise” to refer to any neural activity elicited by an exogenous stimulus not specifically related to the task. This is a slightly different usage than in Harris et al. (2008), in which the effects of adding image noise (manipulated by superimposing varying levels of white noise over a test image) were compared to the effects of TMS on visual discrimination thresholds.

  2. Additional artifact rejection procedures used in the Hamidi, et al. (2009a) study, aimed at removing TMS-induced electrical artifacts, are described in the Appendix.

  3. Given that rTMS was applied at 10 Hz in the Hamidi et al. (2009a) study, it is possible that some entrainment-related neural activity was removed along with TMS-induced electrical artifacts. However, TMS-related artifacts have a number of distinct characteristics that enabled us to minimize this possibility (although we can not entirely rule it out). These are discussed in greater depth in the Appendix.

  4. This overall improvement is not without precedent, in that other studies have produced nonspecific improvement in performance with task-irrelevant “distraction” vs. unfilled delays, when testing takes place in a darkened room (e.g., Postle and Hamidi, 2007; Postle et al., 2006b).

References

  • Arnoult MD, Atteave F (1956) The quantitative study of shape and pattern perception. Psychol Bull 53:452–471

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delorme A, Makeig S (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Esser SK, Hill SL, Tononi G (2005) Modeling the effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on cortical circuits. J Neurophysiol 94:622–639

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feredoes E, Tononi G, Postle BR (2006) Direct evidence for a prefrontal contribution to the control of proactive interference in verbal working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci 103:19530–19534

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrarelli F, Massimini M, Peterson MJ, Riedner BA, Lazar M, Murphy MJ, Huber R, Rosanova M, Alexander AL, Kalin N, Tononi G (2008) Reduced evoked gamma oscillations in the frontal cortex in schizophrenia patients: a TMS/EEG study. Am J Psychiatry 165:996–1005

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grosbras MH, Paus T (2003) Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field facilitates visual awareness. Eur J Neurosci 18:3121–3126

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamidi M, Tononi G, Postle BR (2008) Evaluating frontal and parietal contributions to spatial working memory with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Res 1230:202–210

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamidi M, Slagter HA, Tononi G, Postle BR (2009a) Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation affects behavior by biasing endogenous cortical oscillations. Front Integr Neurosci 3:14. doi:10.3389/neuro.07.014.2009

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamidi M, Tononi G, Postle BR (2009b) Evaluating the role of prefrontal and parietal cortices in memory-guided response with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuropsychologia 47:295–302

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamidi M, Slagter HA, Tononi G, Postle BR (in press) Brain responses evoked by high-frequency repetitive TMS: an ERP study. Brain Stimulat. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2009.04.001

  • Harris JA, Clifford CWG, Miniussi C (2008) The functional effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation: Signal suppression or neural noise generation? J Cogn Neurosci 20:734–740

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heerrmann CS (2001) Human EEG responses to 1–100 Hz Flicker: resonance phenomena in visual cortex and their potential correlation to cognitive phenomena. Exp Brain Res 137:346–353

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang Y-Z, Edwards MJ, Rounis E, Bhatia KP, Rothwell JC (2005) Theta burst stimulation of the motor cortex. Neuron 45:201–206

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ilmoniemi R, Virtanen J, Ruohonen J, Karhu J, Aronen H, Naatanen R, Katila T (1997) Neuronal responses to magnetic stimulation reveal cortical reactivity and connectivity. Neuroreport 8:3537–3540

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jokisch D, Jensen O (2007) Modulation of gamma and alpha activity during a working memory task engaging the dorsal or ventral stream. J Neurosci 27:3244–3251

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahkonen S, Komssi S, Wilenius J, Ilmoniemi RJ (2005) Prefrontal transcranial magnetic stimulation produces intensity-dependent EEG responses in humans. NeuroImage 24:955–960

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Gerloff C (2003) Enhancing cognitive performance with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at human individual alpha frequency. Eur J Neurosci 17:1129–1133

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Komssi S, Kahkonen S, Ilmoniemi R (2004) The effect of stimulus intensity on brain responses evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Hum Brain Mapp 21:154–164

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luber B, Kinnunen LH, Rakitin BC, Ellsasser R, Stern Y, Lisanby SH (2007) Facilitation of performance in a working memory task with rTMS stimulation of the precuneus: frequency- and time-dependent effects. Brain Res 1128:120–129

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Makeig S (1993) Auditory event-related dynamics of the EEG spectrum and effects of exposure to tones. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 86:283–293

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Makeig S, Westerfield M, Jung TP, Enghoff S, Townsend J, Courchesne E, Sejnowski TJ (2002) Dynamic brain sources of visual evoked responses. Science 295:690–694

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Massimini M, Ferrarelli F, Huber R, Esser SK, Singh H, Tononi G (2005) Breakdown of cortical effective connectivity during sleep. Science 309:2228–2232

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miniussi C, Ruzzoli M, Walsh V (in press) The mechanism of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation in cognition. Cortex. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.03.004

  • Palva S, Palva JM (2007) New vistas for α-frequency band oscillations. Trends Neurosci 30:150–158

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palva JM, Palva S, Kaila K (2005) Phase synchrony among neuronal oscillations in the human cortex. J Neurosci 25:3962–3972

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone A, Bartres-Faz D, Keenan J (1999) Transcranial magnetic stimulation: studying the brain-behavior relationship by induction of ‘virtual lesions’. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 354:1229–1238

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pasley BN, Allen EA, Freeman RD (2009) State-dependent variability of neuronal responses to transcranial magnetic stimulation of the visual cortex. Neuron 62:291–303

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perrin F, Pernier J, Bertrand O, Echallier JF (1989) Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 72:184–187

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Postle BR, Hamidi M (2007) Nonvisual codes and nonvisual brain areas support visual working memory. Cereb Cortex 17:2134–2142

    Google Scholar 

  • Postle BR, Ferrarelli F, Hamidi M, Feredoes E, Massimini M, Peterson M, Alexander A, Tononi G (2006a) Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation dissociates working memory manipulation from retention functions in the prefrontal but not posterior parietal, cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 18:1712–1722

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Postle BR, Idzikowski C, Della Salla S, Logie RH, Baddeley AD (2006b) The selective disruption of spatial working memory by eye movements. Q J Exp Psychol 59:100–120

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Regan D (1977) Steady-state evoked potentials. J Opt Soc Am 67:1475–1489

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosanova M, Casali A, Bellina V, Resta F, Mariotti M, Massimini M (2009) Natural frequencies of human corticothalamic circuits. J Neurosci 29:7679–7685

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sharbrough F, Chatrian G-E, Lesser RP, Lüders H, Nuwer M, Picton TW (1991) American Electroencephalographic Society guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. J Clin Neurophysiol 8:200–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silvanto J, Muggleton NG (2008) New light through old windows: moving beyond the “virtual lesion” approach to transcranial magnetic stimulation. NeuroImage 39:549–552

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silvanto J, Muggleton NG, Cowey A, Walsh V (2007) Neural adaptation reveals state-dependent effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Eur J Neurosci 25:1874–1881

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinkkonen J, Tiitinen H, Naatanen R (1995) Gabor filters: an informative way of analysing eventy-related brain activity. J Neurosci Methods 56:99–104

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sperkeijse H, Estevez O, Reits D (1977) Visual evoked potentials and the physiological analysis of visual processes in man. In: Desmedt JE (ed) Visual evoked potentials in man: new developments. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 16–89

    Google Scholar 

  • Srinivasan R, Bibi FA, Nuñez PL (2006) Steady-state visual evoked potentials: distributed local sources and wave-like dynamics are sensitive to flicker frequency. Brain Topogr 18:167–187

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O, Delpuech C, Pernier J (1996) Stimulus specificity of phase-locked and non-phase-locked 40 Hz visual responses in humans. J Neurosci 16:4240–4249

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tass P, Rosenblum MG, Weule J, Kurths J, Pikovsky A, Volkmann J, Schnitzler A, Freund H-J (1998) Detection of n:m phase locking from noisy data: application to magnetoencephalography. Phys Rev Lett 81:3291–3294

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Thut G, Miniussi C (2009) New insights into rhythmic brain activity from TMS–EEG studies. Trends Cogn Sci 13:182–189

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Töpper R, Mottaghy FM, Brügmann M, Noth J, Huber W (1998) Facilitation of picture naming by focal transcranial magnetic stimulation of Wernicke’s area. Exp Brain Res 121:371–378

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Virtanen J, Ruohonen J, Näätanen R, Ilmoniemi RJ (1999) Instrumentation for the measurement of electric brain responses to transcranial magnetic stimulation. Med Biol Eng Comput 37:322–326

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh V, Cowey A (1998) Magnetic stimulation studies of visual cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 2:103–110

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh V, Pascual-Leone A (2003) Transcranial magnetic stimulation: a neurochronometrics of mind. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Walsh V, Rushworth M (1999) A primer of magnetic stimulation as a tool for neuropsychology. Neuropsychologia 37:125–135

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams JH (2001) Frequency-specific effects of flicker on recognition memory. Neuroscience 104:283–286

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ziemann U (in press) TMS in cognitive neuroscience: virtual lesion and beyond. Cortex. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.02.020

Download references

Acknowledgments

The flicker and rTMS experiments were both performed in the laboratory of Giulio Tononi, and many members of this research group provided valuable technical assistance and discussions of the results. The work was supported by R01 MH069448 (B.R.P.), F32 MH088115 (J.S.J.), F30 MH078705 (M.H.), and NARSAD (G.T.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jeffrey S. Johnson.

Additional information

This is one of several papers published together in Brain Topography on the “Special Topic: TMS and EEG”.

Appendix

Appendix

To facilitate comparison of the present results with the results of Hamidi et al. (2009), we include in the following section relevant details of the experimental methods used in that study and in Hamidi et al. (in press).

rTMS

rTMS was applied to two different brain areas: the superior parietal lobule (SPL) and the postcentral gyrus (PCG). The area representing the leg in the somatosensory cortex of the PCG served as a control region for the behavioral study. Both areas were identified based on individual anatomy from whole-brain anatomical MRIs that were obtained for each subject prior to the study (GE Signa VH/I, 256 sagittal slices, 0.5 mm × 0.5 mm × 0.8 mm). An infrared-based frameless stereotaxy system was used to accurately target each brain area with the TMS coil (eXimia NBS, Nexstim, Helsinki, Finland). For all subjects rTMS was applied to the left hemisphere.

In rTMSpresent trials, a 10-Hz rTMS train (110% of resting motor threshold) was applied during the entire 3-s delay period (30 pulses). For each brain area targeted (and, hence, for each session), a total of 2,880 pulses were delivered. TMS was delivered with a Magstim Standard Rapid magnetic stimulator fit with a 70-mm figure-8 stimulating coil (Magstim, Whitland, UK). Because rTMSpresent and rTMSabsent trials were randomly intermixed, the intertrain interval varied. A minimum of 17.1 s separated each train. White noise was played in the background during all trials.

EEG Recording

EEG was recorded with a 60-channel carbon cap and TMS-compatible amplifier (Nexstim, Helsinki, Finland). This amplifier is designed to avoid saturation by the TMS pulse by employing a “sample-and-hold” circuit that keeps the output of the amplifier constant from 100 μs pre- to 2-ms post-stimulus (Virtanen et al. 1999). To further reduce residual TMS-related artifacts, the impedance at each electrode was kept below 3 kΩ (Massimini et al. 2005). The right mastoid served as the reference during recording, and the ERP waveforms were algebraically rereferenced to the average of all 60 electrodes offline. Additionally, eye movements were recorded using two electrodes placed near the eyes. Data were digitized at a rate of 1450 Hz with a 16-bit resolution.

Data Preprocessing

Data were processed offline using the EEGlab toolbox (v. 6.03, Delorme and Makeig 2004) running in a MATLAB environment (Mathworks, Natick, MA). The data were first downsampled to 500 Hz (after application of a low-pass anti-aliasing filter) and then band-pass filtered between 0.1 and 500 Hz. The data were then cleaned of large movement-related artifacts and channels with excessive noise were reinterpolated using spherical spline interpolation, as in the present study.

TMS Artifact Removal with ICA

Although the Nexstim EEG amplifier described above was specifically designed to minimize the electrical artifacts produced by TMS, in some cases there were nonetheless remaining artifacts in the EEG data. The use of ICA to remove this artifact is the primary focus of the Hamidi et al. (in press) paper. In the context of the present paper, this raises some concern that, because TMS was delivered at 10 Hz, evidence of entrainment to the stimulation frequency may have been removed along with TMS-related artifacts. Although there is no way to be sure that some physiological data was not removed along with the 10-Hz TMS artifact, there are several reasons to believe that any removal that did occur was minimal. First, electrical signals originating from the brain become smeared out as they pass through the skull. As a result, physiological EEG signals, even if from a very localized source, are typically observable at many electrodes on the scalp. TMS-related artifacts, on the other hand, originate from outside the skull and, with TMS-compatible EEG systems, localize to only the few electrodes surrounding the TMS coil (Komssi et al. 2004). TMS-related artifacts are also temporally predictable: reliably occurring with the delivery of each pulse and typically lasting only a few milliseconds (Kahkonen et al. 2005; Virtanen et al. 1999). Thus, ICA, a method that separates statistically independent sources from a mixed signal, is ideally suited to separate TMS-related electrical artifacts from physiological data obtained via EEG recording. One qualification of these general points should be noted, however. As discussed below, ICA components may in some cases contain both physiological and artifactual signals. In these cases, the artifact is likely to be much stronger than any, potentially entrainment-related, overlapping physiological signals. Therefore, if one finds a component having the strongest loadings at only a few electrodes, this could present a scaling problem where the much weaker contributions of the entrained physiological signal, which may also be more spatially blurred than the artifact, is no longer visible.

Two rounds of ICA were performed on the data to assure that less prominent TMS artifacts were detected. The first ICA was performed on continuous EEG data, and the second was performed solely on data for epochs where TMS was applied. TMS artifact components were identified using three characteristics (examples of each characteristic can be seen in Fig. 1 of Hamidi et al. in press). First, as described above, the artifact should be relatively spatially localized. Second, the power spectrum of an ICA component should have a strong peak at 10 Hz (accompanied by peaks at every harmonic of 10 Hz), because TMS was delivered at 10 Hz. Although physiological signals that do not reflect entrainment to an exogenous source may also have a peak around 10 Hz (the center of the α-band), this peak typically covers a wider frequency range, and does not exhibit strong harmonics. Additionally, physiological signals show a general 1/frequency pattern in the power spectrum, whereas TMS-related artifacts are superimposed on a flat power spectrum. A third criterion concerned the timecourse of the component activity. With the TMS compatible EEG system, the artifact, if present, is limited to the first 10–15 ms after the pulse (Ilmoniemi et al. 1997; Virtanen et al. 1999). Because the timing of the TMS train was known, an ICA component reflecting the TMS artifact had to peak within a few milliseconds of the start of each TMS pulse in the train.

Although in most instances it was straightforward to differentiate TMS artifacts from physiological data, following the guidelines outlined above, for several subjects ICA also produced one or more components that seemed to contain elements of both neurophysiological data and TMS artifact. In these cases, the component was kept for further analysis, with the idea that a second round of ICA might separate the two. If mixed TMS and physiological components were identified following the second ICA, they were then discarded. Although the potential removal of physiologically relevant data is a legitimate cause for concern, it should be noted that relatively few components (1.6(±1.4) per subject) that included both TMS artifact and physiological activity were removed.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Johnson, J.S., Hamidi, M. & Postle, B.R. Using EEG to Explore How rTMS Produces Its Effects on Behavior. Brain Topogr 22, 281–293 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-009-0118-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-009-0118-1

Keywords

Navigation