Introduction

Gender role conflict, hypermasculinity, and internalized homophobia have negative effects on the health-related risk behaviors and healthcare engagement of Black men in the U.S. who have sex with both men and women (BMSMW). Traditional masculine roles prescribe behaviors such as “Be powerful” and “Do not show fear, be feminine or display affection toward other men” (Hall & Applewhite, 2013; Ward, 2005). Hypermasculinity, an exaggeration of traditional masculine traits, stresses physical strength, aggression, dominance, and sexual prowess, and a man lacking these characteristics is considered weak and feminine (LaPollo, Bond, & Lauby, 2014; Ward, 2005). In a study of men who have sex with men and women (MSMW) (n = 281, Black, 64%, and white, 36%), LaPollo et al. (2014) found hypermasculine ideals to be associated with income, ever being in jail, feeling that it is “very important” to keep MSM behavior secret, and greater internalized homophobia. Some have suggested that “Black masculinities” evolved in response to the emasculation of Black men during slavery and ongoing racism that includes violence, educational and economic deprivation, and sexualization, resulting in hypermasculine prescriptions for behavior (LaPollo et al., 2014; Ward, 2005). Similarly, some have argued that hypermasculinity has been embraced among gay men in response to subordination by a dominant heterosexual male culture (Fields et al., 2015). Both types of masculinity, Black masculinity and hypermasculinity, have also been referred to as “compensatory masculinity” because they are formed “in reaction to blocked access to power and authority” (Fields et al., 2015).

Others point out that researchers have ignored aspects of Black men’s masculinity, such as self-determinism, accountability, family, community, pride, and spirituality, that have helped to maintain Black families and communities over time (Hammond & Mattis, 2005; Hunter & Davis, 1992, 1994; Wade & Rochlen, 2013). Arguably, these positive aspects of Black men’s masculinity have been even more overlooked in reference to Black MSMW. The focus instead has been on how Black men’s socioeconomic status, gender role expectations, and racial stratification may increase motivations for sex with women among men who are attracted to both men and women or only men (Bowleg et al., 2011; Carey, Senn, Seward, & Vanable, 2010; Nunn et al., 2011). This focus on negative aspects may be because of how traditional masculine gender role expectations are associated with health-comprising behaviors and beliefs, such as having multiple sex partners, resistance to using condoms, declining services such as human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) testing, as well as holding negative attitudes toward both women and men who have sex with men (MSM) (Hall & Applewhite, 2013; LaPollo et al., 2014; Mankowski & Maton, 2010; Rhodes et al., 2011). Feeling pressured to live up to masculine or hypermasculine ideals has also been linked with internalized homonegativity (Estrada, Rigali-Oiler, Arciniega, & Tracey, 2011; Miller, 2015). Although hypermasculinity is associated with a higher number of both male and female partners among BMSMW, it appears to have a stronger influence on the number of female partners than male partners (LaPollo et al., 2014).

The role of socialization in masculine gender role expectations has also been overlooked. Black men are subject to different socialization processes than White men (e.g., racial discrimination, economic exclusion); these different social realities lead to different constructions of masculinity (Wade & Rochlen, 2013). Racial differences in the construction and expectations of masculine gender roles might explain differences in behavior between Black and White MSMW. For Black MSMW, hypermasculinity and internalized homophobia are significantly associated with a greater number of recent male and female sex partners, whereas for White MSMW they are associated only with a higher number of female sex partners (LaPollo et al., 2014). Similarly, differences in how masculine gender roles impact those whose behaviors and/or identities reflect attraction to both men and women versus only men might also explain differences in behavior between Black MSMW and Black MSM only. For example, Black MSMW may experience more pressure than do Black MSM to uphold traditional Black family values and fill the role of partners to Black women. Compared with Black MSM, Black MSMW engage in more transactional sex, insertive anal intercourse, substance use, and have more criminal justice involvement; they also report experiencing more physical assaults, intimate partner violence, and depressive symptoms (Dyer et al., 2013; Friedman et al., 2019a; LaPollo et al., 2014). Among gay- and bisexually identified men (of which almost 30% were Black), Feinstein, Moran, Newcomb, and Mustanski (2019) found that bisexually identified men reported more condomless insertive anal sex with casual partners than did gay-identified men. They also reported more frequent marijuana use and lower levels of lifetime HIV testing and pre-exposure prophylaxis (PrEP) use. Another study found that Black MSMW were less likely to report PrEP awareness than were Black MSM; however, among those who were aware of PrEP, Black MSMW were more likely to actually use it than were Black MSM (Friedman et al., 2019b). Finally, MSMW of all race/ethnicities tend to have lower socioeconomic status than do MSM (Shadaker, Magee, Paz-Bailey, Hoots, & NHBS Study Group, 2017), which may foster compensatory masculinity.

Researchers have often grouped Black MSMW with Black MSM or with White MSMW, even though their sexual behaviors, HIV-related risks, racial and sexual identities, and self-perceptions differ. Researchers use measures, outreach strategies, and interventions that have been developed specifically for MSM and/or developed primarily with White MSMW, all of which often explicitly or implicitly assume monosexual behaviors and gay or bisexual identities. For example, existing measures of internalized homophobia for MSM tend to use terms like “gay” and assume that respondents are primarily attracted to men (Mayfield, 2001). This may affect the responses of MSMW for reasons unrelated to their attitudes regarding sex between men. In addition, some Black men have criticized and rejected the term “gay” for cultural and historical reasons, arguing that it has homophobic and Eurocentric origins (Parks, 2001).

New measures, focused on biphobia, have been created since we conducted our research. One new measure of anti-bisexual discrimination was developed with only 15 (4%) of the 422 participants identifying as Black (compared with 345 [82%] who identified as White) (Dyar, Feinstein, & Davila, 2019), and a brief version of the Anti-Bisexual Experiences Scale was administered to 390 participants, of whom only 9 (2.3%) identified as “African American/Caribbean American/Black” (Paul, Smith, Mohr, & Ross, 2014). Blacks comprised 33% of the sample with which the Negative Attitudes Toward Same-Sex Behavior Inventory was developed and validated, yet it did not include race as a domain (Antebi-Gruszka & Schrimshaw, 2018). The small number of Black MSMW included in the development and evaluation of these measures, as well as not including race as a domain, calls into question these instruments’ cultural relevance and suitability for Black MSMW. Research to date hints at the limitations of using the same instrument—either with both MSMW and MSM (regardless of race) or with racially diverse participants (regardless of sexual identity)—to measure a construct that is shaped by each group’s race, culture, and sexual identity (Miller, 2015).

Too often research approaches and survey measures overlook Black men’s multiple and intersecting identities (e.g., race, class, sexual identity) and how these identities reflect inequalities at the sociostructural level, that is, those produced by racism and heterosexism (Bowleg et al., 2017). This is due in part to the fact that a majority of studies on Black men’s sexuality have been designed, conducted, analyzed, and interpreted by researchers who were not Black, which often led to the construction of Black sexuality as hypermasculine, hyperheterosexual, aggressive, and influenced by epistemologies of ignorance that are rooted in racism (Bowleg et al., 2017; Ford, Whetten, Hall, Kaufman, & Thrasher, 2007). An intersectional framework emphasizes that race, gender, class, and sexual identity cannot be extricated from each other, that these identities reinforce each other, and that a person’s social location, such as that determined by racialized processes, cannot be properly understood without accounting for how it intersects with other key locations, such as sexual identity (Bowleg et al., 2017). In the case of Black MSMW, it is important to recognize the complexity of the heteronormative environments within which they live. Their stigmatized sexual statuses, their sometimes-threatened and questioned masculinity, their racial designations, and the high levels of poverty they face cannot be reduced to discrete experiences or identities or analyzed along a single axis (e.g., sexuality). Ignoring key aspects of Black MSMW’s experiences constrains our capacity to understand how they self-identify and how they respond to the expectations of traditional masculinity and heteronormativity.

While developing and testing a holistic HIV risk-reduction intervention for Black MSMW (Harawa et al., 2013), we sought to reduce the negative impact of gender roles on HIV risk and preventive behaviors and healthcare seeking. Prior efforts to address these behaviors among Black men and Black MSMW had considered neither the interrelated experiences of race, class, and “compensatory masculinity,” nor the nontraditional aspects of masculinity valued by Black men. Furthermore, sound measures of biphobia were not available at the time of our research nor were measures that assessed Black MSMW’s overlapping experiences and identities related to race, gender, and sexual orientation. In order to address these gaps, we used an iterative, community-informed process: (1) to modify existing measures of internalized homophobia and gender role conflict for Black MSMW and (2) to develop a measure of integrated racial and sexual identities. Below, we describe each of these measures, the rationale for specific adaptations, the psychometric properties of these scales, and their construct validity.

Method

Development of Measures

Our primary objective was to assess mediators of sexual risk behaviors for an HIV prevention intervention study of low-income Black MSMW (Williams, Ramamurthi, Manago, & Harawa, 2009). We were also interested in exploring associations that had not been adequately studied in this subgroup. The intervention was designed to reduce the frequency of condomless sex and the number of sex partners and to increase HIV testing among Black MSMW (Harawa et al., 2013).

To develop the study’s survey instrument, the investigative team first identified measures of interest for our selected domains. These domains were informed by the behavioral models on which our intervention was based and our formative research with the study population, which included focus groups and one-on-one interviews (Harawa, Williams, Ramamurthi, & Bingham, 2006; Harawa et al., 2008). We then selected from the literature those measures that most closely captured these latent constructs and that had been evaluated with and shown validity in samples of Black adults in the U.S. Next, we reviewed the measures with a community advisory board comprised of Black MSM and MSMW and with community partners who had extensive experience serving these two subgroups. Participants suggested wording modifications and, in some cases, identified items that they perceived to be irrelevant or suggested new items. As a result of this process, we modified two existing scales, the GRCS and the Internalize Homophobia Scale, and wrote new items for a third scale, which we named the Integrated Race and Sexuality Scale. Next, we conducted cognitive interviews with 20 Black MSMW to ensure the modifications and new scale items were clear, easy to understand, and related to the concepts of interest. Minor additional modifications were made based on feedback from this process. These three scales are described below.

Modified Gender Role Conflict Scale (M-GRCS)

The original GRCS was administered using a 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree) response scale (O’Neil, Helms, Gable, David, & Wrightsman, 1986). We deleted seven of the original 37 items and added five new items, resulting in 35 items. The added items were designed to measure the importance of masculine presentation (described below) to participants. Six of the deleted items were from the conflict between work and family relations subscale (e.g., “My work or school often disrupts other parts of my life [home, health, leisure]”), which was deemed to lack relevance for our group of largely unemployed/underemployed men. Szymanski and Carr (2008) also noted concerns about the validity of these items. The seventh item, “Telling my partner my feelings about him or her during sex is difficult for me,” was deleted because our community advisory board and community partners indicated that it seemed repetitive and might lack relevance if individuals were engaging in sex for reasons unrelated to any kind of emotional attachment, such as exchange sex or anonymous sex. Additionally, we made minor wording changes to eight items to improve their clarity and relevance to the target population. This modified version of the GRCS, the M-GRCS, measures participants’ view of gender role stereotypes in four main areas (Bingham, Harawa, & Williams, 2013): (1) success, power, and competition; (2) difficulty expressing emotions or having others express them; (3) difficulty showing or observing affection between men; and (4) masculine presentation (e.g., “Men should never show their feminine side” and “I never want to look or seem weak”).

Internalized Bi/Homophobia Scale (IBHS)

The Internalized Homophobia Scale (IHS) has nine items (Martin & Dean, 1987). The IHS was administered using five response options, from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). While the scale has been shown to have acceptable reliability (α > 0.70), there is limited information about its factor structure. An unpublished analysis suggested a single factor for the IHS items (Herek, Gillis, Cogan, & Glunt, 1997). Although the IHS has been administered in studies with gay-, lesbian-, and bisexual-identified populations, BMSMW comprised a small portion of the study samples (Martin & Dean, 1987). The IHS was administered to a sample of 741 gay-identified men in New York City, but only 11% were men of color (Meyer, 1995). Herek et al. (1997) evaluated a gender-specific version of the scale in a sample of 73 men and 74 women recruited from a lesbian, gay, and bisexual street fair in Sacramento of which 18% were people of color, 14% self-identified as bisexual, and the rest identified as lesbian or gay.

We modified the wording of the nine original IHS items because our study participants were men who reported sex with both men and women, generally were attracted to both men and women, did not necessarily identify as gay or see themselves as part of the gay community, and identified with a range of labels for their sexuality. We also added four items suggested by our community experts either to clarify responses to original questions or to ask specifically about bisexual identification and behavior. We called the new 13-item scale the Internalized Bi/Homophobia Scale (IBHS).

Integrated Race and Sex Scale (IRSS)

This new scale has 11 items assessing the intersecting levels of internalized pride and conflict regarding participants’ status as male members of minority groups defined by sexual behavior and race. IRSS uses a 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) response scale, selected to allow for a neutral option. Four items examined the perceived role of Black homosexual and bisexual men in communities and families (e.g., “Black homosexual and bisexual men contribute to Black communities”). Two items examined perceptions of Black male relationships (e.g., “I cannot imagine a loving sexual relationship between two Black men”), and one item explored whether a Black MSM can fulfill other masculine expectations: “A Black man who has sex with men can still be a strong man.” Three items examined how participants wish to be seen in relation to their gender, race, and sexual orientation (e.g., “It is more important that people see me as a man than as a heterosexual man”), and one item explored the importance of race and sexuality in relation to participants’ gender identity: “Both my race and my sexuality are important to who I am as a man.”

We developed this scale to move beyond items used in prior research that assessed the relative value participants placed on their racial and sexual identities, including those measures that asked participants to rank the relative importance of these identities and affiliations. Specific items further assessed our premise about the centrality of masculine gender roles to participants’ self-perceptions and interactions with others. In addition, the scale was developed to assess whether the intervention led to a stronger and more integrated sense of oneself in relation to these categories.

Analysis

We conducted an exploratory factor analysis of the M-GRCS, IBHS, and IRSS items using categorical principal axis factoring with unweighted least squares (ULS) extraction and Promax factor rotation (Hendrickson & White, 1964). Three different factor criteria were examined: the scree plot, Guttman's weakest lower bound, and Guttman's third bound (Cattell, 1966; Guttman, 1940). The results of the factor analyses were used to create simple-summated multi-item scales. We estimated internal consistency reliability for the scales (coefficient alpha): ≥ 0.90 = excellent; 0.80-0.89 = good; and 0.70-0.79 = acceptable (Cronbach, 1951).

We evaluated the construct validity of the modified scales by estimating their correlations with “criterion” measures. We chose the 7-item Lukwago scale of racial pride (α = 0.84) as the criterion for the IRSS (Lukwago, Kreuter, Bucholtz, Holt, & Clark, 2001) and expected a positive association. We also hypothesized a negative association of the IRSS with the Restricted Emotionality and Discomfort with Affection between Men subscales from the M-GRCS. For the IBHS, we chose several items indicating whether or not a participant had disclosed his sexual orientation to each of several types of individuals, including family members, friends, and female sex partners. We also examined associations of the IBHS with the following: reports of numbers of one’s friends knowing one’s same-sex sexual orientation, the perceived importance of one’s friends knowing this, perceived importance of keeping one’s sexual relationships with men secret, and how often one had sex with a woman who did not know one’s sexual orientation, as additional criteria variables for the IBHS. We chose the IBHS as a criterion for the Restricted Emotionality, Discomfort with Affection between Men, and Success/Power/Competition subscales of the M-GRCS, hypothesizing that those with higher levels of bi/homophobia would report increased higher levels of these M-GRCS constructs.

We examined correlations of the scales with psychological distress (Brief Symptom Inventory-53) (Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983) and self-esteem (10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale) (Rosenberg, 1979). We hypothesized that the IBHS and the M-GRCS would be associated with higher levels of distress and lower levels of self-esteem and that the IRSS would be associated with lower levels of distress and higher levels of self-esteem. Finally, because both gender role conflict and internalized bi/homophobia may reflect an internalization of perceived norms from one’s social networks, we examined associations with the friend and family subscales of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet, & Farley, 1988).

Results

A total of 429 respondents completed the items as part of their baseline (pre-intervention) self-administered (via computer) survey (see Table 1). Men aged 40–49 years were the largest age group (43%), followed by those over 50 years old (24%); those under 18 years of age comprised the smallest group (14%). The majority of the men self-identified as bisexual (61%), followed by heterosexual/straight/down low or DL (21%) and gay or homosexual (13%). Most of the men had a history of incarceration (n = 328, 77%). Although most participants (58%) had completed high school, a general equivalency diploma (GED), or higher educational level (26%), more than half (56%) reported earning less than $1,000 per month and almost half were unemployed (46%). Only 15% of the men reported currently living with a spouse or partner. Forty percent had experienced housing instability in the prior12 months. Nearly half of the participants had previously tested HIV-positive, 43% had last tested HIV-negative, and 8% had never tested.

Table 1 Participant characteristics (n = 429)

The scree plot showed comparable breaks in the scree after 3, 7, and 12 factors. We initially decided to rotate and interpret 7 factors to coincide with the 4 M-GRCS scales, 2 IBHS scales, and our expectation of 1 or 2 IRSS scales. However, only five factors remained when we imposed a minimum factor loading of 0.45 in the rotated solution.

The first and third factors, based on the size of the factor loadings, were defined by subsets of M-GRCS items. The first factor represents both Restricted Emotionality and Discomfort with Affection between Men, and the third factor represents Success/Power/Competition, with one item related to masculine presentation (“One must seem strong to be respected”). Ten of the 13 IBHS items defined the second factor, and IRSS items defined the next two factors, with five and two items, respectively.

Based on the small size of factor loadings, we removed three items from the M-GRCS, three items from the IBHS, and three items from the IRSS from further analyses. Two of the problematic items from the IBHS (“Having sex with other men is not a problem for me” and “I value my sexuality as it is”) and three of the problematic items from the IRSS (“I cannot imagine a loving sexual relationship between two Black men,” “Black men who have sex with men do nothing for Black people,” and “Black homosexual and bisexual men only make it more difficult for black people in general”) were reverse-worded items. Reverse-worded items can be unclear and confusing to respondents (Weijters & Baumgartner, 2012). In addition, we judged the remaining problematic item from the IBHS (“I would not worry about being strictly heterosexual if I could be loved and accepted as a bisexual or homosexual man”) to be poorly constructed. Furthermore, two of the problematic M-GRCS items (“Men who are overly friendly to me make me wonder about their sexual preference” and “Men who touch other men make me uncomfortable”) may have lacked relevance to our sample of behaviorally bisexual men. These items reflect the theme of restricted affection between men. Bisexual men may experience minimal discomfort with expressions of affection to or from other men, especially in discreet settings. Another problematic M-GRCS item (“Moving up the career ladder is important to me”) also lacked relevance to our sample of low-income men because they were not typically in career jobs. In addition, we decided to remove two other problematic M-GRCS items (“I never want to look or seem weak” and “I do not want to seem effeminate, girlish, soft, or womanly”) and one remaining problematic IRSS item (“I wish I could be an open black gay/bisexual/same gender loving man”) from further analyses because they failed to load on any factors in the rotated factor solution. In summary, a total of five M-GRCS items, three IBHS items, and four IRSS items were removed, leaving 30 M-GRCS items, 10 IBHS items, and 7 IRSS items for further analyses.

We repeated the categorical principal axis factoring procedure with ULS extraction and Promax rotation on the final set of 47 items, at which point some of the factor loadings fell between 0.40 and 0.45; however, we retained these items. The scree plot showed a clear break after four factors. The rotated solution with four factors yielded a simple factor structure with nonoverlapping subsets of M-GRCS items defining two factors, the IBHS items defining a single factor, and the IRSS items also defining a single factor (Table 2). The first and third factors in the loading matrix were defined by nonoverlapping subsets of M-GRCS items, reflecting themes of restricted emotionality/discomfort with affection between men and success/power/ competition, respectively. The second factor is an internalized bi/homophobia factor, and the fourth and final factor is the integrated race and sexuality factor.

Table 2 Rotated factor loading and Cronbach alpha coefficients in the solution from the categorical principal axis factoring procedure

Table 2 provides internal consistency reliability estimates and inter-factor correlations. Coefficient alphas for the two M-GRCS factors (α = 0.93 for Factor 1 and 0.87 for Factor 3) and the IBHS factor (2) (α = 0.89) were high and lower but acceptable for the IRSS (α = 0.74). As shown in Table 3, both M-GRCS factors were positively correlated with the IBHS factor. In addition, the first M-GRCS factor (Restricted Emotionality/Difficulty with Affection between Men) was negatively correlated with the IRSS factor, r(411) = -0.21, while the second M-GRCS factor (Success/Power/Competition) had a weak positive correlation with the IRSS, r(412) = 0.09. However, we note that the latter included items related to both general emotionality and to emotion/affection between men. As expected, the IBHS factor was negatively correlated with the IRSS, r(414) = -0.22. Most of these correlations are “medium” size based on Cohen’s rules of thumb: 0.10 is small, 0.24 is medium, and 0.37 is large (Cohen, 1988).

Table 3 Inter-factora correlations

Our examination of correlations between the new/modified factors and the criterion measures found associations in the expected directions (not shown). The product–moment correlation between the IRSS and the Lukwago Racial Pride Scale was large, r(417) = 0.40, p < .0001. Among the 323 participants who had told someone that they had sex with men, we found a large negative correlation of the IBHS with the number of friends knowing their sexual orientation, r (322) = -0.37, p < .0001, and the perceived importance of one’s friends knowing this, r(414) = -0.20, p < .0001. We also found statistically significant negative, small, point-biserial correlations between IBHS and disclosing to the following types of network members: one’s mother, father, siblings, heterosexual friends, homosexual/bisexual/transgender friends, and doctor (all biserial r’s range from -0.13 to -0.21). In addition, we found statistically significant positive correlations between IBHS and the importance of keeping one’s sexual relationships with men secret, r(414) = 0.31, p < .0001, and between IBHS and having sex with women who did not know one’s sexual orientation, r(396) = 0.12, p = .02.

Finally, we examined correlations of the scales with well-established measures of self-esteem, psychological distress, and social support (Table 4). In general, we found statistically significant associations in the expected directions. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale was negatively associated with restricted emotionality/affection between men, internalized bi/homophobia, success/power/competition (Factors 1–3) and positively associated with the IRRS (Factor 4). Statistically significant associations were found between all 9 subscales of the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) and Factors 1–3. In addition, statistically positive associations were found between the friends and family subscales of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) and the IRSS. However, the MSPSS was largely uncorrelated with the three factors representing the M-GRCS, IBHS, and M-GRCS.

Table 4 Correlations of identified factors with self-esteem, psychological distress, and perceived social support

Discussion

The reliabilities of our modified and new measures were generally high. Furthermore, the correlations with racial pride and disclosure of same-sex sexual support the construct validity of the measures. Additionally, their correlations with self-esteem and psychological distress point to the relevance of the measures to the psychological well-being of Black MSMW. The reliabilities were roughly comparable to prior studies that used the original version of the measures. However, prior research on the original GRCS and IHS from which the IBHS was derived would suggest that we would expect a 6-factor structure from these three scales. In other research on the GRCS over the years, studies with participants from multiple races and specifically with gay men consistently supported a 4-factor, rather than a 2-factor structure (Wester, Vogel, O’Neil, & Danforth, 2012; Zhang et al., 2015).

Our analysis of the reduced and refined set of the Modified Gender Role Conflict Scale and the Internalized Bi/Homophobia Scale and the new Integrated Race and Sexuality Scale items administered to a sample of Black MSMW yielded a four-factor structure consisting of Success/Power/Competition and Restricted Emotionality/Discomfort with Affection between Men factors defined by M-GRCS items, an Internalized Bi/Homophobia factor defined by IBHS items, and an Integrated Race and Sexuality Factor defined by seven of the IRSS items. In addition, the Success/Power/Competition and Restricted Emotionality/Discomfort with Affection between Men factors were positively intercorrelated, but negatively correlated with the IRSS factor.

By using an intersectional framework to modify the measures of gender role conflict and bi/homophobia, we were able to address expectations of masculinity that may place a greater emphasis on sexual prowess, physical dominance, and gamesmanship for Black men and less of an emphasis on education, employment, and socioeconomic status than for White men (Fields et al, 2015; LaPollo et al., 2014). The correlations among our factors indicated that high levels of success/power/competition were associated with high levels of restricted emotionality/affection between men and with high levels of internalized bi/homophobia. We also found that internalized bi/homophobia was associated with a decreased likelihood of disclosure to multiple types of individuals in the men’s social network, to their healthcare providers, and to their female sex partners. These patterns of results support the construct validity of the modified scales.

The M-GRCS appears to behave differently in low-income Black MSMW than it does in other subgroups of men. Although this may be explained by our scale modifications rather than the population focus, it is not surprising that gender-role-related conflicts for economically marginalized Black MSMW differ significantly from those observed in the middle-class White heterosexual and gay-identified men who have been the most studied with the GRCS. For example, Black cultural norms regarding emotional expression and affection between men differ from those of many Whites, with greater acceptance of displays of strong emotion and of public affection between close heterosexual male friends, including treating certain friends as family and sharing the material rewards of success (Oware, 2011). Culturally acceptable displays of gender also differ. An activity such as dancing, for example, was deemed feminine or “gay” by White male high school students, while Black male high school students saw dancing as a way to affirm their masculinity (Miller, 2015). Thus, our intersectional framework and our findings bring into sharp relief how some GRCS items may have less relevance than has been seen with samples from other racial/ethnic groups. Our framework also points to the cultural limitations of recently developed measures of bisexuality for BMSMW (Antebi-Gruszka & Schrimshaw, 2018; Dyar et al., 2019; Paul et al., 2014): they did not include enough BMSMW in their samples, ignored race as a domain, or used terms such as “straight” to refer to people other than the participants—even though some of the BMSMW identified themselves as “straight.”

We were somewhat surprised that the M-GRCS and IBHS were largely uncorrelated with social support despite having negative associations with self-esteem and positive associations with psychological distress. One potential explanation to explore in future research is that adhering to these norms and beliefs reflects alignment with like-minded members of the dominant culture, even though the beliefs have costs at the individual psychological level.

Limitations

We used the original (unreduced) version of our M-GRCS scale in a publication that examined associations of gender role conflict with psychosocial factors, sexual risk behaviors, and HIV testing (Harawa et al., 2013). For the factors examined in both papers, we found similar associations for the two different versions of the measure. However, the reduced M-GRCS is a more parsimonious and internally consistent measure than the full M-GRCS. Although the wording changes that we made for both the M-GRCS and the IBHS were intended to make the measure more relevant for the target population, we cannot be sure about whether or not they had other unintended effects on the measures’ psychometric properties. Another limitation is the self-selected nature of our sample who self-identified for participation in an HIV prevention intervention and were from a limited geographic area. Finally, in the context of HIV risk behaviors, it is difficult to define and measure gendered psychosocial constructs such as hypermasculinity and sexual orientation without conflating them with labels and behaviors, e.g., MSM. Hence, these findings need to be replicated in other samples.

Conclusion

The literature does not provide a clear picture of the health impacts of gender role expectations and bi/homophobia on Black men who have sex with men and women. As we have argued, part of the problem is that research has traditionally ignored an intersectional framework for thinking about the identities of Black men in general and Black MSMW in particular. Misunderstandings of the complexity and intersectionality of Black MSMW’s identities may result from differences in the measures used across studies and the use of incommensurate measures for examining gender and masculinity constructs in MSM and MSMW and in Black and White men. From the perspective of HIV prevention, the discrepancies others have observed in the associations of these factors with specific risk and preventive behaviors when studying BMSMW and BMSM or White MSMW corroborate the argument that BMSMW have unique circumstances and racial and cultural contexts that warrant research in their own right (Malebranche, 2008). This study sought to address this by developing an instrument specifically for Black MSMW and examining its psychometric properties in a moderately large sample of men who were defined using a specific but not overly narrow definition of MSMW (any sexual behavior with men within the prior two years). The Integrated Racial and Sexual Identity Scale we developed examines race, gender, and sexuality as joint rather than independent identities. It also takes into account that a narrow focus on gender- and sexual orientation-related attitudes overlooks the ways in which a strong African-American racial identity can contribute to reduced sexual risk behaviors (LaPollo et al., 2014; Li et al., 2018). Although this scale did not perform as well as the modified versions of existing measures, it provides a new tool that can be refined and built upon in future research. This new instrument may contribute to future research in shaping findings on the sexual risk behaviors and the role of masculinity and bi/homophobia in the lives of Black MSM and MSMW.