Skip to main content
Log in

Indirect Measurement of Sexual Orientation: Comparison of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure, Viewing Time, and Choice Reaction Time Tasks

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study was conducted to validate an adaptation of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as an indirect latency-based measure of sexual orientation. Furthermore, reliability and criterion validity of the IRAP were compared to two established indirect measures of sexual orientation: a Choice Reaction Time task (CRT) and a Viewing Time (VT) task. A sample of 87 heterosexual and 35 gay men completed all three indirect measures in an online study. The IRAP and the VT predicted sexual orientation nearly perfectly. Both measures also showed a considerable amount of convergent validity. Reliabilities (internal consistencies) reached satisfactory levels. In contrast, the CRT did not tap into sexual orientation in the present study. In sum, the VT measure performed best, with the IRAP showing only slightly lower reliability and criterion validity, whereas the CRT did not yield any evidence of reliability or criterion validity in the present research. The results were discussed in the light of specific task properties of the indirect latency-based measures (task-relevance vs. task-irrelevance).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Three participants did not provide any information regarding their age.

  2. As is the case for all latency-based measures, we had to hypothesize that reaction times may be affected by participants’ age. We included age as a covariate in all our analyses in order to control for general age-reaction speed effects. For reasons of parsimony, results of the covariate analyses are only reported when significant. Unless otherwise reported, the covariate did not have a significant effect. However, in this case including the covariate–although non-significant–yielded a significant Sexual Orientation × Trial Type interaction effect in the respective ANOVA reported above, F(3, 112) = 3.09 p < .05, η² = .03.

  3. In order to control for possible habituation or practice effects (as discussed by Santtila et al., 2009) due to the high number of trials in the present CRT, we also conducted all analyses separately for the first 108 and second 108 trials. The pattern found in the analysis of all trials still held for both halves of trials, with significant main effects of Age and Sexual Orientation, but no significant two-way or three-way interactions were obtained. Consequently, the AUC indices of both scores were not significant (AUC = .49 for the first 108 trials, and AUC = .56 for the second 108 trials).

References

  • Banse, R., Schmidt, A. F., & Clarbour, J. (2010). Indirect measures of sexual interest in child sex offenders: A multimethod approach. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 37, 319–335. doi:10.1177/0093854809357598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Power, P., Hayden, E., Milne, R., & Stewart, I. (2006). Do you really know what you believe? Developing the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a direct measure of implicit beliefs. The Irish Psychologist, 32, 169–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, Y., Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2002). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 28, 101–138. doi:10.1016/S0065-2407(02)80063-5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Murtagh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2010). Using the Implicit Association Test and the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure to measure attitudes toward meat and vegetables in vegetarians and meat-eaters. Psychological Record, 60, 287–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Waldron, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2009). Testing the validity of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) and the Implicit Association Test (IAT): Measuring attitudes towards Dublin and country life in Ireland. Psychological Record, 58, 389–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourke, A. B., & Gormley, M. J. (2012). Comparing a pictorial Stroop task to viewing time measures of sexual interest. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 24, 479–500. doi:10.1177/1079063212438922.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chivers, M. L., Rieger, G., Latty, E., & Bailey, J. M. (2004). A sex difference in the specificity of sexual arousal. Psychological Science, 15, 736–744. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00750.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cisler, J. M., Bacon, A. K., & Williams, N. L. (2009). Phenomenological characteristics of attentional biases towards threat: A critical review. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33, 221–234. doi:10.1007/s10608-007-9161-y.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, D. L., Barnes-Holmes, D., Gresswell, D. M., Hart, A. J., & Gore, N. J. (2009). Assessing the implicit beliefs of sexual offenders using the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure: A first study. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 21, 57–75. doi:10.1177/1079063208326928.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. D. (2002). The Implicit Association Test as a tool for studying dysfunctional associations in psychopathology: Strengths and limitations. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33, 115–133. doi:10.1016/S0005-7916(02)00024-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dombert, B., Mokros, A., Bruckner, E., Schlegl, V., Antfolk, J., Backstrom, A., … Santtila, P. (2013). The Virtual People Set: Developing computer-generated stimuli for the assessment of pedophilic sexual interest. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 25, 557–582. doi:10.1177/1079063212469062.

  • Ebsworth, M., & Lalumière, M. L. (2012). Viewing time as a measure of bisexual interest. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 161–172. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-9923-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Derakshan, N., & Standage, H. (2011). The assessment of human attention. In K. C. Klauer, A. Voss, & C. Stahl (Eds.), Cognitive methods in social psychology (pp. 15–47). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., Deutsch, R., LeBel, E. P., & Peters, K. R. (2008). Response interference as a mechanism underlying implicit measures—some traps and gaps in the assessment of mental associations with experimental paradigms. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24, 218–225. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.24.4.218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A., McGhee, D., & Schwartz, J. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. doi:10.1037/h0087889.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, G. T., & Rice, M. E. (1996). The science in phallometric measurement of male sexual interest. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 156–160. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imhoff, R., Schmidt, A. F., Bernhardt, J., Dierksmeier, A., & Banse, R. (2011). An inkblot for sexual preference: A semantic variant of the Affect Misattribution Procedure. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 676–690. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.508260.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imhoff, R., Schmidt, A. F., Nordsiek, U., Luzar, C., Young, A. W., & Banse, R. (2010). Viewing time effects revisited: Prolonged response latencies for sexually attractive targets under restricted task conditions. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 1275–1288. doi:10.1007/s10508-009-9595-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imhoff, R., Schmidt, A. F., Weiß, S., Young, A. W., & Banse, R. (2012). Vicarious viewing time: Prolonged response latencies for sexually attractive targets as a function of task- or stimulus-specific processing. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 1389–1401. doi:10.1007/s10508-011-9879-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Israel, E., & Strassberg, D. S. (2009). Viewing time as an objective measure of sexual interest in heterosexual men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 551–558. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9246-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalmus, E., & Beech, A. R. (2005). Forensic assessment of sexual interest: A review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10, 193–218. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2003.12.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the human male. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, F. (1990). The need to view sexual orientation as a multivariable dynamic process: A theoretical perspective. In D. P. McWhirter, S. A. Sanders, & J. M. Reinisch (Eds.), Homosexuality/heterosexuality: Concepts of sexual orientation (pp. 277–282). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight, L. F. M., & Hope, D. A. (2012). Correlates of same-sex attractions and behaviors among self-identified heterosexual university students. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 1199–1208. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-9927-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larue, D., Schmidt, A. F., Imhoff, R., Eggers, K., Schönbrodt, F. D., & Banse, R. (2014). Validation of direct and indirect measures of preference for sexualized violence. Psychological Assessment,. doi:10.1037/pas0000016.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lippa, R. A. (2012). Effects of sex and sexual orientation on self-reported attraction and viewing times to images of men and women: Testing for category specificity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 149–160. doi:10.1007/s10508-011-9898-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pacific Psychological Assessment Corporation. (2004). The Not-Real People Stimulus Set for assessment of sexual interest. Victoria, BC: PPAC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus, D. L., & Reid, D. B. (1991). Enhancement and denial in socially desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 307–317. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perugini, M., Richetin, J., & Zogmaister, C. (2010). Prediction of behavior. In B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 255–277). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prause, N., Janssen, E., & Hetrick, W. P. (2008). Attention and emotional responses to sexual stimuli and their relationship to sexual desire. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 934–949. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9236-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quinsey, V. L., Ketsetzis, M., Earls, C., & Karamanoukian, A. (1996). Viewing time as a measure of sexual interest. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17, 341–354. doi:10.1016/S0162-3095(96)00060-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, S. (1942). The photoscope as an objective device for evaluating sexual interest. Psychosomatic Medicine, 4, 150–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Santtila, P., Mokros, A., Viljanen, K., Koivisto, M., Sandnabba, N. K., Zappalà, A., & Osterheider, M. (2009). Assessment of sexual interest using a Choice Reaction Time task and priming: A feasibility study. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 14, 65–82. doi:10.1348/135532507X267040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savin-Williams, R. C. (2006). Who’s gay? Does it matter? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 40–44. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00403.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, A. F., Banse, R., & Imhoff, R. (in press). Indirect measures in forensic contexts. In F. J. R. Vijver & T. Ortner (Eds.), Behavior based assessment in personality, social and applied psychology. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

  • Snowden, R. J., Craig, R. L., & Gray, N. S. (2011). Indirect behavioral measures of cognition among sexual offenders. Journal of Sex Research, 48, 192–217. doi:10.1080/00224499.2011.557750.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snowden, R. J., & Gray, N. S. (2013). Implicit sexual associations in heterosexual and homosexual women and men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 475–485. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-9920-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snowden, R. J., Wichter, J., & Gray, N. S. (2008). Implicit and explicit measurements of sexual preference in gay and heterosexual men: A comparison of priming techniques and the Implicit Association Task. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 558–565. doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9138-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suchotzki, K., Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., & De Houwer, J. (2013). Reaction time measures in deception research: Comparing the effects of irrelevant and relevant stimulus-response compatibility. Acta Psychologica, 144, 224–231. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.06.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tanner, J. (1978). Foetus into man: Physical growth from conception to maturity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tilcsik, A. (2011). Pride and prejudice: Employment discrimination against openly gay men in the United States. American Journal of Sociology, 117, 586–626. doi:10.1086/661653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vahey, N., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2009). A first test of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure as a measure of self-esteem: Irish prisoner groups and university students. Psychological Record, 59, 371–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, L. W., & Adams, H. E. (1994). Assessment of sexual preference using a Choice Reaction Time task. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 16, 221–231. doi:10.1007/BF02229209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, L. W., & Adams, H. E. (1999). The effects of stimuli that vary in erotic content on cognitive processes. Journal of Sex Research, 36, 145–151. doi:10.1080/00224499909551979.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zamansky, H. S. (1956). A technique for measuring homosexual tendencies. Journal of Personality, 24, 436–448. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1956.tb01280.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jelena Rönspies.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rönspies, J., Schmidt, A.F., Melnikova, A. et al. Indirect Measurement of Sexual Orientation: Comparison of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure, Viewing Time, and Choice Reaction Time Tasks. Arch Sex Behav 44, 1483–1492 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0473-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0473-1

Keywords

Navigation