Introduction

Development of sexual orientation and gender identity in humans has been hypothesized to partly result from an interaction between sex hormones and the developing brain cells. Factors that interfere with this process of sexual differentiation during development in the womb may permanently influence later behavior (Swaab, 2007). Evidence for this hypothesis comes from a number of studies that have investigated the effects of prenatal exposure to atypical levels of (artificial) sex hormones, to anticonvulsants, and to stress on sexual orientation and gender identity. For example, women who were exposed to diethylstilbestrol (DES) during gestation showed increased bisexuality and homosexuality (Ehrhardt et al., 1985; Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 1995). Also, women who were born with congenital adrenal hyperplasia, a disease characterized by high androgen levels during fetal development, have been shown to display more gender identity problems, bisexuality, and homosexuality (Dessens, Slijper, & Drop, 2005; Meyer-Bahlburg, Dolezal, Baker, & New, 2008). Men and women prenatally exposed to anticonvulsants showed a higher rate of gender-identity-related problems and transsexualism (Dessens et al., 1999). Some studies have shown an association between self-reported stress during pregnancy of mothers and male homosexuality, possibly due to the effect of stress hormones on the fetal sex hormone balance (Dörner et al., 1980; Ellis & Cole-Harding, 2001). Other studies, however, failed to confirm this association (Bailey, Willerman, & Parks, 1991; Schmidt & Clement, 1990).

Blanchard and Ellis (2001) reported that homosexual males with older brothers weighed about 170 g less at birth than the heterosexual males with older brothers, a difference hypothesized to result from the progressive immunization of mothers to male specific antigens by each succeeding male fetus. An alternative explanation could be that prenatal undernutrition, for which low birth weight is a proxy, plays a part in this association. Possible effects of prenatal undernutrition on sexual orientation and gender identity in humans have not been studied before. In male rats, it was shown that undernutrition during the last part of gestation (days 14–20) was associated with a 23% reduction in birth weight and a 50% reduction in the size of the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area (SDN-POA), resulting in reduced levels of sexual activity and lower plasma testosterone levels (Anderson, Rhees, & Fleming, 1985).

The Dutch famine birth cohort study offers a unique opportunity to explore possible effects of prenatal exposure to famine on the sexual differentiation of the brain, as was shown in male rats (Anderson et al., 1985). Because of its explorative character, we not only studied possible effects in men, but also in women.

Method

Participants

The Dutch famine birth cohort comprises 2414 men and women born as term singletons between 1 November 1943 and 28 February 1947 in the Wilhelmina Gasthuis, a hospital of the University of Amsterdam. The selection procedures and loss to follow up have been described in detail elsewhere (Painter et al., 2005; Ravelli et al., 1998). Cohort members still living in the Netherlands on 1 September 2002 and whose address was known to the investigators were eligible for participation. Of the original cohort of 2414, 160 babies had not been registered in Amsterdam at birth and were lost to follow up. A further 328 people had died, 213 people had emigrated, 157 people refused permission for the study to record their address, 125 people were not traceable to a current address by the municipalities, and 8 people requested their address be removed from the study’s database. The remaining 1423 eligible people were invited to participate in the study, of whom 860 (60%) agreed to participate. Reasons for not participating included: lack of time, lack of interest, and not being able to participate due to (mental) health problems. People who agreed to participate had mean birth weights similar to those of eligible people not participating (3351 g vs. 3358 g). The study was approved by the local Medical Ethics Committee and carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants received oral and written explanation of the study aim and procedure. The study aim was given only in general terms, i.e., that the researchers wanted to know the sexuality of the participants.

Design

Exposure to famine was defined according to the daily official food rations for the general population aged >21 years (Trienekens, 1985). We considered fetuses to have been exposed to famine if the average daily rations during any 13-week period of gestation were <1000 calories. Therefore, babies born between 7 January and 8 December 1945 were considered exposed. We used periods of 16 weeks each to differentiate between people who were exposed to famine in late gestation (born between 7 January 1945 and 28 April 1945), in mid gestation (born between 29 April and 18 August 1945), and in early gestation (born between 19 August and 8 December 1945). All cohort members born between 1 November 1943 and 6 January 1945 (born before the famine), and between 9 December 1945 and 28 February 1947 (conceived after the famine) had thus not been exposed to famine in utero and served as the control group.

Procedure

Maternal and birth characteristics were provided by medical birth records (Ravelli et al., 1998). The birth order was known, but not the sex of older siblings. Information about socioeconomic and marital status was obtained during a standardized interview. We defined current socioeconomic status according to the International Socioeconomic Index of occupational status, which is based on the participant’s or their partner’s occupation, whichever status was higher (Bakker & Sieben, 1992).

To measure sexual orientation, participants were asked to complete a Dutch translation of the Klein Sexual Orientation Grid (KSOG) (Klein, Sepekoff, & Wolf, 1985). This scale consists of four items concerning behavior (two on behavior between 12 and 17 years of age, two on present behavior), four items concerning fantasy (two on fantasies between 12 and 17 years of age, two on present fantasies), and one item concerning self-identification (heterosexual/homosexual). The participant had to score the items on a 7-point scale ranging from “exclusively with/about own sex” (1 = completely homosexual) to “exclusively with/about other sex” (7 = completely heterosexual) for the behavior and fantasy items, and ranging from “exclusively homosexual” (1) to “exclusively heterosexual” (7) for the self-identification item. The participant could also indicate that the question of the item was not applicable to him/her (except for the self identification item). For an English translation of the KSOG in Dutch, see Appendix. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.90 for the nine items of the KSOG, indicating internal consistency of the items in our study group. Gender identity was measured by an item on which the participant had to indicate to what extent he or she regarded him or herself as transsexual. This item was scored on a 3-point scale ranging from “exclusively transsexual” (1) to “a bit transsexual” (2) to “not at all transsexual” (3).

Statistical Analyses

In Table 2, the result shown on behavior between 12 and 17 years is the downwards rounded mean of the scores on the two items concerning this topic. The same holds for behavior currently, and fantasy between 12 and 17 years and currently. When one of the two items concerning a topic was answered with “not applicable to me” or was not filled out, the answer on the other item was taken as the mean. For the statistical analysis, the mean of the four scores on behavior between 12 and 17 years and currently was taken, resulting in the behavior dimension. When one or more items were answered with “not applicable to me” or was not filled out, the answers on the other items were taken for the calculation of the mean. The same was done for the four scores on fantasy between 12 and 17 years and currently, resulting in the fantasy dimension. We then dichotomized the three remaining dimensions (behavior, fantasy, and self identification) into exclusively heterosexual (0 = all items answered with 7) or non-exclusively heterosexual (1 = at least one item answered with a 6 or lower) for analysis, based on the fact that few participants indicated scores other than 7. We used logistic regression analyses to compare the resulting frequencies of classes 0 and 1, percentages of men, primiparity, and marital status between those not exposed and those exposed to famine during gestation. Possible differences in age, birth weight, and socioeconomic status were analyzed with t tests. The gender-identity item was analyzed separately. Besides prenatal famine exposure, we also analyzed possible relations between sexual orientation and birth order, birth weight, socioeconomic status, and marital status. We additionally adjusted the famine analyses for these variables by adding them to the regression model. We analyzed data separately for men and women. All tests were performed two-tailed; p-values <.05 were considered to be statistically significant.

Results

Table 1 shows that of the 852 participants who completed the KSOG, 380 (45%) were men and 472 (55%) were women, with a mean age of 58 years (SD, 1 year). A total of 348 (41%) participants were exposed to famine during gestation. Mothers exposed to famine during gestation were less often primiparous and babies exposed during gestation had lower birth weights compared to those not exposed to famine.

Table 1 Maternal, birth, and adult characteristics according to prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine

Tables 2 and 3 show that there were no significant differences in frequencies of the scores on the KSOG dimensions behavior, fantasy, and self identification between participants exposed to famine and participants not exposed to famine. Men who were prenatally exposed to famine did not more often report non-exclusively heterosexual behavior (OR 1.2 [95% CI 0.7–2.0]), fantasies (1.3 [0.7–2.3]) or self-identification (1.1 [0.6–1.9]) than men not exposed. Exposed women also did not more often report non-exclusively heterosexual behavior (1.2 [0.8–1.9]), fantasies (0.9 [0.6–1.3]) or self-identification (1.0 [0.6–1.5]) than women not exposed. One (0.3%) of the 380 men who was born before the famine and three (0.6%) of the 472 women, of whom two were born before the famine and one exposed in late gestation, considered themselves exclusively homosexual.

Table 2 Sexual orientation in men as a function of famine exposure
Table 3 Sexual orientation in women as a function of famine exposure

A total of three participants indicated having some gender-identity problems (score 2). One was a man exposed to famine in late gestation, one was a woman born before the famine, and one was a woman exposed to famine in late gestation.

A large number of people indicated that one or more items were not applicable to him/her (score 8) or they did not fill out one or more items. Proportions for men and women who had at least one non-applicable/missing score on any item differed between exposed and not exposed men (p = .02), but not between exposed and not exposed women. Men exposed to famine during gestation more often had one or more non-applicable/missing scores (54%) compared to men not exposed (48%), especially when exposed in late (55%) or mid (55%) gestation.

There were no significant associations between socioeconomic status and homosexual behavior, fantasies or self-identification. Women who had never married more often considered themselves non-exclusively heterosexual (OR = 2.3, p = .03). In men, the birth order was positively associated with non-exclusively heterosexual identification: per child ranking later in ascending order of birth sequence the OR for a non-exclusively heterosexual identification was 1.1 (p = .04). Birth weight of men also had a small effect. Per kilo increase in birth weight the OR for a non-exclusively heterosexual identification was 1.7 (p = .07, adjusted for birth order). Adjusting the famine analyses for the variables birth order, birth weight, socioeconomic status, and marital status did not change the results.

Discussion

We found no effect of prenatal exposure to famine on sexual orientation and gender identity. Men and women exposed and not exposed to famine in utero were similar in terms of hetero/homosexual behavior, fantasies, and self-identification and gender identity.

In our study group, the prevalence of men who identified themselves as exclusively homosexual was 0.3%, while in women it was 0.6%. This is much lower than the mean prevalence of 7% in men and 6% in women in the general Dutch population as reported by the Rutgers Nisso group, the Dutch Institute for Social Sexual Research (Bakker & Vanwesenbeeck, 2006). An explanation for this large discrepancy could be that, in this somewhat older age-group, declaring oneself homosexual may still be a larger taboo than it is among younger people. Another explanation could be that participants felt a lack of privacy completing the questionnaire, because there was almost always a nurse nearby. These explanations could also explain the large number of people that used the “not applicable to me” option to answer items or did not fill out items at all. We tried to minimize the number of missing topics by taking the non-missing item as the mean when the other one of the two items concerning a topic was answered with “not applicable to me” or was not filled out, although this handling of missing items could have inflated non-heterosexuality because of the transient experimentation that may go on in adolescence. On the other hand, our results match those of another study among younger individuals that used the KSOG to assess sexual orientation and also found that nearly all (292 of 294) subjects reported predominant or exclusive heterosexual behavior and feelings and were self identified heterosexuals (Dessens et al., 1999).

Despite the probable underreporting of homosexuality, the validity of our data was supported by the association we found between the unmarried status of women and a non-exclusively heterosexual identification. Also, although we had no data on the number of preceding boys or girls, the number of preceding children was positively associated with a non-exclusively heterosexual identification. This result probably reflects the association between a greater number of older brothers and homosexuality, the so-called fraternal birth order effect (Blanchard, Zucker, Siegelman, Dickey, & Klassen, 1998). The fraternal birth order effect has been suggested to result in a lower birth weight (Blanchard & Ellis, 2001). Unexpectedly, however, we found a weak association between higher birth weight and a non-exclusive heterosexual orientation in men. This is opposite to the finding of Blanchard and Ellis (2001) that homosexual males weighed about 170 g less at birth than heterosexual males in a much larger population, and may thus be an accidental finding.

No participants indicated being exclusively transsexual. The prevalence for male-to-female transsexuals in the Netherlands is 1:11,900, and for female-to-male transsexuals is 1:30,400 (Bakker, Van Kesteren, Gooren, & Bezemer, 1993). Therefore, even if transsexualism were increased 10-fold in groups of people exposed to undernutrition than in the general population, the chance of finding an effect of prenatal exposure to famine on transsexuality in our cohort was small.

In conclusion, we did not find that exposure to famine in utero permanently affects the development of sexual orientation and gender identity. The small sample size of participants with non-exclusively heterosexual identification (possibly due to underreporting of homosexuality) reduced our power to detect any differences.