Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Rhetorical Citizenship and the Science of Science Communication

  • Published:
Argumentation Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Public policy decisions often require rhetorically-engaged citizens to have some understanding of the science and technology involved. On many current issues (GMO crops, vaccinations, climate change) sectors of the public hold views differing from those of most scientists, and they often do not support proposals based on the scientists’ views. The overall cultural authority of science has also been challenged in the last decade by several negative trends in the sciences themselves, including widely-reported cases of fraud and failures in replication. With the support of professional science organizations, science communication specialists have stepped in aggressively to address science’s communication problems scientifically. This paper will examine the assumptions behind their advice on scientific information, their recommended strategies of framing, narration, and projecting trustworthiness, and their characterizations of audiences and the nature of science itself. From the perspective of rhetorical argumentation, the science communication literature does not promote addressing audiences as citizens capable of rational argumentation. But the science of science communication is likely to remain the dominant approach to public science with the professional science community.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Any overall decline in the prestige of science, at least in the US, has been rebutted by the Pew Research Council, another source of survey-derived assessments, which claims that public perceptions of sciencehave not declined since the 1970s. They report that in 2018 44% of U.S. adults surveyed expressed a “great deal of confidence” in scientists, though percentages varied in relation to specific issues (Funk and Kennedy 2019).

  2. https://www.nsf.gov/events/event_summ.jsp?cntn_id=117845.

  3. Leshner’s list is longer and somewhat different from that offered in this chapter. He names on a PowerPoint slide, “Incidents of scientific misconduct, human subjects concerns, animal welfare issues, conflict of interest problems, publishing by press release, hyperbolic and exaggerated claims, appearing to suppress dissenting views, mistakes in scientific papers, failures to replicate.”.

  4. Van Noorden lists falsification, self-plagiarism, plagiarism, honest error and irreproducible results as the reasons for retraction. Moylan and Kowalczuk include among reasons compromised peer review, data falsification, image duplication, plagiarism, and a co-author unaware of a publication.

  5. 5 May 2017, 17 May 2017, 21 August 2017. See also The Week, 18 April 2016; Aeon, November 7, 2017. For summaries of the “science is broken” theme see Hilgard and Jamieson (2017), and National Science Foundation (2018), “Threats to Science’s Reputation,” 46.

  6. https://www.nsf.gov/events/event_summ.jsp?cntn_id=124056&org=OISE&preview=false.

  7. https://www.aaas.org/programs/communicating-science.

  8. https://www.aaas.org/page/center-public-engagement-science-and-technology/2019-annual-meeting-communicating-science-seminar.

  9. https://www.compassscicomm.org/compass-scicomm-dc.

  10. Cacciatore, Scheufele and Iyengar’s article is a perfect example of a definitional dissociation argument as described in Perelman and Olbrechts-Tyteca’s The New Rhetoric. The authors start with a confused concept, frame, and dissociate it into two variants under conveniently alliterative labels (emphasis and equivalence), subsequently evaluating one of the pair more positively than the other. Once the pair are in place, they are further separated in a series of antitheses. On this difference see Druckman and Lupia (2017, p. 3).

  11. For an analysis of Carbon Capture and Storage campaigns in terms of “information frames” interacting with other frames see Whitmarsh et al. (2019).

  12. In one of the articles, Downs defines narrative as a narrator’s voice setting up a conflict followed by action over time to a resolution (2014, p. 13627) while in another Dahlstrom defines narrative as a structure of cause and effect relationships between events over time that impact particular characters, and he stresses narrative as the antithesis of “logical-scientific communication” (2014, p. 13614). Each article cites a different meta-analytical study showing inconsistent results from narratives (Downs, 13628; Dahlstrom, p. 13615).

  13. https://sydney.edu.au/news-opinion/sydney-ideas/2018/is-storytelling-bad-for-science.html.

References

  • Allison, David B., Andrew W. Brown, Brandon J. George, and Kathryn A. Kaiser. 2016. Reproducibility: A tragedy of errors. Nature 530: 27–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, Monya, and Dan Penny. 2016. Is there a reproducibility crisis? Nature 533: 452–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Begley, C.Glenn, and Lee M. Ellis. 2012. Drug development: Raise standards for preclinical cancer research. Nature 483: 531–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, Daryl. 2011. Feeling the future: Experimental evidence for anomalous retroactive influences on cognition and affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 100(3): 407–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benedict, Carey. 2015. Science, now under scrutiny itself. New York: New York Times.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besley, John C., Anthony Dudo, and Martin Storksdieck. 2015. Scientists’ views about communication training. Journal of research in science teaching 52(2): 199–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besley, John C., Anthony D. Dudo, Shupei Yuan, and Niveen Abi Ghannam. 2016. Qualitative interviews with science communication trainers about communication objectives and goals. Science Communication 38(3): 356–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, Steven, and Rino Rappuoli. 2010. A crisis of public confidence in vaccines. Science Translational Medicine 2(61): 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohannon, John. 2013. Who’s afraid of peer review. Science 342: 60–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buehl, Jonathan. 2016. Revolution or evolution? Casing the impact of digital media on the rhetoric of science. In Science and the internet: Communicating knowledge in a digital age, ed. Alan G. Gross and Jonathan Buehl, 1–9. Amityville, NY: Baywood Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burdick, Alan. 2017. “Paging Dr. Fraud”: The fake publishers that are ruining science. The New Yorker https://www.newyorker.com/tech/annals-of-technology/paging-dr-fraud-the-fake-publishers-that-are-ruining-science.

  • Cacciatore, Michael A., Dietram A. Scheufele, and Shanto Iyengar. 2016. The end of framing as we know it ...and the future of media effects. Mass Communication and Society 19(1): 7–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, Siri. 2012. Government sanctions Harvard psychologist. Science 337: 1283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Check, Erika, and David Cyranoski. 2005. Korean scandal will have global fallout. Nature 438: 1056–1057.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cicero, 1981. Rhetorica ad Herennium [Loeb Classics]. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Committee on the Science of Science Communication. 2017. Communicating science effectively: A research agenda. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahlstrom, Michael F. 2014. Using narratives and storytelling to communicate science with nonexpert audiences. PNAS 111 (Supplement 4): 13614–13620.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Vrieze, Jop. 2017. Science wars’ veteran has a new mission. Science 358: 159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downs, Julie S. 2014. Prescriptive scientific narratives for communicating usable science. PNAS 111(Supplement 4): 13627–13633.

    Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, James N. and Arthur Lupia. 2017. Using frames to make scientific communication more effective. The Oxford handbook of the science of science communication, eds. Kathleen Hall Jamieson, Dan Kahan, and Dietram A. Scheufele, 351–360. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Editorial. 2012. Fraud in the scientific literature. New York: New York Times.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, Tamsin. 2017. The perils of explaining science. BBC Inside Science [Radio 4: 12 January 2017]. https://www.bbc.co.uk/programmes/b087pf71.

  • Engber, Daniel. 2017. Is science broken? Or is it self-correcting? Slate (21 August).

  • Enserink, Martin. 2012. Final report on Stapel also blames field as a whole. Science 338: 1270–1271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahnestock, Jeanne, and Marie Secor. 1988. The stases in scientific and literary argument. Written Communication 5(4): 427–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahnestock, Jeanne, and Marie Secor. 1990. A rhetoric of argument, 2nd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahnestock, Jeanne. 2013. Promoting the discipline: Rhetorical studies of science, technology and medicine. Poroi 9.1, 6.

  • Fang, Ferris C., R. Grant Steen, and Arturo Casadevall. 2012. Misconduct accounts for the majority of retracted scientific publications. PNAS 109(42): 17028–17033.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feenstra, C.F.J., T. Mikunda, and S. Brunsting. 2010. What happened in Barendrecht? Case study on the planned onshore carbon dioxide storage in Barendrecht, the Netherlands. Global CCS Institute: Energy Research Center of the Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fountain, Henry. 2014. Science journal pulls 60 Papers in peer-review fraud. New York: New York Times.

    Google Scholar 

  • Funk, Cary and Brian Kennedy. 2019. Public confidence in scientists has remained stable for decades. Fact Tank. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2019/03/22/public-confidence-in-scientists-has-remained-stable-for-decades/.

  • Franzosi, Roberto and Stephanie Vicari. 2013. What’s in a text? Answers from frame analysis and rhetoric for measuring meaning systems and argumentative structures. chrome- extension://oemmndcbldboiebfnladdacbdfmadadm/https://www.unive.it/media/allegato/Scuola-Dottorale/2013/Rhetoric_and_Frame_Analysis_May_2013.pdf.

  • Franzosi, Roberto and Stephanie Vicari. 2018. What’s in a text? Answers from frame analysis and rhetoric for measuring meaning systems and argumentative structures.” Rhetorica 36.4: 393- 429 [a different version from the 2013 paper with the same title].

  • Hamilton, Walter, Trans. 1986. Plato: Gorgias. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin Books.

  • Harris, Gardiner. 2010. Journal retracts 1998 paper linking autism to vaccines. New York: New York Times.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, Richard. 2017. Rigor mortis: How sloppy science creates worthless cures, crushes hope, and wastes billions. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, Joseph and Kathleen Hall Jamieson. 2017. Science as ‘broken’ versus science as ‘self- correcting’: How retractions and peer-review problems are exploited to attack science. Oxford handbook of the science of science communication, eds. Kathleen Hall Jamieson, Dan Kahan, and Dietram A. Scheufele, 85–92/New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Holland, Earle M. 2007. The risks and advantages of framing science. Science 317: 1168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoy, Anne Q. 2018. Engagement helps scientists tackle global challenges. Science 361: 372–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ioannides, John P.A. 2005a. Contradicted and initially stronger effects in highly cited clinical research. JAMA 294(2): 218–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ioannides, John P.A. 2005b. Why most published research findings are false. PloS medicine 2.8.e124: 0696-0701.

  • Iyengar, Shanto, and Douglas S. Massey. 2019. Science communication in a post-truth society. PNAS 116(16): 7653–7661.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jamieson, Kathleen Hall. 2018. Crisis or self-correction: Rethinking media narratives about the well-being of science. PNAS 115(11): 2620–2627.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahan, D.M. 2012. Why are we poles apart on climate change? Nature 488: 255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, Yarden. 2013. Against storytelling of scientfic results. Nature Methods 10(11): 1045.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, George, Trans. 1991. Aristotle: On rhetoric. New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Kennedy, George, Trans. 2003. Progymnasmata: Greek textbooks of prose composition and rhetoric. Atlanta: Society of Biblical Literature.

  • Kim, Sei-Hill, Ed. 2019. Beyond framing: A forum for framing researchers. Journalism & mass communication quarterly 96(1):12–30.

  • Kock, Christian. 2011. Generalizing stasis theory for everyday use. In Bending opinion: Essays on persuasion in the public domain, ed. Ton Van Haaften, Henrike Jansen, Jaap DeJong, and Willem Koetsenruijter, 81–93. Leiden: Leiden University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kock, Christian. 2017. Gorgias reloaded: A new-found dialogue between Gorgias and Socrates. Deliberative rhetoric: arguing about doing, 18–25. Windsor, Ontario: Windsor Studies in Argumentation.

  • Kolata, Gina. 2013. Scientific articles accepted (personal checks, too). New York: New York Times.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolata, Gina. 2018. His promise to heal bad hearts relied on a mountain of false data. New York: New York Times.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolata, Gina. 2019. ‘Predatory publisher’ is fined $50 million. New York: New York Times.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krippendorff, Klaus. 2017. Three concepts to retire. Annals of the international communication association 41(1): 92–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuijper, I.Margriet. 2011. Public acceptance challenges for onshore CO2 storage in Barendrecht. Energy Procedia 4: 6226–6233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuypers, Jim A. 2010. Framing analysis from a rhetorical perspective. In Doing news framing analysis, ed. Paul D’Angelo and Jim A. Kuypers, 286–311. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leshner, Alan I. 2006. Science and public engagement. The chronicle of higher education 53(8): 65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leshner, Alan I. 2012. Capably communicating science. Science 337: 777.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leshner, Alan I. 2013. Communication challenges in the life sciences. PILS Roundtable (3 June 2013).

  • Leshner, Alan I. 2015. Bridging the opinion gap. Editorial. Science 347: 459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, Simon L. 2014. Scientist-versus-activist debates mislead the public.” Nature 506: 409.

  • Lockwood, Toby. 2017. Public outreach approaches for carbon capture storage projects. London: IEA Clean Coal Centre.

    Google Scholar 

  • Makri, Anita. 2017. Give the public the tools to trust scientists. Nature 541: 261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mellor, Felicity. 2018. Review: The Oxford handbook of the science of science communication. Public understanding of science 27(6): 750–752.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moylan, Elizabeth C., and Maria K. Kowalczuk. 2016. Why articles are retracted: A retrospective cross-sectional study of retraction notices at BioMed Central. British Medical Journal Open 6: e012047.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Academy of Sciences. 2018. The science of science communication III. Stephen Olson, Rapporteur. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.

  • National Science Board. 2018. Science and engineering indicators 2018. Arlington, VA: National Science Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Science Foundation. 2017. InfoBrief. National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics. NSF 18-300.

  • Nisbet, Matthew C. 2010. Framing science: a new paradigm in public engagement. In Communicating Science, eds. LeeAnn Kahlor and Patricia A. Stout, 40–57. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbet, Matthew C. and Chris Mooney. 2007. Framing science. Science 316:56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, Randy. 2015. Houston, we have a narrative: Why science needs story. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Open Science Collaborative. 2015. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 349: 940–944.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perelman, Chaim, and Lucie Olbrechts-Tyteca. 1969. The new rhetoric: A treatise on argumentation. Notre Dame IN: University of Notre Dame Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollack, Andrew. 2014. Stem cell research papers are retracted. New York: New York Times.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, Florian, Thomas Schlange, and Khusru Asadullah. 2011. Believe it or not: How much can we rely on published data on potential drug targets? Nature Reviews Drug Discovery 10(9): 712.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roston, Michael. 2015. Retracted scientific studies: A growing list. New York Times (28 May): https://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2015/05/28/science/retractions-scientific-studies.html?mtrref=www.google.com&gwh=CE4855B88B8AE48D77737CE90D27BE5F&gwt=pay.

  • Rowland, F. Sherwood. 1993. President’s lecture: The need for scientific communication with the public. Science 260: 1571–1576.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, Jean-Jacques. 2001. Society talks back. Nature 412: 585–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schenkel, Roland. 2010. The challenge of feeding scientific advice into policy-making. Science 330: 1749–1751.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, Tom. 2018. Preprints could promote confusion and distortion. Nature 559: 445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, Joseph P., Leif D. Nelson, and Uri Simonsohn. 2011. False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science 22(11): 1359–1366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Światkowski, Wojciech, and Benoît Dompnier. 2017. Replicability crisis in social psychology: Looking at the past to find new pathways for the future. International review of social psychology 30(1): 111–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trafimow, David, and Michael Marks. 2015. Editorial. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 37: 1–2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuller, David. 2012. Fallout from fatigue syndrome retraction is wide. New York: New York Times.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Linden, Sander, Anthony Leiserowitz, Seth Rosenthal, and Edward Maibach. 2017. Inoculating the public against misinformation about climate change. Global Challenges https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/gch2.201600008.

  • Van Noorden, R. 2011. The trouble with retractions”. Nature 478: 26–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wasserstein, Ronald L., and Nicole A. Lazar. 2016. The ASA’s statement on p-values: Context, process and purpose. The American statistician 70(2): 129–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitmarsh, Lorraine, Dimitrios Xenias, and Christopher R. Jones. 2019. Framing effects on public support for carbon capture and storage. Palgrave Communications. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-019-0217-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, Carl. 2012. A sharp rise in retractions prompts calls for reform. New York: New York Times.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jeanne Fahnestock.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fahnestock, J. Rhetorical Citizenship and the Science of Science Communication. Argumentation 34, 371–387 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10503-019-09499-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10503-019-09499-7

Keywords

Navigation