Skip to main content
Log in

Looking for Arguments

  • Published:
Argumentation Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

How do people find arguments while engaged in a discussion? Following an analogy with visual search, a mechanism that performs this task is described. It is a metarepresentational device that examines representations in a mostly serial manner until it finds a good enough argument supporting one’s position. It is argued that the mechanism described in dual process theories as ‘system 2’, or analytic reasoning fulfills these requirements. This provides support for the hypothesis that reasoning serves an argumentative function.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The Psychology of Proof (Rips 1994) for mental logic theory, How we Reason (Johnson-Laird 2006) for mental models theory and Hypothetical Thinking (Evans 2007) for (one) dual process theory. It should be said however that Rips has devoted a lot of work to argumentation aside from his theories of reasoning.

  2. Note that the example provided is one of a mixed difference of opinion—that is, one speaker rejects the acceptability of a standpoint. The example of a non-mixed difference could also have been provided—for instance, Margo could simply have raised doubts about the option of taking the Tube.

  3. A similar idea is expressed by Perelman and Olbrechts-Tyteca in their classic study of argumentation: ‘One datum in argumentation consists of the agreements available to the speaker as supports for his argument. But this element is so large and capable of being used in so many different ways, that the manner in which one makes use of it is of paramount importance. Accordingly, before examining the use of this datum in argumentation, it is essential that we say something of the part played by preliminary selection of the elements that are to serve as the starting point of the argument and by the adaptation of these elements to its purpose’ (Perelman and Olbrechts-Tyteca 1969, p. 115).

  4. See Sperber (2000). This point does not garner unanimity (Dancy 2000) but is consensual enough to warrant accepting it, at least provisionally, here.

  5. Accessibility, or relevance, will be particularly effective at organizing the space of possible arguments after someone has already given an argument. In this case, relevance mechanisms may triangulate quite effectively on counter-arguments. For instance, if Margo says ‘walking will be fast’, one of the first things that may spring to Simon’s mind is ‘but taking the Tube will be even faster,’ precluding the need for an extensive search.

  6. This argument is developed in Mercier and Sperber (in press) so it will only be brushed here.

  7. The exception would be arguments that are made very often in similar contexts and for similar audiences. In this case, it should be possible to have an idea of what a good argument may be. A teacher may think, for instance, that arguments by analogy are particularly efficient in explaining a certain class of concepts.

References

  • Anderson, R.C., C. Chinn, J. Chang, M. Waggoner, and H. Yi. 1997. On the logical integrity of children's arguments. Cognition and Instruction 15(2): 135–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R.C., C. Chinn, M. Waggoner, and K. Nguyen. 1998. Intellectually stimulating story discussions. In Literacy for all, ed. J. Osborn and F. Lehr, 170–196. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailenson, J.N., and L.J. Rips. 1996. Informal reasoning and burden of proof. Applied Cognitive Psychology 10(7): 3–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L.W. 1983. Ad hoc categories. Memory and Cognition 11(3): 211–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baum, L.A., J.H. Danovitch, and F.C. Keil. 2007. Children’s sensitivity to circular explanations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 100(2): 146–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, D.C., and Z. Dienes. 1993. Implicit learning. Hove: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brem, S.K., and L.J. Rips. 2000. Explanation and evidence in informal argument. Cognitive Science 24: 573–604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J.S., J.J. Goodnow, and G.A. Austin. 1956. A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butera, F., P. Legrenzi, G. Mugny, and J.A. Pérez. 1992. Influence sociale et raisonnement. Bulletin de Psychologie 45: 144–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., A. Liberman, and A.H. Eagly. 1989. Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In Unintended thought, ed. J.S. Uleman and J.A. Bargh, 212–252. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corner, A., U. Hahn, and M. Oakfsord. 2006. The slippery slope argument: probability, utility and category reappraisal. In Proceedings of the 28th Annual meeting of the cognitive science society.

  • Cowley, M., and R.M.J. Byrne. 2005. When falsification is the only path to truth. Paper presented at the twenty-seventh annual conference of the cognitive science society, Stresa, Italy.

  • Dancy, J. 2000. Practical reality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, E., T. Gilovich, and D.T. Regan. 2002. Motivated reasoning and performance on the Wason selection task. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28(10): 1379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J., and G. Humphreys. 1989. Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review 96: 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T. 1989. Bias in human reasoning: Causes and consequences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T. 1996. Deciding before you think: Relevance and reasoning in the selection task. British Journal of Psychology 87: 223–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T. 1998. Matching bias in conditional reasoning: Do we understand it after 25 years? Thinking and Reasoning 4: 45–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T. 2002. Logic and human reasoning: An assessment of the deduction paradigm. Psychological Bulletin 128(6): 978–996.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T. 2003. In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(10): 454–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T. 2006. The heuristic-analytic theory of reasoning: Extension and evaluation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 13(3): 378–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T. 2007. Hypothetical thinking: Dual processes in reasoning and judgment. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T. 2008. Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59: 255–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T., and K. Frankish (eds.). 2009. In two minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T., and J.S. Lynch. 1973. Matching bias in the selection task. British Journal of Psychology 64(3): 391–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T., and D.E. Over. 1996. Rationality and reasoning. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T., and P.C. Wason. 1976. Rationalization in a reasoning task. British Journal of Psychology 67: 479–486.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., and G.V. Bodenhausen. 2006. Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin 132(5): 692–731.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., M.J. Rattermann, and K.D. Forbus. 1993. The roles of similarity in transfer: Separating retrievability from inferential soundness. Cognitive Psychology 25(4): 524–575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., P.M. Todd, and ABC Research Group. 1999. Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T., D. Griffin, and D. Kahneman (eds.). 2002. Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Girotto, V., M. Kemmelmeier, D. Sperber, and J.-B. Van der Henst. 2001. Inept reasoners or pragmatic virtuosos? Relevance and the deontic selection task. Cognition 81(2): 69–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hahn, U., and M. Oaksford. 2007. The rationality of informal argumentation: A bayesian approach to reasoning fallacies. Psychological Review 114(3): 704–732.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hahn, U., M. Oaksford, and H. Bayindir. 2005. How convinced should we be by negative evidence? In Proceedings of the 27th Annual meeting of the cognitive science society.

  • Johnson-Laird, P.N. 2006. How we reason. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P.N., P. Legrenzi, V. Girotto, and M.S. Legrenzi. 2000. Illusions in reasoning about consistency. Science 288(5465): 531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. 2003. A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist 58(9): 697–720.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., and S. Frederick. 2002. Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgement. In Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment, ed. T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, and D. Kahneman, 49–81. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., P. Slovic, and A. Tversky. 1982. Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klauer, K.C., J. Musch, and B. Naumer. 2000. On belief bias in syllogistic reasoning. Psychological Review 107(4): 852–884.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klayman, J. 1995. Varieties of confirmation bias. In Decision making from the perspective of cognitive psychology, ed. J.R. Busemeyer, R. Hastie, and D.L. Medin, 385–418. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klayman, J., and Y. Ha. 1987. Confirmation, disconfirmation, and information in hypothesis testing. Psychological Review 94: 211–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, G. 1998. Sources of power: How people make decisions. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. 1991. The skills of arguments. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., V.F. Shaw, and M. Felton. 1997. Effects of dyadic interaction on argumentative reasoning. Cognition and Instruction 15: 287–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legrenzi, P., V. Girotto, and P.N. Johnson-Laird. 1993. Focussing in reasoning and decision making. Cognition 49: 37–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W.J.M. 1989. Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W.J.M., A. Roelofs, and A.S. Meyer. 1999. A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 1–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, M. 1966. Hypothesis behaviour by humans during discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology 71: 331–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, Z. 2002. A saliency map in primary visual cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(1): 9–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, E.J., and L.J. Ball. 2005. Think-aloud protocols and the selection task: Evidence for relevance effects and rationalisation processes. Thinking and Reasoning 11: 35–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maciejovsky, B., and D.V. Budescu. 2007. Collective induction without cooperation? Learning and knowledge transfer in cooperative groups and competitive auctions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92(5): 854–870.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, A.B., W.T. Maddox, and D.A. Worthy. 2006. Choking and excelling under pressure. Psychological Science 17: 944–948.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H. 2011a. On the universality of argumentative reasoning. Journal of Cognition and Culture 11(1–2): 85–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H. 2011b. Reasoning serves argumentation in children. Cognitive Development 26(3): 177–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H. 2011c. When experts argue: Explaining the best and the worst of reasoning. Argumentation 25(3): 313–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H. What good is moral reasoning? Mind & Society (in press).

  • Mercier, H., and H. Landemore. Reasoning is for arguing: Understanding the successes and failures of deliberation. Political Psychology (in press).

  • Mercier, H., and D. Sperber. 2009. Intuitive and reflective inferences. In In two minds, ed. J.S.B.T. Evans and K. Frankish. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H., and D. Sperber. 2011. Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34(2): 57–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milch, K.F., E.U. Weber, K.C. Appelt, M.J.J. Handgraaf, and D.H Krantz. 2009. From individual preference construction to group decisions: Framing effects and group processes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes.

  • Moshman, D., and M. Geil. 1998. Collaborative reasoning: Evidence for collective rationality. Thinking and Reasoning 4(3): 231–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mussweiler, T., and F. Strack. 1999a. Comparing is believing: A selective accessibility model of judgmental anchoring. In European review of social psychology, vol. 10, ed. W. Stroebe and M. Hewstone, 135–168. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mussweiler, T., and F. Strack. 1999b. Hypothesis-consistent testing and semantic priming in the anchoring paradigm: A selective accessibility model. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 35: 136–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mussweiler, T., F. Strack, and T. Pfeiffer. 2000. Overcoming the inevitable anchoring effect: Considering the opposite compensates for selective accessibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 26(9): 1142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mynatt, C.R., M.E. Doherty, and W. Dragan. 1993. Information relevance, working memory and the consideration of alternatives. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 46A: 759–778.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuman, Y. 2003. Go ahead, prove that God does not exist! On high school students’ ability to deal with fallacious arguments. Learning and Instruction 13(4): 367–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuman, Y., M.P. Weinstock, and A. Glasner. 2006. The effect of contextual factors on the judgement of informal reasoning fallacies. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 59(2): 411–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R.S. 1998. Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomena in many guises. Review of General Psychology 2: 175–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R.E., and L. Ross. 1980. Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice–Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R.E., and T. Wilson. 1977. Telling more than we can know. Psychological Review 84(1): 231–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford, M., and U. Hahn. 2004. A Bayesian approach to the argument from ignorance. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 58(2): 75–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perelman, C., and L. Olbrechts-Tyteca. 1969. The new rhetoric: A treatise on argumentation. Notre Dame: University of Notre Dame Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D.N. 1985. Postprimary education has little impact on informal reasoning. Journal of Educational Psychology 77: 562–571.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R.E., and J.T. Cacioppo. 1986. The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 19, ed. L. Berkowitz, 123–205. Orlando: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R.E., and D.T. Wegener. 1998. Attitude change: Multiple roles for persuasion variables. In The handbook of social psychology, vol. 1, ed. D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, and G. Lindzey, 323–390. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I., and C.R.R. Snyder. 1975. Attention and cognitive control. In Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium, ed. R.L. Solso. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A.S. 1993. Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rips, L.J. 1994. The psychology of proof: Deductive reasoning in human thinking. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rips, L.J. 1998. Reasoning and conversation. Psychological Review 105: 411–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rips, L.J. 2002. Circular reasoning. Cognitive Science 26: 767–795.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, M.J., and E.J. Newton. 2002. Inspection times, the change task, and the rapid response selection task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 54: 1031–1048.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozenblit, L., and F. Keil. 2002. The misunderstood limits of folk science. Cognitive Science 26: 521–562.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D.L. 1987. Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 13(3): 501–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shafir, E., I. Simonson, and A. Tversky. 1993. Reason-based choice. Cognition 49(1–2): 11–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaklee, H., and B. Fischhoff. 1982. Strategies of information search in causal analysis. Memory & Cognition 10(6): 520–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, V.F. 1996. The cognitive processes in informal reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning 2(1): 51–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H.A. 1982. Models of bounded rationality. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S.A. 1996. The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin 119(1): 3–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snoeck Hankemans, A.F. 1992. Analysing complex argumentation. The reconstruction of multiple and coordinatively compound argumentation in a critical disussion. Amsterdam: SicSat.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. 2000. Metarepresentations in an evolutionary perspective. In Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective, ed. D. Sperber, 117–137. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. 2001. An evolutionary perspective on testimony and argumentation. Philosophical Topics 29: 401–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., F. Cara, and V. Girotto. 1995. Relevance theory explains the selection task. Cognition 57: 31–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., and D. Wilson. 1995. Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. 2004. The Robot’s rebellion. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toulmin, S. 1958. The uses of argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., and D. Kahneman. 1974. Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science 185: 1124–1131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Henst, J.-B. 2006. Relevance effects in reasoning. Mind & Society 5(2): 229–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Henst, J.-B., D. Sperber, and G. Politzer. 2002. When is a conclusion worth deriving? A relevance-based analysis of indeterminate relational problems. Thinking and Reasoning 8: 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Eemeren, F.H. 2010. Strategic maneuvering in argumentative discourse: Extending the pragma-dialectical theory of argumentation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Eemeren, F.H., B. Garssen, and B. Meuffels. 2009. Fallacies and judgments of reasonableness: Empirical research concerning the pragma-dialectical discussion rules. Amsterdam: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Eemeren, F.H., and R. Grootendorst. 2004. A systematic theory of argumentation: The pragma-dialectical approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Eemeren, F.H., R. Grootendorst, and F. Snoeck Henkemans. 1996. Fundamentals of argumentation theory. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walton, D.N., C. Reed, and F. Macagno. 2008. Argumentation schemes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P.C. 1966. Reasoning. In New horizons in psychology: I, ed. B.M. Foss, 106–137. Harmandsworth: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P.C., and J.S.B.T. Evans. 1975. Dual processes in reasoning? Cognition 3: 141–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P.C., and P.J. Brooks. 1979. THOG: The anatomy of a problem. Psychological Research 41: 79–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webb, N.M., M.L. Franke, M. Ing, A. Chan, T. De, D. Freund, et al. 2008. The role of teacher instructional practices in student collaboration. Contemporary Educational Psychology 33(3): 360–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinstock, M., Y. Neuman, and I. Tabak. 2004. Missing the point or missing the norms? Epistemological norms as predictors of students’ ability to identify fallacious arguments. Contemporary Educational Psychology 29(1): 77–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T.D., and E.W. Dunn. 2004. Self-knowledge: Its limits, value, and potential for improvement.

  • Wilson, T.D., S. Lindsey, and T.Y. Schooler. 2000. A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review 107(1): 101–126.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hugo Mercier.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mercier, H. Looking for Arguments. Argumentation 26, 305–324 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10503-011-9256-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10503-011-9256-1

Keywords

Navigation