Skip to main content
Log in

Predicting one’s own death: the relationship between subjective and objective nearness to death in very old age

  • Original Investigation
  • Published:
European Journal of Ageing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous research found that the perception of a limited remaining lifetime is related to goal setting, social network composition, attitudes, and behavior. However, to better understand those findings, it is important to know if this subjective perception of being close to death corresponds with the time a person actually survives. The aim of the present study was to examine the predictive and time–dynamic relationship between subjective and objective nearness to death using 16-year longitudinal data from the Berlin Aging Study (Baltes and Mayer 1999; N = 516 older adults between 70 and 104 years). Older adults who felt close to death at the first measurement occasion were more likely to die over the following 16 years than persons who did not report feeling close to dying. Results of multilevel analyses revealed that there was a time–dynamic relationship such that subjective nearness to death increased as a function of objective nearness to death. Our results indicate that very old adults seem to have quite accurate perceptions of their nearness to death.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Due to a shortened assessment protocol, no data was available for T2.

  2. We acknowledge that—as it is generally the case for longitudinal studies that use distance-to-death as a time metric—data incompleteness was not completely missing at random (i.e., due to mortality; but see Schafer and Graham 2002, who suggest that missing at random procedures may be applicable in such cases). This may have reduced the variance as well as the covariance of our variables.

  3. Objective nearness to death explained 6% of the within-person variance in subjective nearness to death (\( R_{\text{within}}^{2} = 0.058 \)).

References

  • Baltes PB, Mayer KU (eds) (1999) The Berlin aging study: aging from 70 to 100. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes PB, Smith J (2003) New frontiers in the future of aging: from successful aging of the young old to the dilemmas of the fourth age. Gerontology 49:123–135

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes MM, Freund AM, Horgas SL (1999) Men and women in the Berlin Aging Study. In: Baltes PB, Mayer KU (eds) The Berlin Aging Study: aging from 70 to 100. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, pp 259–281

  • Bosworth HB, Schaie KW, Willis SL, Siegler IC (1999) Age and distance to death in the Seattle Longitudinal Study. Res Aging 21:723–738

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter J, Rothermund K (2003) Intentionality and time in human development and aging:compensation and goal adjustment in changing developmental contexts. In: Staudinger UM, Lindenberger U (eds) Understanding human development: dialogues with lifespan psychology. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 105–124

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter J, Rothermund K, Kranz D, Kühn W (2010) Final decentrations: personal goals, rationality perspectives, and the awareness of life’s finitude. Eur Psychol 15:152–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen LL (2006) The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science 312:1913–1915

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chida Y, Steptoe A (2008) Positive psychological well-being and mortality: a quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosom Med 70:741–756

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerstorf D, Ram N, Röcke C, Lindenberger U, Smith J (2008) Decline in life satisfaction in old age: longitudinal evidence for links to distance-to-death. Psychol Aging 23:154–168

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg JL, Arndt J (2008) The implications of death for health: a terror management model for behavioral health promotion. Psychol Rev 115:1032–1053

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg J, Pyszczynski T, Solomon S, Rosenblatt A, Veeder M, Kirkland S et al (1990) Evidence for terror management theory II: the effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview. J Pers Soc Psychol 58:308–318

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurd MD, McGarry K (2002) The predictive validity of subjective probabilities of survival. Econ J 112:966–985

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Idler EL, Benyamini Y (1997) Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. J Health Soc Behav 38:21–37

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jopp D, Rott C, Oswald F (2008) Valuation of life in old and very old age: the role of sociodemographic, social, and health resources for positive adaptation. The Gerontologist 48:646–658

    Google Scholar 

  • King LA, Hicks JA, Abdelkhalik J (2009) Death, life, scarcity, and value: an alternative perspective on the meaning of death. Psychol Sci 20(12):1459–1462

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kotter-Grühn D, Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn A, Gerstorf D, Smith J (2009) Self-perceptions of aging predict mortality and change with approaching death: 16-year longitudinal results from the Berlin Aging Study. Psychol Aging 24:654–667

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang FR (2000) Endings and continuity of social relationships: maximizing intrinsic benefits within personal networks when feeling near to death. J Soc Pers Relationsh 17:155–182

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang FR, Carstensen LL (2002) Time counts: future time perspective, goals, and social relationships. Psychol Aging 17:125–139

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang FR, Baltes PB, Wagner GG (2007) Desired lifetime and end-of-life desires across adulthood from 20 to 90: a dual-source information model. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 62B:P268–P276

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawton MP, Moss M, Hoffman C, Kleban MH, Ruckdeschel K, Winter L (2001) Valuation of life: a concept and a scale. J Aging Health 13:3–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawton MP, Moss MS, Winter L, Hoffman C (2002) Motivation in later life: personal projects and well-being. Psychol Aging 17:539–547

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindenberger U, Baltes PB (1997) Intellectual functioning in old and very old age: cross-sectional results from the Berlin Aging Study. Psychol Aging 12:410–432

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Littell RC, Miliken GA, Stoup WW, Wolfinger RD (1996) SAS systems for mixed models. SAS Institute, Cary, NC

    Google Scholar 

  • Little RJA, Rubin DB (2002) Statistical analysis with missing data. Wiley, New York, NY

  • Martens A, Greenberg J, Schimel J, Landau M (2004) Ageism and death: effects of mortality salience and perceived similarity to elders on reactions to elderly people. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 30:1524–1536

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maxfield M, Pyszczynski T, Kluck B, Cox C, Greenberg J, Solomon S et al (2007) Age-related differences in responses to thoughts of one’s own death: mortality salience and judgements of moral transgressions. Psychol Aging 22:341–353

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer KU, Maas I, Wagner M (1999) Socioeconomic conditions and social inequalities in old age. In: Baltes PB, Mayer KU (eds) The Berlin Aging Study: aging from 70 to 100. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, pp 227–255

  • Mirowsky J (1997) Age, subjective life expectancy, and the sense of control: the horizon hypothesis. J Gerontol Soc Sci 52B:S125–S134

    Google Scholar 

  • Mirowsky J (1999) Subjective life expectancy in the US: correspondence to actuarial estimates by age, sex, and race. Soc Sci Med 49:967–979

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mroczek DK, Spiro A III (2007) Personality change influences mortality in older men. Psychol Sci 18:371–376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neimeyer RA, Wittkowski J, Moser R (2004) Psychological research on death attitudes: an overview and evaluation. Death Stud 28:309–340

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski T, Greenberg J, Solomon S, Arndt J, Schimel J (2004) Why do people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical review. Psychol Bull 130:435–468

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schafer J, Graham J (2002) Missing data: our view of the state of the art. Psychol Methods 7:147–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman MEP (2000) Optimism, pessimism, and mortality. Mayo Clin Proc 75:133–134

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith J, Delius JAM (2010) Längsschnittliche Datenerhebungen der Berliner Altersstudie (BASE). Studiendesign, Stichproben und Forschungsthemen 1990–2009 [Longitudinal data collections of the Berlin Aging Study: Design, samples, and research topics 1990–2009]. In: Lindenberger U, Smith J, Mayer KU, Baltes PB (eds) Die Berliner Altersstudie, 3rd edn. Akademie Verlag, Berlin, pp 113–131

  • Smith VK, Taylor DH Jr, Sloan FA (2001) Longevity expectations and death: can people predict their own demise? Am Econ Rev 91:1126–1134

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinhagen-Thiessen E, Borchelt M (1999) Morbidity, medication, and functional limitations in very old age. In: Baltes PB, Mayer KU (eds) The Berlin aging study: aging from 70 to 100. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, pp 131–166

    Google Scholar 

  • van Doorn C, Kasl SV (1998) Can parental longevity and self-rated life expectancy predict mortality among older persons? Results from an Australian cohort. J Gerontol Soc Sci 53B:S28–S34

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegelmann JP, Lippke S, Schwarzer R (2006) Subjective residual life expectancy in health self-regulation. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 61B:P195–P201

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This article reports data from the Berlin Aging Study (BASE; www.base-berlin.mpg.de). BASE was initiated by the late Paul B. Baltes, in collaboration with Hanfried Helmchen, Psychiatry, Elisabeth Steinhagen-Thiessen, Internal Medicine and Geriatrics, and Karl Ulrich Mayer, Sociology. Financial support came from the Max Planck Society, the Free University of Berlin, the German Federal Ministry for Research and Technology (1989–1991, 13 TA 011 + 13 TA 011/A), the German Federal Ministry for Family, Senior Citizens, Women, and Youth (1992–1998, 314-1722-102/9 + 314-1722-102/9a), and the Berlin-Brandenburg Academy of Sciences’ Research Group on Aging and Societal Development (1994–1999). During preparation of this article, Dana Kotter-Grühn was supported by a research fellowship (KO 3579/3-1) awarded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft/German Research Foundation (DFG). We thank the anonymous reviewers for very useful comments on a previous draft.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dana Kotter-Grühn.

Additional information

Communicated by H.-W. Wahl.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kotter-Grühn, D., Grühn, D. & Smith, J. Predicting one’s own death: the relationship between subjective and objective nearness to death in very old age. Eur J Ageing 7, 293–300 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10433-010-0165-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10433-010-0165-1

Keywords

Navigation