Introduction

Konrad Lorenz introduced the concept of imprinting in his seminal work, "Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels", published in The Journal of Ornithology in 1935. Imprinting is a process allowing precocial animals, especially nidifugous birds, to form a lasting social bond with a conspicuous stimulus encountered at an early age (Vallortigara and Versace 2018). In his seminal work, Lorenz had already noted that imprinting was closely associated with the bird's "inborn perceptory patterns", which serve as innate biases guiding the young birds in selecting proper stimuli.

Building on Lorenz's seminal work and taking advantage of the domestic chick as a model organism, ethologists began to experimentally investigate specific aspects of imprinting, such as the timing, duration, and characteristics of the stimuli that elicit and guide this process (Ramsay and Hess 1954). Just after hatching, chicks were exposed to a plethora of stimuli while scientists focused on two measures: the initial responses and approach/following behaviors of the chicks toward a stimulus, and the ability of the chicks to discriminate objects they have been exposed to (also termed ‘imprinting stimuli’) from novel ones (Andrew 1991; Bateson 1964; Bolhuis 1991). While the extent to which chicks initially approach and follow one stimulus has often been used to assess the effectiveness of a stimulus in eliciting imprinting, the extent to which chicks discriminate their imprinting stimulus from novel ones has commonly been used to evaluate the strength and development of imprinting and its memory. Using these measurements, it has been shown that imprinting must occur in the first days following hatching, typically up to day three in chicks (Yamaguchi et al. 2012) and that 14 h of exposure to a predisposed stimulus are sufficient for domestic chicks to develop a strong and steady preference for it (Lemaire et al. 2021). Additionally, certain features, such as color (red, orange, and blue), structures (face-like stimuli or head regions), and motion patterns (biological motion), are more likely to attract chicks' attention as well as facilitate future imprinting, confirming Lorenz’s original observations (Bolhuis 1991; Lemaire and Vallortigara 2023; Miura et al. 2019; Regolin et al. 2000; Rosa-Salva et al. 2015, 2021; Vallortigara 2021).

Nowadays, imprinting has become a valuable tool for investigating cognition and the onset of social behaviors in the laboratory (Di Giorgio et al. 2017; Rogers 2014; Rose 2000, 2003; Solomonia and McCabe 2015; Versace and Vallortigara 2015). However, certain aspects related to the optimal methods for leveraging the imprinting phenomenon have yet to be fully elucidated. While previous studies suggested that real moving stimuli are more effective than stationary ones in eliciting imprinting (Ten Cate 1986, 1989), other research suggested that flickering stimuli—i.e., stimuli appearing and disappearing at a specific rate—are as effective as motion stimuli (James 1959, 1960; Pumphrey 1948). Over the years, different flickering rates have been studied using flickering lights (Gottlieb and Simner 1969; Simner 1973), and it has been found that lights flickering at around 4 Hz were more effective at eliciting approach while lights flickering at higher frequencies (such as 12 and 24 Hz) were aversive. Now that the study of imprinting has moved from exposure to real stimuli to virtual ones, the most effective procedures for displaying stimuli on video screens for imprinting purposes require further investigation.

The present study aims to elucidate the effectiveness of different virtual stimuli presentation methods in an imprinting context and assess how these influence the imprinting memory of domestic chicks. Our investigation is based on the use of an automated setup previously developed (Lemaire et al. 2021; Zanon et al. 2021) and utilizes well-established imprinting procedure and stimuli, for which we know chicks’ imprinting preferences and naive biases (Lemaire et al. 2021). In a first experiment, we evaluated the attractiveness of a stimulus to newly hatched chicks by manipulating its dynamical presentation on screens. Initially, we assessed the attractiveness of various flickering frequencies to find the most attractive ones below the chicks’ critical flicker fusion frequency (Lisney et al. 2011). Subsequently, we selected the most appealing frequencies and compared them to other presentation methods (translatory motion and stationary display) that have been reported to have varying levels of attractiveness and that could influence imprinting. In the second experiment, we investigated whether different presentation methods of an imprinting stimulus influence chicks’ memory and development of the imprinting memory. To achieve this, we exposed chicks to the same stimulus as in the first experiment, using the different motion methods (stationary, translatory motion, and flickering) and subsequently assessed the chicks' preference for their imprinting stimulus in a dual-choice test, contrasting it with a novel stimulus.

Methods

Subjects

We used 272 White Leghorn chicks of the Ross 308 strain (200 in Experiment 1 and 72 in Experiment 2). Eggs were obtained from a local hatchery (Azienda Agricola Crescenti, Brescia) and incubated in our laboratory at the University of Trento at 37.7 °C and 40% humidity. To prevent tactile and visual experiences for the chicks, hatching occurred in individual black boxes located within a darkened hatching chamber maintained at 37.7 °C and 60% humidity. All procedures received approval from the Ethical Committee of the University of Trento and the Italian Ministry of Health (permit number 324/2022-PR).

Apparatus

The experimental apparatus consisted of a rectangular arena (60 cm × 90 cm × 90 cm; see Fig. 1a) equipped with high-frequency screens positioned on the shortest walls and used to display stimuli (for additional details, refer to Zanon et al. 2021). The animal behaviors were recorded using a Microsoft LifeCam mounted 105 cm above the apparatus. Food and water were available ad libitum in the apparatus center. Nine apparatuses were used concurrently.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic representation of the apparatus (a), stimuli and procedures used in Experiment (b) 1 and 2 (c)

Stimuli and presentation

We employed 3D stimuli (a blue -hex: 2EBAFF- cube and a green -hex: 30B619- hourglass) previously used in a study investigating imprinting preferences over time (Lemaire et al. 2021). The stimuli were displayed as 2D images on high-frequency screens with a refresh rate of 120 Hz. Using 'ImprintSchedule', an open-source program (described in Zanon et al. 2021), we manipulated the methods used to display our stimuli. We utilized three distinct motion paradigms: flickering (with 9 levels tested in Experiment 1: 0.5, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 10, 20, and 40 Hz; and 2 levels tested in Experiment 2: 1 and 2 Hz), translatory motion (horizontally moving back and forth across the screen length in 9 s), or stationary (static image at the screen center) (see Fig. 1b, c). In the flickering stimulation type, stimuli were presented with equal durations of appearance and disappearance, ensuring a consistent timing pattern across all tested frequencies.

Procedure

In both experiments, immediately after hatching, chicks were briefly sexed by examining their feathers under dim light and then placed in the center of their individual apparatus where they remained for the entire duration of the experiments. Experiment 1 consisted of a 14-h exposure period during which chicks were exposed to the blue cube described earlier (imprinting phase). These 14 h were divided into 14 sessions of 59 min, interrupted by 1-min intervals of black screens (Fig. 1b). The appearance of the stimuli on the screens was counterbalanced across sessions. According to Lemaire et al. (2021), this procedure is known to induce strong and stable imprinting preferences with the blue cube used here.

Experiment 2 started with the same procedure as Experiment 1 (as shown in Fig. 1c), but the chicks remained in their apparatus for the entire night (10 h with empty black screens) to be tested the following day. During the test phase, which lasted 14 h, the chicks were exposed to their imprinting stimulus (blue cube) as well as a novel stimulus (green hourglass). The 14 h of testing were divided into 25 sessions of 30 min each, with each session interrupted by a 5-min black screen interval. Before the first test session, the chicks were gently relocated to the center of their apparatus to avoid any positional bias. As for the imprinting sessions, the appearance of the stimuli on the screens was counterbalanced across testing sessions.

Data analysis

In both experiments, we used DeepLabCut (Nath et al. 2019) to track the location of the chicks within the cage, and we assessed their preference for a stimulus by measuring the time spent close to it. An area within 30 cm from the screen was considered as a choice for the relative stimulus.

During the imprinting phase of both experiments, we analyzed the time spent in the closest zone to the stimulus (values ranging from 0 to 14 h) across different stimulation types (9 in the primary analysis of experiment 1 and 4 levels in the secondary analysis of experiment 1: [0.5, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 10, 20, 40 Hz] and [stationary, translatory motion, 1 and 2 Hz] respectively); 4 levels in experiment 2: stationary, translatory motion, 1 and 2 Hz) and sex (2 levels). The data was log transformed to meet parametric assumptions. In case the assumptions were met, we performed analyses of variance (ANOVA), followed by t-tests to investigate the significant effects. When the parametric assumptions were not met (even after data transformation), we performed a permutation test using the aovp function (lmPerm package) as a non-parametric equivalent of the ANOVA followed by t-tests to investigate the significant effects.

During the test phase of experiment 2, we analyzed the chicks’ preference for the imprinting stimulus compared to a novel stimulus across different stimulation types (4 levels: stationary, translatory motion, and 2 different flickering frequencies [1 and 2 Hz]) and sex (2 levels). We calculated the preference for the imprinting stimulus (PI) using the following formula:

$${\text{PI }} = t_{{{\text{imprinting}} \,{\text{stimulus}}}} /t_{{{\text{total}}}} \times 100$$

where timprinting stimulus is the time spent close to the imprinting stimulus and ttotal is the sum of time spent close to both stimuli.

A score of 50% indicates no preference, a score above 50% indicates a preference for the imprinting stimulus and a score below 50% indicates a preference for the unfamiliar stimulus. As the parametric assumptions were not met, we performed a non-parametric analysis for longitudinal data (nparLD package) that included the different stimulation types, sex and time (25 test sessions) as factors. Finally, for the overall and longitudinal analyses, we performed t-tests to investigate the chicks' preference for the imprinting stimulus against the chance level (index of preference at 50%) and between groups. In addition, we performed Pearson tests to investigate whether the time spent by the chicks close to their imprinting stimulus during the imprinting phase was correlated with the chicks’ preference for their imprinting stimulus during the overall and first session of the testing phase.

Results

Experiment 1

The data are shown in Fig. 2. The analysis revealed a significant effect of frequency (F(8,153) = 12.64, p < 0.001), but no effect of sex (F(1,153) = 3.14, p = 0.08) and no interaction effect (F(8,153) = 1.11, p = 0.36). Chicks spent more time near the stimulus with lower frequencies (0.5, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 Hz) compared to higher frequencies (10, 20, 40 Hz; see supplementary Table S1 for pairwise comparisons and descriptive statistics). Among the lower frequencies, 2 and 4 Hz seemed most effective in attracting chicks to the stimulus (2 Hz: mean = 11.56 h, 95% CI = 10.20–12.92; 4 Hz: mean = 11.78 h, 95% CI = 10.45–13.11), while 0.5, 1, 3, and 5 Hz elicited slightly lower responses (0.5 Hz: mean = 11.11, 95% CI = 10.09–12.13; 1 Hz: mean = 10.24, 95% CI = 8.81–11.68; 3 Hz: mean = 11.17, 95% CI = 10.19–12.15; 5 Hz: mean = 10.09, 95% CI = 8.45–11.74). Among the higher frequencies, 10 Hz (mean = 7.63, 95%CI = 5.82–9.43) was more effective than 20 and 40 Hz (20 Hz: mean = 6.71, 95% CI = 5.34–8.10; 40 Hz: mean = 5.61, 95% CI = 4.35–6.87), but none of the pair comparisons reaches significance (see Table S1 for descriptive statistics).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Time spent close to the imprinting stimulus across the 14 imprinting hours, displayed at different frequencies (statistics are reported in Table S1). Data points represent the individual scores of males in purple and females in orange

Flickering stimuli have not often been used to imprint chicks on virtual stimuli in the laboratory. To further explore the effectiveness of the lower flickering frequencies, we selected two of the flickering frequencies producing strong attraction responses, and compared them with two common stimulation types used in laboratory settings: translatory motion and stationary display. Among the lowest frequencies, we selected 2 Hz, which visually elicited one of the strongest responses with the smallest interquartile range and whiskers and 1 Hz which visually elicited one of the smallest responses with the largest interquartile range and whiskers. The results are shown in Fig. 3. Our ANOVA revealed no significant effect of stimulation type (F(3,67) = 0.91, p = 0.44), sex (F(1,67) = 1.34, p = 0.24), or interaction (F(3,67) = 2.28, p = 0.087). Static (mean = 10.69, 95% CI = 9.43–11.95) and translatory (mean = 10.38, 95% CI = 8.96–11.81) stimuli showed similar results to the flickering ones (1 Hz: mean = 10.24, 95% CI = 8.81–11.68; 2 Hz: mean = 11.56, 95% CI = 10.20–12.92).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Time spent close to the imprinting stimulus displayed at 1, 2 Hz, in a translatory motion or being static in the screen center. Data points represent the individual scores of males in purple and females in orange

Experiment 2

In the second experiment, we examined whether similar levels of attractiveness would result in the development of different filial imprinting preferences and memory. We replicated the imprinting phase of Experiment 1 including two low flickering frequencies (1 and 2 Hz) translatory and static stimulations. Following imprinting, we then tested the chicks' preference for their imprinting stimulus in a repeated dual-choice task with a novel stimulus, following the procedure of Lemaire et al. (2021).

Imprinting phase

During the imprinting phase, when chicks were exposed to only one stimulus, we found no significant differences between the stimulation types (flickering 1 Hz, flickering 2 Hz, translatory motion, and stationary; F(3,64) = 0.52, p = 0.67), sex (F(1, 64) = 0.01, p = 0.92), or interactions (F(3,64) = 0.36, p = 0.78). The data is shown in Fig. 4. All chicks showed similar attraction levels to the displayed stimulus (static: mean = 11.85, 95% CI = 11.02–12.67; translatory: mean = 11.32, 95% CI = 10.56–12.07; 1 Hz: mean = 11.47, 95% CI = 10.42–12.53; 2 Hz: mean = 11.56, 95% CI = 10.51–12.60), regardless of the stimulation type, replicating the results from the first experiment. Interestingly, the more nuanced response observed at 1 Hz was no longer visually perceived, suggesting that the distribution observed within the lowest frequencies in the first experiment was likely due to chance induced by the relatively small sample size.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Time spent close to the imprinting stimulus displayed at 1 Hz, 2 Hz, in a translatory motion or being static in the screen center. Data points represent the individual scores of males in purple and females in orange

Test phase

During the repeated exposure (25 sessions of 30 min) to the imprinting vs. unfamiliar stimulus, we found a significant effect of stimulation type (statistic(2.57) = 4.26, p < 0.01) but no effect of session (statistic(12.82) = 1.41, p = 0.15), sex (statistic(1) = 1.12, p = 0.29), or interactions (see statistics for interactions). The data is shown in Fig. 5. Overall, all stimulation types showed a significant preference for their imprinting stimulus (static: t(17) = 3.73, p value < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.88, mean = 64.92, 95% CI = 56.49–73.36; translatory: t(17) = 5.86, p value  < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.38, mean = 67.97, 95% CI = 61.50–74.44; flickering [1 Hz]: t(17) = 2.34, p value  < 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.55, mean = 55.60, 95% CI = 50.54–60.66; flickering [2 Hz]: t(17) = 3.94, p value  < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.93, mean = 59.89, 95% CI = 54.57–65.19). However, the preference strength varied between stimulation types. Chicks exposed to translatory stimuli had a stronger preference for their imprinting stimulus than chicks exposed to flickering stimuli (translatory vs. flickering [1 Hz]: t(32.13) = 3.18, p value  < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 1.06; translatory vs. flickering [2 Hz]: t(32.73) = 2.04, p value  < 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.68) but not than chicks exposed to static stimuli (t(31.86) = 0.61, p value  = 0.55, Cohen’s d = 0.20). Chicks exposed to static stimuli tended to have a stronger preference for their imprinting stimulus compared to chicks exposed to 1 Hz (t(27.83) = 2.0, p value = 0.055, Cohen’s d = 0.67) but not 2 Hz flickering stimuli (t(28.61) = 1.07, p value = 0.30, Cohen’s d = 0.36). Differences in preference between chicks exposed to 1 or 2 Hz flickering stimuli were not significant (t(33.93) = − 1.23, p value  = 0.23, Cohen’s d = 0.41).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Chicks’ preference for their imprinting stimulus across sessions (b) and overall (a) between different stimulation types. In a, data points represent individual scores of males in purple and females in orange and stars represent significance levels (p < 0.05, *; p < 0.01, **; p < 0.001, ***). In b, error bars represent SEM

Although the non-parametric longitudinal analysis did not reveal any interaction between session and stimulation types (due to the large number of testing sessions and an overall preference for chicks’ imprinting stimulus), it is worth noting that the initial preference (see session 1) visually appeared different between stimulation types. Indeed, in the first testing session (first 30 min after the night period following imprinting), chicks exposed to translatory stimuli seemed to have an initial preference for the imprinting stimulus (mean = 68.07, 95% CI = 46.80–89.34). Intriguingly, this initial preference was absent when chicks were exposed to static stimuli (mean = 49.57, 95% CI = 27.31–71.83) and seemed to reverse when exposed to flickering stimuli (1 Hz: mean = 30.84, 95% CI = 11.32–50.37; 2 Hz: mean = 34.38, 95% CI = 15.62–53.13). However, none of these visual tendencies reached significance, likely due to the low sample size for a non-repeated measurement design (static: t(17) = − 0.040, p value  = 0.97, Cohen’s d = 0.0096; translatory: t(17) = 1.079, p value  = 0.09, Cohen’s d = 0.42; flickering [1 Hz]: t(17) = − 2.07, p value  = 0.054, Cohen’s d = 0.49; flickering [2 Hz]: t(17) = − 1.76, p value  = 0.097, Cohen’s d = 0.41).

Importantly, the observed preference index during the overall testing period and the first testing session did not show a significant correlation with the time spent by the chicks close to their imprinting stimulus during imprinting. Specifically, Pearson's test on data from the overall testing period yielded r = 0.08, with a p value of 0.52. Likewise, Pearson's test on data from the first testing session yielded r = 0.01, with a p value of 0.96.

Discussion

This study aimed to assess the effectiveness of different methods, used to display stimuli on video screens, in capturing the attention of chicks in an imprinting context. We examined various flickering frequencies to determine the most effective ones in eliciting approach and maintaining chicks’ attention, and compared them to static and moving stimuli commonly used in laboratory settings.

Our findings align with Simner's experiment (1973), which demonstrated that flickering lights with frequencies ranging from 0.5 to 4 Hz were more effective in capturing chicks' attention compared to higher frequencies of 12 and 24 Hz. Consistent with Simner's hypothesis, we observed that chicks exposed to a stimulus flickering between 0.5 and 5 Hz showed higher attraction as they spent significantly more time near it compared to chicks exposed to the same stimulus flickering at 10, 20, or 40 Hz. The underlying reasons for this early attraction to lower flickering frequencies remain to be further investigated, with theories suggesting a link to systemic rhythms present during hatching in chicks (Gottlieb 1968; Simner and Kaplan 1977).

Interestingly, our results suggest that chicks exhibit a similar level of attraction toward a stimulus flickering at low frequency compared to a stimulus that is either moving or static on the screen. While the idea proposed by Menner (Pumphrey 1948) and James (1959, 1960), stating that flickering should be as attractive as moving stimuli, is supported by our initial experiment, the absence of a difference in attractiveness between flickering and moving stimuli compared to static stimuli is surprising and not in line with the existing literature (Ten Cate 1986, 1989) nor with Lorenz's original observation (1935). One possible explanation could be the salience of the stimulus used in our experiment, as we employed a conspicuous and effective blue stimulus known to elicit strong imprinting responses. It is plausible that small differences in attractiveness may not have been discernible solely through the chicks' approach and following behaviors. Another possibility is that the sample size used in our initial experiment was insufficient to demonstrate significant differences for some of the lowest frequencies tested. Our second experiment supplemented our investigation by examining chicks' ability to discriminate between their imprinting stimulus and a novel stimulus after one day of imprinting, thereby providing a more accurate representation of imprinting development and memory.

First, it should be noted that the time chicks spent near their imprinting stimulus during the imprinting phase of this second experiment was not a strong predictor of their preference index, both during the overall testing period and the first testing session. The absence of correlation enabled us to dissociate the two measures. In terms of imprinting preferences, our overall results indicated no difference between chicks exposed to translatory motion and those exposed to static stimuli. Both methods yielded a similar preference and imprinting strength for the imprinting stimulus, replicating findings from Lemaire et al. (2021). However, distinctions emerged when chicks were imprinted with a flickering stimulus. Both frequencies exhibited a lower preference for their imprinting stimulus compared to translatory and to a lower extent to static stimulations. While the absence of a difference in imprinting preference between chicks imprinted with moving or static stimuli requires further investigation, our results indicate that imprinting with flickering frequencies does not generate the same preference observed in chicks imprinted with moving stimuli. Additionally, not all low frequencies were equally effective in eliciting imprinting and establishing a strong memory. Further experiments with larger sample sizes are warranted to identify the most efficient lowest frequencies (ranging from 0.5 to 5 Hz) in eliciting and maintaining imprinting memory, as well as to explore the slight temporal differences that were visually observed in the time profile, but that could not be statistically analyzed (see Fig. 5b).

In conclusion, our investigation provides valuable insights into the types of stimulation that can be employed in studies making use of imprinting in virtual environments. While we demonstrated that low flickering frequencies (0.5–5 Hz) were as effective as translatory and static stimulation in capturing and maintaining chicks' attention, we also found that some of the lowest frequencies were less effective in eliciting and building a strong imprinting memory. Conversely, high frequencies (10–40 Hz) were not effective in capturing and sustaining chicks' attention. Further investigations are needed to explore whether frequencies higher than 40 Hz could match the attractiveness levels of the lowest frequencies and to understand the development of imprinting memory with high frequencies. It is worth noting that our current experimental setup limited us to a maximum frequency of 40 Hz due to computational stability constraints. However, our results imply that high frequencies are less attractive compared to lower ones. Moreover, while our focus was primarily on determining optimal flicker rates compared to a single motion type and speed, it would be interesting to extend similar procedures to test different motion types and velocities that elicit imprinting and examine their influence on the development of imprinting memory. These future studies would provide a more comprehensive understanding of the effects of various stimuli on imprinting and its cognitive and social aspects in neonatal animals. Overall, our work presents a standardized procedure for investigating imprinting and leveraging it to explore cognition and various aspects of social behavior in neonatal animals. By uncovering the effectiveness of different stimuli in capturing and maintaining chicks' attention, our study contributes to the growing body of knowledge on animal behavior and offers valuable insights for designing experiments in virtual environments.