Introduction

Climate change models predict a warmer climate and greater variability in weather components, such as temperature or rainfall, across the world in the coming decades. Our knowledge of the ecological responses to climate change in different organisms is still developing (Dunn et al. 2010; du Plessis et al. 2012; Halupka and Halupka 2017). Understanding the responses of animals to changing environments is essential to predicting the extent to which populations and species will adapt to a rapidly changing world. Birds provide an excellent model system to study such impacts on life-history traits. Avian incubation, as a vital component of parental care heavily dependent on thermal conditions, provides a particularly suitable system in which trade-offs between investment in egg heating and adult self-maintenance may be studied (Deeming 2002; Lundblad and Conway 2021) in the context of changing temperature conditions.

Ambient temperature influences energy expenditure during incubation, because at lower ambient temperatures, incubation efficiency must be increased to keep eggs within temperature ranges suitable for successful embryo development. Females may respond to this limitation by optimizing their incubation rhythm (Conway and Martin 2000a, b). The incubation rhythm consists of alternating periods where females leave the clutch to obtain food (off-bouts) and return to re-warm the eggs (on-bouts; Deeming 2002). Egg temperature at the incubation stage and its variation can influence embryonic development and affect numerous offspring fitness-related traits (reviewed in DuRant et al. 2013a). In particular, ambient temperature is commonly expected to influence avian incubation behaviour (Conway and Martin 2000a, b). However, despite the critical role that incubation plays in avian reproduction, few experimental studies have examined the relationship between temperature in the nest and incubation behaviour in natural systems and relatively similar manipulations resulted in opposite results (Bryan and Bryant 1999; Nilsson et al. 2008; Pérez et al. 2008; Ardia et al. 2009; Ardia et al. 2010; Álvarez and Barba 2014; Coe et al. 2015; Vaugoyeau et al. 2017). Virtually, all previous attempts did that by directly applying thermal manipulations to the eggs, thus creating largely unnatural conditions that factor out female behaviour—which can be an important modulator of the in-nest thermal conditions. Moreover, when it comes to the impact of increasing temperature in relation to the climate change, most previous experiments used unrealistic temperature increases, not corresponding to projected climate scenarios. Climate models predict increases in mean temperature (between 1.5 and 3 °C in the twenty-first century; Meehl et al. 2000; Pachauri et al. 2014; IPCC 2020) across most of the lands—a degree of heating rarely applied as thermal manipulation in bird incubation studies. Therefore, we need more information on how biologically realistic increases in ambient temperature (and consequently—changes to the overall nest thermal microclimate) could affect the incubation behaviour of females, with cascading effects on the offspring.

Impacts of thermal conditions during incubation on offspring traits are well documented. Changes in embryonic temperature may alter developmental trajectories (Olson et al. 2008) to ultimately constrain or improve offspring fitness-related traits (DuRant et al. 2013a). Indeed, some studies showed that temperature during early embryonic development at the incubation stage has potential to affect the physiological maturation, structural size and immunocompetence (Hepp et al. 2006; Hopkins et al. 2011; Nord and Nilsson 2011; DuRant et al. 2013b; Griffith et al. 2016; Vaugoyeau et al. 2017; Andreasson et al. 2018). Such alterations may in turn affect condition and survival of offspring (Berntsen and Bech 2016). However, experimental studies that simultaneously investigate female incubation behaviour and offspring fitness-related traits with respect to external thermal conditions are scarce in songbirds (Pérez et al. 2008; Ardia et al. 2009; Álvarez and Barba 2014; Coe et al. 2015; Vaugoyeau et al. 2017).

Here, we manipulated the developmental conditions of bird embryos by experimentally changing the inner nest box temperature during incubation to investigate the effects of in-nest pre-hatching temperature changes on female incubation behaviour and offspring fitness-related traits. We chose this form of thermal manipulation instead of heating the eggs to create more realistic manipulated conditions that factor in female behaviour and better reflect impacts of ambient temperature increases caused by climate warming. The study was performed in a wild population of the Collared Flycatcher Ficedula albicollis. Hole-nesting birds, readily breeding in provided nest boxes, offer a perfect system for studying incubation-related patterns and processes. We predicted that temperature in the nest box would explain variation in female incubation behaviour. Specifically, we predicted that females should decrease incubation effort in heated nests but not in control ones. Moreover, we sought to determine whether elevated temperature in the nest during incubation influences fledglings’ body mass and tarsus length. Embryos developing in higher ambient temperature in the nest can expend less energy and potentially hatch with more energy reserves (Larsen et al. 2003). Therefore, we expected that nestlings hatched in heated nests would be heavier and have larger structural size at fledging.

Methods

Study  area

The field study was carried out during two breeding seasons in years 2018 and 2019. Study plots with nest boxes were located in the southern part of the island of Gotland, Sweden (57°03′  N, 18°17′  E). Collard flycatchers breed there in standard wooden nest boxes (29 × 10 × 10 cm, 3.2‐cm diameter entrance hole) placed on tree at approximately 1.3 m above ground level in a grid of 30–50 m between adjacent boxes. Collared flycatchers on Gotland usually lay one clutch per year, consisting of six eggs on average (range 4–8 eggs) that hatch in the beginning of June. Females lay one egg per day and the incubation is expected to start at the day when the female has laid her final egg (Koski et al. 2020). The first eggs are laid in the first half of May and incubation is undertaken solely by females. Nestlings hatch after approximately 14 days of incubation and remain in the nests for additional 14–16 days, fed by both parents. They reach the maximum body mass at the age of 10–11 days and lose some mass prior to fledging. After fledging, young collared flycatchers stay close to the nest for 2 weeks and are still fed by parents.

During each breeding season (May–June), all breeding attempts were regularly monitored. Regular nest box checks (every 4 days) from the beginning of May established the date of clutch initiation. From the fifth egg onwards (assuming one egg was laid per day), the checks were continued every other day to determine clutch size. Nests where incubation had already started (day of incubation start = day 0 of incubation) were visited every third day to perform experimental procedures. After hatching (hatching day = day 0 of nestling life), nests were visited on days 2nd and 12th to obtain nestling measurements.

As many adult birds as possible were caught using clip-traps or mist-nets, and (if not already ringed) marked with a numbered aluminium ring. The females were caught, while incubating (at least the 6th day of incubation), while males were caught while feeding the nestlings between day 6 and 12 after hatching. Birds were aged based on a distinctive moult limit visible within the greater wing coverts, wear of primaries and the colouration of the inside of upper mandible, and in most cases, their age was confirmed based on the past ringing data. Sex was determined based on plumage characteristics (Svenson 1992). Each year, basic nestling measurements were recorded: body mass on days 2 and 12, tarsus length on day 12 and the number of successfully fledged offspring. The number of successfully fledge offspring was assessed by checking for any dead unfledged individuals between days 16 and 18 after hatching. Blood samples (ca. 20 μl) were collected from all nestlings on day 2 and stored in 96% ethanol for further genetic analyses. DNA was extracted from blood samples with QIAamp DNA Blood Mini Kit according to a standard protocol. Sexing of nestlings was performed via PCR using the P2-P8 primers (Griffiths et al. 1998) that amplify the fragments of sex-specific CHD locus.

Experimental procedure

A total of 40 nests (20 heated and 20 control) of the Collared Flycatcher were used in each of breeding season. On the first day of incubation (we recognized incubation when eggs were warm), half of randomly selected nests were assigned to the heated group (experimental nests) and the other half remained unmanipulated (control nests). We tried to create dyads of nests with the same laying date and similar clutch size (± 1 egg). Experimental nests were supplied with active heat-packs (Aqua pack—heat-pack 72 h, size 15.5 × 11.0 cm, producing heat through the oxidation of iron powder, HEATPACK.de). We fixed the heat-packs to the inner side of a nest box roof, thus suspending it approx. 20 cm above the flycatcher nest. Inactive (exhausted) heat-packs were installed in control nest boxes in the same way. Heated nests were visited every 72 h to change the heat-packs; control nests were visited with the same frequency. To remove additional sources of variation, in both experimental and control nests, we standardized the height of the nest (to ~ 12 cm) by modifying the amount of nesting material in the lower part of the nest. We manipulated the temperature inside the nest box throughout the whole incubation period. Temperatures in nests-boxes were monitored in both experimental and control nests to make sure that thermal manipulation was effective. To achieve this, nest boxes were fitted with temperature sensors (Thermochron iButton DS1923; 6 × 17 mm, temperature range: − 20 to 85 °C; resolution 0.0625 °C) attached to the wall of the nest box approximately 5 cm above the nest cup. Temperature has been recorded every 15 min during the experimental period in the control and experimental boxes.

Incubation behaviour analysis

We collected data on incubation behaviour only in 2019. In total, incubation rhythm was followed in 21 females: 11 from control nests and 10 from heated nests. On day 6 of the incubation period, temperature loggers (Thermochron iButtons) were placed in nests among the eggs. The technique based on the iButton temperature logger provided reliable and accurate measurement of female incubation behaviour in previous studies (Amininasab et al. 2016; Bueno-Enciso et al. 2017). The female nest attentiveness data obtained using the camera were highly correlated with the data obtained using temperature logger data (see details in Amininasab et al. 2016; Bueno-Enciso et al. 2017). The loggers stayed there for the whole following day and were removed on the subsequent day. To analyse incubation behaviour, we used records from only one day (i.e., day 7 of incubation). The loggers were set to record temperature in 2-min intervals. Such interval is suitable to assess female incubation behaviour based on fluctuations of temperature measured in the nest cup (Capilla-Lasheras 2018). Due to a limited number of loggers, we were not able to get data on longer periods of incubation. We did not find the loggers to be removed from nests by females. To get data on incubation behaviour for each female, we applied the ‘incR’ package implemented in R, version 4.0.3 (Capilla-Lasheras 2018; R Core Team 2020). The package allows to automatically extract data on presence or absence of an incubating female (so-called on-bouts and off-bouts) based on the nest and ambient temperatures at every time point along an incubation time series. As we had a limited number of loggers, we could not record ambient temperatures using loggers. Therefore, we used data from a nearby weather station (Visby Airport Station) where temperature was recorded every 20 min (https://www.wunderground.com/history/daily/se/visby/ESSV/date/2019-4-28). Nevertheless, we were able to perform the analysis, because incubation scoring is performed based on averaged (per hour) ambient temperature (Capilla-Lasheras 2018). Ambient temperature was much lower than incubation temperature and they did not overlap with each other, excluding the problem with larger measurement intervals of ambient temperatures. Before running automated scoring of incubation behaviour using the ‘incR’ package, we had set several user-defined parameters. First, we fixed parameters lower.time and upper.time [which determine a time window when a female is assumed to be always incubating (i.e. for diurnal birds the window is in the night)] at 2200 and 0300, respectively. We had also fixed the parameter temp.diff.threshold that controls for a difference which is allowed between nest and ambient temperature at 4 °C. The sensitivity parameter, that allows to reduce the off-bout threshold when incubation temperature is close to ambient temperature, was fixed at 0.25. The maxNightVariation parameter, which determines the maximum variation allowed in the lower.time–upper.time window and controls for big drops in nest temperature during that temporal window, was set to 2 °C. All parameters were set up based on recommendations validated by Capilla-Lasheras (2018). We generated four variables that were in the focus of our interest, i.e., egg incubation temperature, percentage time spent in the nest by incubating females, and the number and averaged duration of off-bouts. All those variables referred to a period between 3 a.m. and 10 p.m.

Statistical analyses

All analyses were performed in the R environment, ver. 4.0.3 (R Core Team 2020). To compare control and heated nests in respect to egg incubation temperature, percentage time spent by incubating females in the nest, the number and averaged duration of off-bouts made by incubating females, we performed independent t tests. To examine differences in hatching success (expressed as the proportion unhatched eggs in relation to all laid eggs in a brood) between control and heated nests, we fitted a GLM (generalized linear model) with treatment and year as fixed factors, and a logit-link function. To analyse body mass on day 2 and 12 after hatching and tarsus length on day 12 in the offspring, we fitted linear mixed models (LMMs) using the ‘lme4’ package, ver. 1.1.25 (Bates et al. 2015). To analyse offspring survival in period from hatching day to day 12 after hatching we fitted a generalised linear mixed model (GLMM) with binomial error variance and logit-link function using the same ‘lme4’ package (Bates et al. 2015). All initial models included the treatment (control vs. heated nests), offspring sex (female vs. male) and year (2018 vs. 2019) as categorical factors, whereas hatching date, brood size, tarsus length (only in the model analysing body mass on day 12) and body mass on day 2 after hatching (only in the model analysing offspring survival) were included as continuous covariates. Female identity was introduced to models as a random factor. To reduce initial models, we removed non-significant covariates (if P > 0.10), starting from the least significant term. To test fixed effects in LMMs, we used F-tests with the degrees of freedom approximated by the Kenward–Roger method, by applying the ‘pbkrtest’ package, ver. 0.4.8, and ‘lmerTest’ package, ver. 3.1.3 (Halekoh and Hojsgaard 2014; Kuznetsova et al. 2017). The least square means with SEs for categorical variables were estimated from the final models using the ‘emmeans’ package, ver. 1.5.2 (Lenth 2020). In case of the GLMM, fixed effects were examined using the Wald χ2 test. To visualise differences in means between control and heated nests we employed the ‘ggplot2’ package, ver. 3.3.2 (Wickham 2016). In all analyses with Gaussian error variance, we visually checked for normality and homogeneity of residuals. If necessary, the dependent variable was log-transformed and the information on that is included in “Results”. All statistical tests were two-tailed with the significance level set at P < 0.05. There are differences in sample sizes among analyses because of nest abandonment or missing measurements. We provide means ± SE in the “Results” section; in case of transformed-dependent variables, back-transformed values on original scale are presented.

Results

Below, every time we report on results concerning temperature measurements, we refer to the following temperature parameters named in a consistent way: ambient temperature—measured by the weather station in Visby; nest box temperature or ambient temperature in the nest—measured with an iButton placed on the nest box wall; incubation temperature—measured in the nest cup during incubation.

Pre-treatment effects

Females assigned to control and experimental groups did not differ in body mass measured at the 6th day of incubation (linear model controlling for year effect; treatment: F1, 55 = 1.02, P = 0.32, year: F1, 55 = 2.59, P = 0.11). Control and experimental broods also did not differ in laying date (linear model controlling for year effect; treatment: F1, 76 = 0.00, P = 0.96, year: F1, 76 = 74.48, P < 0.001) and clutch size (liner model controlling for year effect; treatment: F1, 76 = 0.01, P = 0.92, year: F1, 76 = 8.86, P = 0.004).

Effect of experimental procedure on nest box temperature

Our manipulation of the level of temperature in the nest was successful; temperature measured inside the nest box was higher in heated compared to control nests by 2.5 °C (linear model controlling for year effect; treatment: F1, 59 = 28.27, P < 0.001, year: F1, 59 = 2.71, P = 0.10; mean ± SE for heated vs. control nests: 17.4 ± 0.3 and 14.9 ± 0.3 °C). Differences in temperature detected inside nest boxes between heated and control nests were observed in both breeding seasons, and temperature detected outside the nest boxes was always lower compared to temperatures observed inside the nest boxes (Fig. 1). Furthermore, the analysis of maximum temperatures noted inside the nest boxes showed similar pattern; maximum temperatures in heated nest boxes were also higher than in control nest boxes (linear model controlling for year effect; treatment: F1, 59 = 11.11, P = 0.001, year: F1, 59 = 9.27, P = 0.003; mean ± SE for heated vs. control nests: 22.2 ± 0.5 and 19.9 ± 0.5 °C).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Differences in temperature detected inside nest boxes between heated and control nests and temperature detected outside the nest boxes in 2018 and 2019. Sample sizes are presented above bars

Effects of treatment on female incubation behaviour

Elevated temperature in the nest box did not affect egg incubation temperature [log-transformed variable, t19 = − 0.38, P = 0.71; control vs. heated nests: 34.8 ± 0.3 °C (range 32.6–35.4) and 34.9 ± 0.3 °C (range 33.6–35.9)]. However, we found that incubating females spent more time in control nests compared to heated nests [log-transformed variable, t19 = − 2.93, P = 0.009; control vs. heated nests: 84.8 ± 1.7% (range 78.3–96.7) and 79.8 ± 0.8% (range 75.7–84.3); Fig. 2]. Moreover, females from heated nests had more off-bouts during incubation than females from control nests (t19 = − 2.56, P = 0.019; control vs. heated nests: 37.2 ± 3.8 (range 10–51) and 50.9 ± 3.8 (range 32–71); Fig. 3). The mean duration of off-bout did not differ between females that incubated eggs in control and heated nests [t19 = − 0.58, P = 0.57; control vs. heated nests: 5.6 ± 0.2 min. (range 4.7–6.7) and 5.9 ± 0.3 min. (range 4.8–8.0)].

Fig. 2
figure 2

The effect of manipulation of nest temperature on time spent in the nest by incubating females. Back-transformed (from logarithm) means with SEs are shown. Sample sizes are presented above bars

Fig. 3
figure 3

The effect of manipulation of nest temperature on the number of off-bouts made by incubating females. Means with SEs are shown. Sample sizes are presented above bars

Effects of treatment on hatching success

The manipulation of temperature inside the nest boxes during incubation period did not affect hatching success as indicated by lack of differences in the proportion of unhatched eggs among control and experimental nests (treatment: F1, 61 = 0.87, P = 0.36, year: F1, 61 = 0.00, P = 0.98; control vs. heated nests: 0.16 ± 0.04 vs. 0.11 ± 0.04).

Effects of treatment on offspring body mass and structural size

Body mass of nestlings on day 2 after hatching was not affected by the treatment, offspring sex, year, hatching date and brood size (Table 1). In contrast, we found that nestling body mass measured on day 12 after hatching was significantly affected by manipulation of nest temperature. Offspring reared in heated nest were heavier compared to offspring reared in control nests (Table 1, Fig. 4). Nestling tarsus length on day 12 after hatching was not influenced by the treatment, sex and year (Table 1).

Table 1 Results of a linear mixed models analysing effects of manipulation of nest box temperature during the incubation period on nestling body mass (day 2 and 12) and tarsus length (day 12)
Fig. 4
figure 4

The effect of manipulation of nest temperature on body mass of 12-day-old nestlings. Least square means with SEs are shown. Sample sizes are presented above bars

Effects of treatment on offspring survival

Offspring survival until day 12 after hatching was not affected by heating treatment (Wald \(\chi_{1}^{2}\) = 0.00, P = 0.99; N = 265), the year of study (Wald \(\chi_{1}^{2}\)  = 0.81, P = 0.37), brood size (Wald \(\chi_{1}^{2}\) = 0.11, P = 0.074, statistics at dropping from model), and hatching date (Wald \(\chi_{1}^{2}\) = 0.27, P = 0.60, statistics at dropping from model). However, we found that male nestlings survived better than female nestlings (Wald \(\chi_{1}^{2}\) = 5.92, P < 0.015; the proportion of survived males and females: 0.93 ± 0.02 vs. 0.63 ± 0.04), and nestling body mass on day 2 after hatching positively predicted the probability of offspring survival (Wald \(\chi_{1}^{2}\) = 8.47, P < 0.004).

Discussion

We demonstrated that ambient temperature in the nest during incubation had a significant effect on both female incubation behaviour and offspring performance in the collared flycatcher. Specifically, in line with our prediction, we found that females from experimentally heated nests spent less time in the nest and had more off-bouts during incubation than females from control nests. Simultaneously, we showed that females from both control and experimental nests spent similar time on off-bouts, which could be easily explained by the fact that off-bout duration may be constrained to minimize rewarming eggs costs (Biebach 1986; Reid et al. 2000).

Similar to the previous studies, we showed that behaviour of incubating females and ultimately their incubation effort are particularly sensitive to nest microclimate (Conway and Martin 2000a, b; Cresswell et al. 2003; Olson et al. 2008, van de Ven et al. 2020, Lundblad and Conway 2021). Several studies examined the relationship between ambient temperature and incubation behaviour in birds, and the one frequent finding is that the lower temperatures caused females to spend less time off the nest per bout (Conway and Martin, 2000a, b; Ardia et al. 2010; Boulton et al. 2010; Amininasab et al. 2016). Moreover, a few studies have shown that higher ambient temperatures can be associated with reduced incubation period (Ardia et al. 2006) and increased off-bout duration (Conway and Martin 2000a, b; Camfield and Martin 2009; Boulton et al. 2010). In contrast, Pérez et al. (2008) experimentally heated eggs of the tree swallow (Tachycineta bicolor) during incubation by an average of 6.9 °C and showed that in boxes with elevated temperature females increased time spent in the nest during incubation. Nevertheless, it is difficult to explain these mixed results, since they used drastically different thermal regimes.

In our study, an increase in temperature within a nest box by 2.5 °C on average reduced the time spent in the nest by incubating females. Moreover, this change in incubation behaviour may be attributed to reduced energetic demands exerted on females due to relatively less heat being transferred to eggs, allowing them to forage for longer periods while leaving the nest unattended (Conway and Martin 2000a, b; Griffith et al. 2016). Reduced energy expenditure during incubation, and increased foraging time during the incubation period may positively affect parental condition, which in turns enables them to forage more effectively later while feeding nestlings, leading to improved nestling body condition (Griffith et al. 2016). Indeed, we showed that offspring from experimentally heated nests had larger body mass on day 12 in comparison to the control ones. Body mass at fledging was proposed as a reliable index of the survival probability for avian offspring (e.g., Magrath 1991). Also, in the study population of collared flycatchers, higher body mass at fledgling seem to be the most important trait affecting juvenile survival (Linden et al. 1992). Note that body mass at the day 12 were affected by many factors such brood size, hatching date and tarsus length. The results are in line with previous studies, which showed that these factors may explained the variation in body mass at the day 12 in these species (e.g., Linden et al. 1992; Merilä 1996). However, we did not show that our experimental manipulation affects offspring survival. We only found that female offspring experienced higher mortality than male offspring until day 12 after hatching. This result suggests that male and female nestlings differ in their sensitivity to some environmental or social conditions experienced in the nest during development. Moreover, we showed that offspring survival was correlated with the body mass at hatching: heavier nestlings experienced lower mortality at pre-fledgling period which indicates that larger size at hatching gives advantage in chances to survive up to leaving the nest.

Our results may suggest that higher temperature in the nest reduces an energetic cost of females during incubation and leads to re-allocation of resources into the post-hatching period. Unfortunately, our study design did not allow us to separate the effect of the heating treatment on eggs from the effects of being raised by a heated female, as cross-fostering procedure was not applied. However, we did not observe the elevated temperature in the nest box to affect egg incubation temperature. Moreover, since we did not observe that offspring from the experimentally heated nests had larger body mass at hatching in comparison to the control ones, the observed differences in body mass between control and experimental fledglings could be related to female behaviour. Indeed, previous work on birds have also shown that changes in energetic investment during one life-history stage can have carry-over effects on subsequent life-history stages within reproductive bouts (Heaney and Monaghan 1996; Pérez et al. 2008). Specifically, study on the tree swallow revealed that such an effect is probably due to the behaviour of heated females, as heated females provisioned their nestlings at a greater rate than did control females (Pérez et al. 2008).

In conclusion, given the obvious importance of temperature to embryo development and the female's response to experimentally altered temperature, the inconsistent relationships between ambient temperature and incubation behaviour in avian studies are still an enigma. Much more research is needed to understand how thermal environmental conditions affect incubation behaviour, and how their effects on fitness arise. Such insights will not only provide foundational information regarding avian evolution and ecology, but also contribute to avian conservation.