Introduction

Sociality influences survival and reproductive success (Davies et al. 2012) via modulating the costs and benefits associated with predation risk, access to food (Krause and Ruxton 2002), and information (Dall et al. 2005). Trade-offs between foraging behaviour and antipredator behaviour are well-documented (Verdolin 2006), and the strength of association has been shown to be modulated by social factors (e.g., group size, group composition), individual factors (e.g., age, sex), and environment (e.g., habitat structure, season; Widdig et al. 2001; Lehmann et al. 2007; Boucherie et al. 2016; Strandburg-Peshkin et al. 2017). Affiliative interactions, for instance, can be used to quantify social relationships in complex social systems (Wey et al. 2008). Intense social relationships are observed between reproductive partners as well as parents and offspring (Dunbar and Shultz 2010), but also between unrelated group members (Silk et al. 2006; Lehmann et al. 2007; Cameron et al. 2009; Palagi and Cordoni 2009; Boucherie et al. 2016).

Understanding behavioural variation associated with sex and age in relation to group-level attributes is particularly important for conservation management (Bengston and Jandt 2014; Estrada et al. 2017). Reproductive success may be influenced by the quality of long-term social bonds (Silk 2007) as well as by an individual’s social position within a group. Highly social female White-Faced Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus capucinus imitator) with many affiliative ties, for example, occupy a central social position and have increased offspring survival as compared to other females in the same group (Kalbitzer et al. 2017). Mortality risk may also be associated with an individual’s social position within a group (Kurvers et al. 2014). Yellow-Bellied Marmots (Marmota flaviventris) with few affiliative ties, for instance, tend to experience high mortality risk (Blumstein et al. 2009). In contrast, agonistic networks can generate information about the competitive abilities of individuals. Older adult Yellow-Bellied Marmots seem to initiate more and receive fewer agonistic interactions than younger adults (Wey and Blumstein 2010). This may increase male reproductive success, because females tend to prefer competitive males (Hirsch and Maldonado 2011). Affiliative interactions are shaped by socio-positive relationships (Hirsch et al. 2012), while inter-individual distance (i.e., proximity) within a group is shaped by agonistic interactions and food competition (Hirsch et al. 2012) as well as predator avoidance (Romey and Wallace 2007). Social relationships within groups can be quantified by network analysis, which is a useful tool to quantify sex and age patterns of affiliative and agonistic interactions and proximity in group living animals. Such knowledge can be applied to better understand individual variation in species of conservation concern (Jacoby et al. 2011; Snijders et al. 2017).

An individual’s decision about ‘with whom’ and ‘to which extent’ to interact in a group can be affected by intrinsic (e.g., motivation, sex, age) and extrinsic (e.g., proximity, group composition) factors that may vary across seasons (Whirligig Beetles (Coleoptera, Gyrinidae): Romey and Wallace 2007; Yellow-Bellied Marmots: Wey and Blumstein 2010; White-Faced Capuchins: Crofoot et al. 2011). For instance, in complex social systems, where individuals tend to associate with friends of their friends, such long-term social relationships are known to modulate the number of affiliative ties among members of a group (Vervet Monkeys (Chlorocebus pygerythrus): Borgeaud et al. 2016; Spotted Hyenas (Crocuta Crocuta): Illany et al. 2015). Relationship patterns may dynamically change across time as individuals join or leave a group (Blonder et al. 2012; Kubitza et al. 2015) in the course of seasonal migratory and dispersal processes (Idani 1991; Blumstein et al. 2009; Pinter-Wollman et al. 2009) as well as reproductive phases (Hamede, Bashford et al. 2009; Brent et al. 2013).

Understanding the social network of threatened group-living animals is a core challenge for behavioural ecologists especially when the group composition is seasonal. In this study, we explore patterns of social behaviour and inter-individual distance (i.e., proximity) in the Northern Bald Ibis (Geronticus eremita), an endangered (BirdLife International 2020) colonial breeding species with fission–fusion foraging patterns. In addition to changes in seasonal movement, such as dispersal and/or local migration, and the formation of seasonally monogamous reproductive pairs, diurnal patterns of association form as the group roosts together at night and forages in subgroups during the day (Böhm and Pegoraro 2011). Individuals tend to form new reproductive pairs each year, though in some cases, partners of the previous year re-pair (Wickler and Seibt 1985; Böhm and Pegoraro 2011). The species is colonial throughout the year (del Hojo et al. 1992). Eggs are generally laid between March and May (del Hojo et al. 1992). Juveniles are cared for by the parents until approximately 2 weeks after fledging (Böhm and Pegoraro 2011). A social dominance hierarchy is generally established between the members of a colony, in which dominance rank increases with age (Sorato and Kotrschal 2006) and males are dominant over females (Böhm and Pegoraro 2011). Younger birds are more likely to disperse than older birds (Böhm et al. in press). Juveniles seem to have a different overwintering area than adults, which was observed in the extinct Middle East population (Lindsell et al. 2009). In this seasonally and socially dynamic system, understanding how age, sex and reproductive partners affect patterns of relationship between individuals can contribute to our knowledge of the potential link between sociality and fitness. Northern Bald Ibis affiliative interactions between pair partners scaled negatively with endoparasite burden during periods of elevated stress (e.g., reproductive period, Puehringer-Sturmayr et al. 2018) suggesting that being embedded in a pair bond may have a positive impact on individual fitness (Frigerio et al. 2016). Therefore, Northern Bald Ibis might be a suitable model to investigate the relationships between an individual’s network position (including other metrics, such as density) and risks (e.g., disease transmission, increased competition) and opportunities (e.g., access to social information) with respect to reproduction and survival.

In the present study, we assessed the effect of season (autumn and winter), sex, age and reproductive partner on proximity between individuals and social behaviour (i.e., affiliative and agonistic interactions between colony members) in a free-flying and individually marked colony of Northern Bald Ibis. We tested the following predictions: (1) there will be an effect of season on inter-bird proximity and the number of agonistic and affiliative interactions, as Northern Bald Ibis engage in pre-breeding displays during autumn and establish new pair bonds during winter (Böhm and Pegoraro 2011), (2) there will be an effect of sex, age and reproductive partner on the observed patterns. Specifically, we predict (1) more agonistic interactions in males during winter than in autumn in competition for mates and nest sites, (2) therefore, generally more agonistic interactions in males than females, and (3) comparable levels of affiliative behaviours in males and females due to relatively symmetrical patterns of interaction within pairs (Sorato and Kotrschal 2006). We also predict that older and more experienced birds will occupy more socially central positions (determined through both affiliative and agonistic interactions in separate social networks) within the group during both seasons associated with known benefits of socially central positions for survivorship in males and females (Stanton and Mann 2012; Archie et al. 2014; Kalbitzer et al. 2017). Older and experienced birds may profit from a socially central position by reducing stress load via affiliative ties (Puehringer-Sturmayr et al. 2018), by increasing fitness via the engagement in breeding activities (Formica et al. 2012; Kalbitzer et al. 2017), and by gaining access to resources via agonistic encounters and rank acquisition (Kulahci et al. 2016).

Methods

Study population and area

A colony of free-flying Northern Bald Ibis was established at the Konrad Lorenz Research Center (KLF) in 1997 in coordination with the European Breeding Programme (Böhm 1999; Tuckova et al. 1998). This research facility is situated 5 km south of Grünau im Almtal/Upper Austria (47.852778°N, 13.955556°E). The birds are housed in a year-round open aviary (approximately 20 × 15 × 7 m [L × B × H]; referred to as ‘nesting site’ hereafter) in the local Cumberland Game Park. The aviary is used for roosting as well as for breeding in spring. The birds can leave the aviary at any time and roam the foraging grounds in the area. During winter (from mid-December until March) and depending on the harsh alpine weather conditions, the ibis generally stay at their nesting site. The birds are supplemented with food twice per day during winter periods when resources are scarce and later during spring at the continuation of the breeding season (hash made from 1-day-old chicks and beef heart, mixed with insects and water-soaked dog food; generally from November to May). All the birds are individually marked with coloured leg rings. Birds were habituated to the presence of humans. Observations were conducted in the surroundings of the Konrad Lorenz Research Center (i.e., meadows and a river with gravel banks) and the adjacent Cumberland Game Park (i.e., at their nesting site). At the start of data collection, the colony consisted of 39 individuals (24 males and 15 females, aged from 7 months to 17 years; mean age ± SD = 3.92 ± 4.52; Table S1).

Data collection

Data were collected from 21 October 2016 until 28 February 2017. The observation period was divided into two phases: (1) autumn, data collected from October to December and (2) winter, data collected from January to February. Observations were performed between 0800 AM and 0400 PM of all birds present at that time. We did focal observations of all birds by applying a continuous recording method (Altmann 1974) with each protocol lasting 10 min. Each individual was observed 46.5 ± 1.7 times (mean ± SE), totalling 1814 observation protocols. TS collected a total number of 890 protocols during autumn (Nprotocols field = 727, Nprotocols aviary = 163) and a total number of 924 protocols during winter (Nprotocols field = 211, Nprotocols aviary = 713). All behavioural data were collected with the software Prim8 Mobile (mobile computing to record nature, https://www.prim8software.com/; McDonald and Johnson 2014). We monitored frequencies of initiated and received affiliative (greeting, bill shaking, clasped necks, preening invitation, preening, contact sitting) and agonistic behaviours (threat, threat greeting, pecking, defensive threat, displacement, attempted displacement, fighting; for a detailed description of the listed behaviours see Pegoraro 1992). Additionally, proximity (< 0.5 m, based on previous observations; measured as duration) of the focal animal to other colony members was measured by visual estimation. For each colony member that came within a 0.5 m radius of the focal individual during the 10 min observation time, we recorded the duration of the proximity between the focal bird and other colony members. Proximity estimation for ibis was trained before starting data collection by estimating and then measuring the distance between bins.

Data analysis

Social network analyses and calculation of network metrics were performed in Ucinet v. 6.699 (Bogatti et al. 2002). The randomization procedures to determine if the networks are ‘non-random’ were computed in R version 4.0.2 (R Core Team 2020). For the visualisation of the networks, we used the software Gephi 0.9.2 (Bastian et al. 2009). To analyse the influence of age on the position of the individuals within the network, the age in months was calculated for each focal animal. Since preliminary analysis showed differences in the network metrics (i.e., degree, in-degree, out-degree, eigenvector centrality) of proximity and affiliative interactions between autumn and winter (Wilcoxon signed rank test with continuity correction conducted via R version 3.6.1 (R Core Team 2019); see results), we analysed the two seasons for all response variables in separate models.

Proximity (i.e., all colony members within 0.5 m of the focal individual) was measured to analyse the inter-individual distance during natural foraging and roosting. Four directed and two undirected social networks were created (Farine and Whitehead 2015) for affiliative and agonistic interactions (one directed network each for autumn and winter) as well as proximity (one undirected network each for autumn and winter) split by season. Nodes were defined as the individuals within the colony and edges represented the interactions between those individuals. For affiliative and agonistic networks, edges represented the raw frequency count of interactions between individuals. In the proximity network, a simple ratio index (time observed together/time both observed * 100) was used to calculate the edge weights. Social networks were examined at node-, dyadic- and network-level. To measure individual variation in sociality at the node-level, we calculated different network metrics for each individual in each network representing initiated and received social interactions, i.e., weighted degree (sum of all individuals that interacted with a specific individual), weighted in-degree (corresponds to received affiliative and agonistic behaviours; number of colony members from whom an interaction is received), weighted out-degree (corresponds to initiated affiliative and agonistic behaviours; number of colony members towards which an interaction is initiated) and eigenvector centrality (a measure of social centrality within the network which is proportional to the sum of the centralities of an individual’s neighbours). Those network metrics were selected, because we distinguished between initiated and received behaviours, and because in Northern Bald Ibis, affiliative interactions increase during the pre-breeding and breeding season and decrease after the offspring fledged; also, most affiliative interactions occur between former and prospective mates. Degree and eigenvector centrality measure the number of incident links and the quality of those links (i.e., the importance of nodes in a network), which is an important measure to assess how an individual’s associations differ between sex of the interactants and change with season and age.

Binary networks (connection between two individuals was coded as 1 and no connection between two individuals was coded as 0) were used to calculate density at the network-level (proportion of possible connections used within a network).

To assess whether associations among pairs of individuals deviated from random variation, we performed pre-network randomizations using data stream permutations for focal sampling data collection protocols and compared the strength of associations of the original and permuted data sets via t-test with the Animal Network Toolkit Software (ANTs) R package (Sosa et al. 2020). Data stream permutations swap a single association in each permutation and this permutation can be controlled according to the focal observation number.

Due to the non-independence of network data and consequently the violation of the assumptions of classical statistical analyses, we applied a random permutation approach. Using Node-Level Regression with 1000 permutations, we investigated which factors influenced social structure in the Northern Bald Ibis. Node-Level Regression performs a basic Ordinary Least Squares (OLS) linear regression and uses random permutations method for constructing sampling distributions of R-squared and slope coefficients to estimate standard errors and significance (Hanneman and Riddle 2005). We defined (1) degree (i.e., proximity), (2) out-degree (i.e., initiated affiliative and agonistic behaviours), (3) in-degree (i.e., received affiliative and agonistic behaviours) and (4) eigenvector centrality (i.e., proximity, initiated and received affiliative and agonistic behaviours) as dependent variables. Sex and age (in months) were included as independent variables.

At the dyadic-level a Double Dekker Semi-Partialling Multiple Regression Quadratic Assignment Procedure (MR-QAP; Dekker et al. 2007) with 1000 permutations was used to examine whether same sex, differences in age and reproductive partners predict proximity and social interactions (affiliative and agonistic interactions) between individuals. MR-QAP performs a regression between a dependent matrix (i.e., observed data) and one or more independent matrices (e.g., similarities in attributes of nodes such as sex). The double semi-partialling algorithm proceeds in two steps: (1) a standard multiple regression is computed across corresponding cells of the dependent and independent matrices and (2) together rows and columns of the matrices are randomly permuted, after which the regression is recomputed. To estimate standard errors, this step is repeated 1000 times. For each coefficient, the proportion of random permutations that showed a coefficient as extreme as the one for observed values is counted. We conducted six separate MR-QAPs with the network proximity in autumn, proximity in winter, agonistic interactions in autumn, agonistic interactions in winter, affiliative interactions in autumn and affiliative interactions in winter as the dependent matrices, while sex-based homophily (same sex), age-based heterophily (differences in age in months) and reproductive partners (i.e., previous ones before 2017 and partners in 2017) were used as the independent matrices. For the homophily matrix, same sex dyads were coded as 1, while different sex dyads were coded as 0. In the heterophily matrix, the absolute difference of age in months between dyads was calculated. Former or prospective reproductive partners were coded as 1 and other conspecifics as 0.

Results are presented as mean + standard deviation.

Results

Proximity patterns

The proximity network strength of association deviated from random variation during autumn (tobserved = 32.687, tpermutation mean = 36.389, pone-tailed = 0.032) and winter (tobserved = 29.470, tpermutation mean = 35.765, pone-tailed =  < 0.001). We found an effect of season, reproductive partner and age on proximity patterns, but no differences in sex. In autumn, individuals spent less time in close proximity (i.e.,  < 0.5 m) to conspecifics than in winter. The social centrality position of individuals within the colony did not differ between seasons (Table S2).

Autumn

Network density was high (97.3%), meaning that almost all individuals within the colony were generally in close proximity to each other. Individuals spent more time in close proximity to previous or new reproductive partners compared to other colony members. Sex-based homophily or age-based heterophily did not influence proximity patterns within the colony (Table S3). No sex differences were found in proximity patterns and centrality position (Fig. 1a; weighted degreemales = 34.88 + 13.29, weighted degreefemales = 32.47 + 12.83, eigenvector centralitymales = 0.15 + 0.07, eigenvector centraltyfemales = 0.14 + 0.07). However, older individuals spent generally more time in close proximity to other colony members and thus occupied a more central position within the network than younger individuals (Table S4).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Undirected proximity networks of the Northern Bald Ibis colony during autumn (a) and winter (b). Males are highlighted in black, females in grey. Node size increases with the age of the individuals. Edge size represents how often an individual was observed near another colony member. Nmales = 24, Nfemales = 15

Winter

Network density decreased during winter (84.4%) as compared to autumn, meaning that all individuals were generally less often in close proximity to each other compared to autumn. Colony members spent more time in close proximity to previous or new reproductive partners compared to other individuals. Sex-based homophily and age-based heterophily had no effect on proximity patterns (Table S3). Sex did not influence proximity patterns and centrality position within the network (Fig. 1a; weighted degreemales = 63.92 + 15.08, weighted degreefemales = 56.00 + 18.97, eigenvector centralitymales = 0.15 + 0.06, eigenvector centralityfemales = 0.14 + 0.08). Older birds spent more time in close proximity to other colony members and occupied more central positions (Table S4).

Agonistic interactions

The agonistic association network deviated from random variation during autumn (tobserved = 9.245, tpermutation mean = 24.900, pone-tailed = 0.006) and winter (tobserved = 9.655, tpermutation mean = 23.384, pone-tailed =  < 0.001). We found no effect of season or reproductive partner, but effects of age and sex on patterns of agonistic interactions. Initiated and received agonistic interactions as well as the centrality position within the network did not differ between autumn and winter (Table S2).

Autumn

A network density of 18.4% indicated low numbers of possible connections within the agonistic network during autumn. Agonistic behaviours were not shown towards previous or new reproductive partners. Sex-based homophily had no influence on the selection of interaction partner (Table S3) and sex did not modify initiating or receiving agonistic interactions (Fig. 2a; out-degreemales = 10.63 + 8.57, out-degreefemales = 8.87 + 7.29, in-degreemales = 8.46 + 5.23, in-degreefemales = 12.33 + 4.56). The centrality positions occupied within the social network did not vary with sex or age (Table S4; eigenvector centralitymales = 0.14 + 0.06, eigenvector centralityfemales = 0.16 + 0.08). Agonistic interactions occurred mostly between individuals of similar age (Table S3). Younger individuals received more and initiated fewer agonistic interactions (Table S4).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Directed social networks of the initiated and received agonistic interactions in autumn (a) and winter (b). Males are highlighted in black, females in grey. Node size increases with the age of the individuals. Edge size represents how often an individual was involved in an agonistic interaction with another colony member. Edge colour indicates the origin of the behaviour and the direction of the association is shown clockwise. Nmales = 24, Nfemales = 15

Winter

As in autumn, the network density of the agonistic network during winter was 18.4%, indicating only few connections between conspecifics. Previous or new reproductive partners were not engaged in agonistic interactions with each other. Agonistic interactions were mostly exchanged between individuals of the same sex (Table S3). Males initiated more agonistic behaviours and occupied a more central position within the network than females (Fig. 2b; out-degreemales = 13.79 + 8.03, out-degreefemales = 6.80 + 4.99, eigenvector centralitymales = 0.17 + 0.05, eigenvector centralityfemales = 0.12 + 0.04). However, receiving agonistic interactions did not vary with sex (Table S4; in-degreemales = 11.88 + 5.58, in-degreefemales = 9.87 + 5.94). Agonistic interactions occurred mostly between individuals of similar age (Table S3). Older birds initiated more and received fewer agonistic interactions. However, age did not influence the centrality position occupied within the network (Table S4).

Affiliative interactions

The affiliative association network deviated from random variation during autumn (tobserved = 3.885, tpermutation mean = 11.937, pone-tailed = 0.002) and winter (tobserved = 3.843, tpermutation mean = 11.125, pone-tailed = 0.002). We found effects of season, reproductive partner, age and sex on patterns of affiliative interactions. Generally, more affiliative behaviour was initiated and received during winter than autumn, independent of sex and age. Furthermore, the social centrality position of single individuals within the network did not differ between seasons (Table S2).

Autumn

A network density of 5.2% was found in the affiliative network during autumn. Affiliative interactions were mostly observed between previous and new reproductive partners. All focal animals except for two were involved in affiliative interactions. However, sex-based homophily and age-based heterophily had no influence on the selection of interaction partner (Table S3). We found no sex differences in initiating and receiving affiliative behaviour as well as no effect of sex or age on the individual centrality position within the network (Fig. 3a; out-degreemales = 11.71 + 13.94, out-degreefemales = 17.73 + 16.81, in-degreemales = 11.88 + 14.97, in-degreefemales = 17.47 + 16.23, eigenvector centralitymales = 0.03 + 0.14, eigenvector centralityfemales = 0.05 + 0.18). Older birds increasingly initiated and received affiliative behaviours (Table S4).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Directed social networks of the initiated and received affiliative interactions in autumn (a) and winter (b). Males are highlighted in black, females in grey. Node size increases with the age of the individuals. Edge size represents how often an individual was involved in an affiliative interaction with another colony member. Edge colour indicates the origin of the behaviour and the direction of the association is shown clockwise. Nmales = 24, Nfemales = 15

Winter

Similar to autumn, the network density reached 4.7% in the affiliative network during winter, indicating relatively few connections. Affiliative interactions occurred mostly between previous and new reproductive partners. Sex-based homophily and age-based heterophily had no influence on the selection of interaction partner (Table S3). Females received significantly more affiliative interactions than males (Fig. 3b; in-degreemales = 19.83 + 26.87, in-degreefemales = 38.73 + 34.22). However, initiating affiliative interactions did not vary with sex (out-degreemales = 24.83 + 29.78, out-degreefemales = 30.73 + 30.59). Sex and age had no influence on the centrality position occupied within the network (eigenvector centralitymales = 0.03 + 0.14, eigenvector centralityfemales = 0.05 + 0.18). Older birds initiated as well as received significantly more affiliative behaviours (Table S4).

Discussion

In our study we document strong seasonal differences in patterns of proximity and social behaviour in the endangered Northern Bald Ibis. Specifically, we recorded (1) lower inter-individual distance (high proximity network density) during autumn compared with winter, (2) no seasonal difference in patterns of agonistic behaviour but more agonistic interactions in males than females, (3) seasonal and sex differences in affiliative behaviour with more affiliative behaviour during winter and more affiliative behaviour received by females than males, (4) reproductive partners as preferred affiliative interaction partners, and (5) an effect of age on social and centrality position within the group, with younger birds being less central in the proximity network and receiving more agonistic interactions.

During winter, individuals associated with fewer other colony members, as shown by the number of associates, but they had stronger connections to specific individuals. Relationship patterns may have changed over time as a consequence of reproductive seasonality (Brent et al. 2013) as breeding pair formation starts anew before the onset of the forthcoming breeding season (i.e., already in autumn; Böhm and Pegoraro 2011). This was also supported by our findings that especially older individuals and reproductive partners (former or prospective mates) were observed in close proximity. Seasonal changes in affiliative interactions may also reflect differences in pair bond stability, with loose bonds during the non-breeding season and stronger bonds during the breeding season (Rowley 1985). Furthermore, season-dependent changes in associations could have important implications for conservation: For example, identifying transmission pathways of infectious diseases within an endangered species’ population but also between populations (Hamede et al. 2009). Contrary to our expectations, the occurrence of agonistic encounters did not differ between seasons, whereas affiliative interactions were more evident during winter. Increased affiliative interactions may indicate a function for pair bond quality and mate familiarity. In fact, there is evidence from socially monogamous Blue-Footed Boobies (Sula nebouxii) that affiliative interactions enhance within pair coordination and cooperation during brooding and chick rearing (Sánchez-Macouzet et al. 2014).

Sex and age may strongly influence how individuals aggregate and with whom they interact (Widdig et al. 2001; Boucherie et al. 2016; Rose and Croft 2018). In the present study, patterns of agonistic and also to some extent affiliative interactions differed considerably between males and females during winter. High frequencies of initiated agonistic interactions mostly between males may reflect male-male competition for nest sites. In turn, high frequencies of received affiliative behaviours in females hint at male-male competition for mates. Previous or potentially prospective reproductive partners may invest in pair bond formation and subsequently in pair bond quality with a high exchange of affiliative behaviours. Thus, it seems that, as in other species, males and females have different behavioural patterns during winter, the onset of the breeding season, which may reflect individual investment into pair bond formation and maintenance (Röell 1978; McGraw et al. 2010; Kubitza et al. 2015). By showing consistent levels of aggressiveness (high or low) individuals may avoid extended fights: Males, for instance, may benefit in male-male agonistic interactions through consistency in their high levels of aggressive behaviours, whereas females may benefit from low levels of agonistic behaviours (Dall et al. 2004). Our results further imply that females received affiliative behaviours predominantly from previous or potentially prospective mates.

Partner affiliation may bring significant benefits to both sexes, as socio-positive relationships can be beneficial in terms of lower levels of stress hormones, which may have a positive impact on the immune system (Puehringer-Sturmayr et al. 2018) or better access to food and protection against predators (Kalbitzer et al. 2017). This suggests that affiliative interactions among Northern Bald Ibis may also be interpreted as an investment of a partner towards containing the potentially detrimental effect of high parasite load on fitness (Frigerio et al. 2016), which was shown in Cape Ground Squirrels (Xerus inauris; Hillegass et al. 2010). In the present study, affiliative interactions frequently occurred between previous or prospective reproductive partners. However, other avian species living in year-round sociality showed different patterns: Rooks (Corvus frugilegus), for instance, do not restrict affiliative behaviour to breeding partners (Boucherie et al. 2016). Future research could test for the quality of the pair bond or for re-mating occurrence in relation to patterns of affiliative behaviour during the breeding season.

As in other species (mammals: Bekoff 1972; primates: Chalmers 1983; honey bees: Amdam 2011), our findings in Northern Bald Ibis support the idea that social interactions with conspecifics and the individual social role change with age. Stability of relationships can influence how a population reacts to environmental change. For instance, social instability may reduce survival and fitness by increasing inter-individual aggression (Linklater et al. 1999). Notably, dispersal decisions might be a consequence of early social experience (Bekoff 1977; Holekamp 1984). Up to 50% of the juveniles in the study population disperse, some of which return during autumn (Böhm et al. in press). In our study, young Northern Bald Ibis occupied social positions at the edge of the proximity network; in addition, they received many agonistic interactions and almost no affiliative interactions. Individuals of similar age mainly engaged in agonistic interactions with each other, but there was no evidence of them being in closer proximity or being involved in affiliative interactions with conspecifics of similar age. Thus, juveniles might not have formed socio-positive relationships among themselves.

Different positions within a social network might also be associated with age-related mortality risk and potential fitness consequences. Juvenile Greylag Geese (Anser anser), for example, became more successful as breeders later in life than others if they had higher social connectivity during earlier life stages (Szipl et al. 2019). Because juvenile Greylag Geese form long-lasting social bonds with their parents (Rutschke 1982), this may influence reproduction early in life due to the parents’ associated social position within the flock (Szipl et al. 2019); but this does not apply to Northern Bald Ibis, as offspring do not maintain long-lasting relationships with their parents (Böhm and Pegoraro 2011). As our study shows, juvenile Northern Bald Ibis occupy social positions on the edge of the network and tend to disperse instead of staying.

In addition to the parameters considered in this study (i.e., season, reproductive partner, sex and age), several additional factors could have an impact on patterns of relationships, such as density and distribution of food (Chapman 1990; Howery et al. 1998), the presence of peers (Howery et al. 1998) or behavioural phenotype (i.e., passive or active; Croft et al. 2009). Density and distribution of food, for instance, generally determines group size, as shown for instance in Spider Monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi; Chapman 1990). Even though our Northern Bald Ibis population receives supplemental feeding during winter, behavioural observations were not performed in this context; therefore, we tend to exclude such an effect on inter-individual distance (i.e., proximity) or agonistic encounters in the present study. Furthermore, social factors (i.e., peers) can alter location and habitat use by increasing distance or associations to specific individuals (e.g., yearling cattle, Howery et al. 1998). Our results suggest that mainly pair partners (former/prospective mates) are involved in establishing social ties. This suggests that colony formation in the Northern Bald Ibis serves mainly predator avoidance and info-parasitism, but is not the basis for cooperation that goes beyond the reproductive pair, whereas Greylag Geese establish female-bonded clans within the flock in addition to the reproductive pair (Frigerio et al. 2001). Behavioural traits may also modify an individual's number of ties, as passive individuals may seek refuge in a social group (e.g., Guppies (Poecilia reticulata), Croft et al. 2009). Regarding the role of individual behavioural phenotype, our unpublished data suggest that passive juveniles are more likely to be in the centre of a social group compared to active juveniles (Puehringer-Sturmayr et al. in prep). We are aware that the aviary conditions might have affected the patterns we observed. Most birds remained at their nest site during winter, probably because of the harsh alpine weather conditions, while the birds freely roamed the foraging grounds throughout the valley during autumn. Aviary size could have led to a concentration of interactions and proximity between colony members in winter. However, as the birds were able to leave the aviary at any point, and we indeed made observations outside the aviary as well, we conclude that the observed patterns of relationships were not fully constrained by the location.

Investigating patterns of relationships in an endangered species is relevant for conservation and reintroduction projects in the wild. The social complexity of a Northern Bald Ibis colony is a challenge when reintroducing individuals (Bowden et al. 2010) as the social structure of a released group may have tremendous effects on reproductive success (Böhm and Pegoraro 2011). Our results imply that young Northern Bald Ibis may be less optimal for reintroduction because of greater mortality and dispersal risk associated with their social position within the network than adults. However, in combination with ‘human parents’ for the chicks, the release of groups of hand-reared Northern Bald Ibis chicks with contact to human parents has been shown to be effective (Jordan et al. 2003). From other study systems, there is evidence that the age structure of a released group has an immense effect on survival and fertility (Sarrazin and Barbault 1996); therefore, designing release programs that take into account age-specific bottlenecks to survival and the strengths and limitations of hand rearing should inform approaches. Specifically, understanding the social position of individuals could help to select specific individuals for reintroduction and to predict seasonal trends in habitat use associated with behaviour (Snijders et al. 2017), age-related mortality risk associated with different positions within the social network (Ellis et al. 2017), and not least the role of male affiliative behaviour for pair bond formation (Kubitza et al. 2015).