Introduction

The defence of access to limited resources, such as refuges, mates, nest sites or food, produces benefits for the individual. Simultaneously, high costs related to patrolling, chasing intruders, signalling, the risk of predation and the risk of injury are generated. If the benefits from exclusive access to limited resources exceed the costs of defence, territoriality is observed (Brown 1964). The possibility of recognition between familiar neighbours and unfamiliar strangers can reduce the costs of territorial defence by preventing unnecessary conflicts with familiar individuals (Temeles 1994). Neighbours do not necessarily constitute a serious threat to the territory holder, whereas any stranger could be a potential rival seeking a territorial insertion or takeover. Therefore, after the borders of the territory are established, the reaction of the territory-holder to an intrusion by a familiar neighbour should be less aggressive than the reaction to an intrusion by a stranger. This reduction of aggression towards a neighbour has been termed the “dear enemy phenomenon” (Fisher 1954) and has been observed in numerous territorial species, including mammals, birds, reptiles, amphibians and insects (Davis 1987; Temeles 1994). However, a “dear enemy” relationship may be flexible and may evolve with the social and ecological circumstances (Briefer et al. 2008b). In certain cases, a neighbour can be more threatening than a stranger (Temeles 1990). Moreover, the presence of a female within a male’s territory can modify the male’s aggressive responses to neighbours and strangers (Leiser 2003). Neighbours can be treated as a dear enemy only in the middle of the breeding season, when territories are stable, but not at the beginning and at the end of the season (Briefer et al. 2008b). Moreover, a group signature can be used by birds for neighbour–stranger discrimination (Briefer et al. 2008a). In the above-mentioned situations, intrusions of neighbours or strangers represent varying levels of threat to the territory-holder. A different response should therefore be observed if the ability to recognise individuals is present.

In birds, acoustic signals are commonly used in individual recognition (Catchpole and Slater 2008). Calls or songs enable discrimination between parent and offspring (Jouventin et al. 1999), between mates (Clark et al. 2006) and between territorial neighbours and strangers (Stoddard 1996). neighbour–stranger discrimination (hereafter NSD) in a territorial-defence context has been studied primarily in songbirds, Oscines, i.e. birds acquiring a song through learning during ontogeny (Stoddard 1996). Relatively little is known about NSD in non-learning bird species. Only a few studies have presented evidence for NSD in grouse (Falls and McNicholl 1979), tyrant flycatchers (Lovell and Lein 2004), shearwaters (Mackin 2005), loons (Mager et al. 2010), wood hoopoes (Radford 2005) and owls (Hardouin et al. 2006). Furthermore, several researchers have suggested the potential occurrence of NSD based on individually specific call characteristics in non-learners (e.g. Peake et al. 1998; Rebbeck et al. 2001; Ręk and Osiejuk 2011a).

In this study, we investigated vocal NSD in the Corncrake, Crex crex, a non-learning (Brenowitz 1991), migrant rail species, Rallidae, in its natural environment. During the breeding season, Corncrakes inhabit wet, open areas with dense vegetation, where visual contact is very difficult (Green et al. 1997; Schäffer 1999). Corncrakes are sequentially polygamous, i.e. both males and females change partners between breeding attempts (Green et al. 1997). Males defend territories. The size of a territory ranges from 1 to more than 50 ha. The current knowledge of the dispersion and annual survival of the Corncrake is rudimentary. However, within-season movements of males, both within hundreds of metres and over distances of more than 10 km, are not infrequent (Schäffer 1999). A return of ringed males to the same territories after 2 years has also been observed (B. Olech, personal communication).

At night, territorial Corncrake males give a very loud, bisyllabic and monotonous “cracking” call (Fig. 1). The function of this call appears to be equivalent to the function of song in songbirds, i.e. mate attraction, territory defence and announcement (Cramp and Simmons 1980). In contrast to the songs of many songbirds, this Corncrake call is seemingly very simple in structure. However, there is a higher degree of variation among individuals than within individuals in the temporal and frequency properties of calls (Peake et al. 1998; Osiejuk and Olech 2004; Ręk and Osiejuk 2011a). Therefore, NSD is possible in the Corncrake, at least in theory. In particular, the individually characteristic pattern of intervals between successive maximal amplitude peaks within a syllable (termed pulse-to-pulse duration, PPD; Peake et al. 1998) is often used by observers for the individual discrimination of males (Peake and McGregor 2001; Terry and McGregor 2002). However, the ability of Corncrake males to use PPD to discriminate among other males has never been confirmed.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Sonogram of Corncrake, Crex crex, call. The characteristics used for call measurement are shown: SYL1 first syllable, SYL2 second syllable, INT1 within-call interval, INT2 between-call interval

In this study, we used interactive playback to experimentally examine the ability of Corncrakes to discriminate between the calls of familiar neighbours and unfamiliar strangers. We analysed the behaviour of territory holders during simulated intrusions by neighbours and strangers. Both vocal and non-vocal responses to the playback were considered.

Methods

Study area

The breeding period of the Corncrake in Poland extends from the middle of May to the middle of August (Green et al. 1997). Our study was conducted during the middle of the Corncrake breeding season, between 5 June and 1 July 2010. We tested 43 territorial males from two localities in central (Kampinoski National Park, 52°19′N, 20°23′E, n = 28) and eastern Poland (Upper Nurzec River Valley, 52°36′N, 23°13′E, n = 15). Both localities are Important Birds Areas of international importance in Poland, with the size of the Corncrake population estimated as 110–140 males in Kampinoski National Park and 206–229 in the Upper Nurzec River Valley (Wilk et al. 2010). The Corncrakes were irregularly distributed in both localities. The distance between a territorial male and its nearest neighbour ranged from 50 to 340 m (mean = 191 m, SD = 76.5 m). The birds were not individually marked. We used PPD to confirm the identity of the males according to the method proposed by Peake et al. (1998).

Preparation of call stimuli and experimental protocol

To prepare neighbour call stimuli, we recorded calling males 1–3 days before the experiment. Recordings were made from a distance of ca. 10 m with an Edirol R-09 recorder and a Sennheiser ME 67 directional microphone with a K6 power unit (frequency response 40 Hz to 20 kHz). Recordings were made at night (2200–0400 hours local time). The exact locations of the calling males were determined with Garmin GPS receivers. Stranger stimuli were prepared with recordings from the local populations collected in 2007 and 2008 (Kampinoski National Park, n = 103; Nurzec Valley, n = 58). Recordings from Kampinoski National Park from 2007 and 2008 had a 22.05 kHz/16 bit sampling rate. The rest of the recordings had a 44.1 kHz/16 bit sampling rate. Therefore, all recordings were calibrated to the same digital quality (22.05 kHz/16 bit sampling) and were transferred to a PC workstation. The playbacks were digitally prepared to match a 95 ± 5 dB signal pressure level (at 1 m), an average natural amplitude for the Corncrake call (based on the authors’ unpublished data). An earlier study of Corncrake vocalisation (Osiejuk et al. 2004) showed that the Corncrake call characteristic termed “rhythm” is individually variable within a single season and depends on the males’ size and testosterone level. Rhythm is calculated as RHYTHM = INT2/(SYL1 + INT1 + SYL2) (where SYL1 is the duration of the first syllable, SYL2 the duration of the second syllable, INT1 the duration of the within-call interval, and INT2 the duration of the between-call interval) and reflects the monotonous (low value of RHYTHM) or intermittent (high value of RHYTHM) character of calling, which can be detected easily by the human ear. Moreover, males respond more aggressively to the playback of an intermittent rhythm than to that of a monotonous rhythm, and males with intermittent calls are more aggressive (Ręk and Osiejuk 2010). To overcome this problem, we controlled rhythm in our experiment. To each male call used in the playback experiment, we randomly assigned a stranger male call with the same rhythm as the neighbour call used in the paired treatment. Thus, the rhythms of the neighbour and the stranger stimuli did not differ.

Avisoft SASLab Pro v.5.0.16 software (Specht 2007) was used for all the acoustic measurements and for the preparation of the song playback stimuli. To measure the temporal characteristics of the call (SYL1, SYL2, INT1, INT2, and RHYTHM), we used the following set of parameters: FFT length = 1,024, Frame size (%) = 25, Window = Hamming, and Temporal Overlap = 98.43 %. These parameter values produced a 112 Hz bandwidth with 21 Hz frequency and 0.72 ms time resolution (Specht 2007).

Experimental design and playback procedure

We used an interactive playback experiment consisting of two trials in which the call of a neighbour and the call of a stranger were presented (in random order) to the subject. A neighbour was defined as an individual with a territory bordering the territory of the subject (but no more distant than 300 m), whereas a stranger was defined as a random male from the local population whose territory was more than 5 km distant from the subject and whose call was recorded during a different year. Both treatments (neighbour and stranger stimuli) were performed on the same night between 2230 and 0330 hours (local time). The time between treatments ranged from 0100 to 0350 hours. In this way, we avoided the influence of different weather conditions or changes in male status between treatments. Moreover, the use of this time period between successive trials was sufficient to allow the birds to return to their pre-stimulus levels of behaviour. We used 43 unique call recordings of different neighbours and 43 of different strangers to avoid pseudoreplication (Kroodsma 1989; Kroodsma et al. 2001). We did not control the presence of females or the stage of the breeding cycle. However, paired males are rarely vocally active, whereas males without females were found to sing during 95 % of night-time checks (Tyler and Green 1996). Therefore, we may assume that the tested males were not paired. During each experiment, we simulated a situation in which a neighbour or stranger male appeared in the territory of the subject. Before the experiment, one person placed a speaker (SEKAKU WA-320, 20-W amplifier and 50–15 000 Hz frequency range; Taichung, ROC Taiwan), connected to a Creative ZEN player, between the subject and the neighbour (<0.5 m above ground, approximately 20 m from the subject). In the second treatment, the loudspeaker was also put between the subject and the neighbour, ca. 20 m from the subject, but in a different place (usually 20–30 m from the previous location). The observer did not know which playback (neighbour or stranger) was tested during the experiment. Simultaneously, the second person approached the neighbour. The proximity of a person caused the neighbour to remain silent during the experiment. Each trial lasted 6 min and consisted of two parts. In the first part of the trial, the male’s behaviour was recorded for 1 min before the playback started. During the second part of the trial, the male’s behaviour was recorded for 5 min during the playback. The stimulus was played if the subject was vocally active and was turned off 10 s after the termination of calling by the subject. The stimulus was played again only if the subject started calling again. This approach was chosen based on our previous observations of Corncrake behaviour during playback experiments. Corncrake males often fall silent after receiving the signal and begin calling again if they intend to locate the sender. In our experiment, the duration of the playback provided to the male ranged from 10 s (if the male was silent during the first and subsequent seconds of the experiment) to 5 min (if no pauses in calling were longer than 10 s in duration). Such an experimental approach, in which the tested male determines the length of the playback he receives, reflects the best natural mode of interaction between real rivals. A similar approach has been used successfully in playback experiments with Corncrake (Ręk and Osiejuk 2011b).

Response measures and statistical analyses

During the experiments, we observed the behaviour of the subject and recorded the calls given by the responding male (Edirol R-09 recorder and Sennheiser ME 67 directional microphone). Corncrake males display very strong territorial behaviour. The aggressive response to simulated territorial intrusion begins with an approach (on foot or in flight) towards the loudspeaker. The males also perform intermittent cracking calls, switch to soft gurgling-mewing calls and finally attack the loudspeaker (Ręk and Osiejuk 2010, 2011b). We chose to record 10 measures of response to assess the effects of the male’s reaction towards the playback of neighbour and stranger stimuli. The measured responses described vocalisations, approaching the speaker, attacks and latencies of response (Table 1). Because it was not possible for the observer to precisely determine the distance between the speaker and the bird during the entire experiment, he always marked a 5-m radius around the speaker before the experiment and focused on detecting approaches closer to the speaker by the males. Attacks on the speaker were easy to detect even at night. Because the speaker was placed on a plastic box, it was easy to hear when the male hit the box.

Table 1 Measures of response to playback of Corncrake, Crex crex, calls of a neighbour and a stranger

The birds’ responses during the experiments with playback were analysed with Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE), an extension of the quasi-likelihood approach that permits the analysis of nested structure in an experimental design (two treatments for each male) and allows the analysis of data with different distribution patterns and incorporating additional predictors. We used binomial dependent variables (with values of 0 or 1) to describe the differences in attacks on the speaker, the latency to the first approach within 5 m, the total time within 5 m of the speaker and the number of gurgling-mewing calls. The value 1 indicated the following outcomes: more frequent attacks on the speaker, a more rapid approach to within 5 m of the speaker, more time spent within 5 m of the speaker and the more frequent performance of gurgling-mewing calls. We fitted the data with a binomial distribution (logit link function). To describe the remaining vocal responses, we measured the durations of SYL1, SYL2, INT1 and INT2, we calculated RHYTHM, and we counted the numbers of syllables (SYL1 + SYL2) performed during a 5-min playback experiment. Corncrake males may make relatively long pauses between calls. These longer pauses increase the value of INT2 considerably. In this paper, we focused on continuous series of calls. Therefore, we arbitrarily decided that a series was completed if INT2 exceeded 2 s. This value was chosen based on measurements of hundreds of naturally vocalising males from Polish populations, where the longest second interval was 1.92 s in length.

We measured all Corncrake calls performed during the experimental procedure and calculated the mean value of each variable. The mean values were used in subsequent analyses. The SYL1, SYL2, INT1, INT2 and RHYTHM variables were fitted with a gamma distribution (log-link function), and the numbers of syllables performed during playback were fitted with a negative binomial distribution (log-link function). We used the following categorical predictor variables in all the models: playback treatments (neighbour or stranger), locality (Kampinoski National Park or Nurzec River Valley) and order of playback (neighbour first vs. stranger first). Quasi-likelihood under the independence model criterion (for choosing the best correlation structures) and its corrected version (for choosing the best subsets of predictors) were used to select the model parameters. In all, we fitted 10 independent models. The best model was chosen for each measure of response during the experiments.

Additionally, to describe the general vocal reaction towards the call of the intruder, we compared the characteristics of the calls given 1 min before the beginning of the playback and during 5 min of the playback experiment. We calculated the mean values for each period of time. We then performed paired t tests (mean values of variables: SYL1, SYL2, INT1, INT2 and RHYTHM) and a Wilcoxon paired test (variable: number of syllables (SYL1 + SYL2)/1 min). Males that were calling before the experiment and were silent or uttered fewer than 5 calls during the experiment were excluded from the comparison of SYL1, SYL2, INT1, INT2 and RHYTHM. A Bonferroni correction for the p value was incorporated. All p values are two-tailed. All the statistical analyses were performed in PASW Statistics 18.

Results

We used interactive playbacks to determine whether Corncrake males (n = 43) distinguish between the calls of a familiar neighbour and an unfamiliar stranger. We found significant differences in the behavioural reactions (number of attacks on the speaker, time spent less than 5 m from the speaker and latency to approach less than 5 m from the speaker) and no differences in the vocal responses towards the playbacks of the neighbour and stranger calls. Differences in playback duration among treatments were not significant (paired t test; t = 0.188, df = 42, p = 0.857).

Behavioural response

During the playback experiments, 26 males approached to a distance less than 5 m from the speaker, and 14 of them attacked the speaker. Seven males approached to a distance greater than 5 m the speaker, and 11 males did not approach the speaker during both treatments. The GEE showed significant differences between the behavioural responses to the playbacks of the calls of the familiar neighbour and the unfamiliar stranger (Table 2). During the playbacks of the stranger stimulus, the territory holders attacked the speaker more frequently, approached to a distance less than 5 m more rapidly, and spent more time close to the speaker (Table 1; Fig. 2). In all cases, the influence of the sequence of playback (neighbour first vs. stranger first) or population was not significant (Table 2).

Table 2 Results of generalized estimating equations showing behavioural responses towards neighbour and stranger stimuli
Fig. 2
figure 2

Comparison of the binomial responses to playback of neighbours’ and strangers’ calls. Only the males that showed any response, at least for one treatment, were considered. a The percentage of males that attacked the speaker more often during the neighbour and stranger stimuli (n = 14); b the percentage of males that approached more rapidly within 5 m of the speaker during the neighbour and stranger stimuli (n = 26); c the percentage of males that spent more time within 5 m of the speaker (n = 26)

Vocal response

Only 3 males did not perform cracking calls during the 5-min playback stage of the experiments. Low-amplitude gurgling-mewing calls were performed by 10 males towards the neighbour stimulus and by 13 males towards the stranger stimulus. This signal was not observed before the playback experiment, and only two males performed it after the experiment. We did not find significant differences between the vocal responses towards a neighbour and a stranger stimulus (in all GEE models p > 0.05) (more details in Table 3). However, we found that many temporal parameters of the calls changed after the start of the playback regardless of the stimuli used. Paired tests showed significant differences between the duration of INT1, INT2, and RHYTHM and numbers of syllables ((SYL1 + SYL2)/1 min) performed before and during the playback. However, the differences in SYL1 (after a Bonferroni adjustment) and SYL2 were not significant (Table 4). During the intruder playback, the Corncrake males significantly increased INT1, INT2, and RHYTHM and performed fewer syllables than before the playback. Overall, the birds responding to playbacks called less intensively and with a more intermittent rhythm compared to natural vocalisations undisturbed by playback.

Table 3 Results of generalized estimating equations showing vocal responses towards neighbour and stranger stimulus
Table 4 Results of paired tests showing general vocal reaction toward call of intruder

Discussion

The results of this study demonstrated the ability of Corncrake males to discriminate between the calls of neighbours and strangers. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to experimentally demonstrate NSD in the order Gruiformes and is one of only a few studies showing that NSD occurs in non-learning bird species. Our results are consistent with the “dear enemy phenomenon” (Fisher 1954) and also suggest that Corncrakes use NSD to maintain the cost of territorial defence at a reasonable level. The more rapid approach to the speaker, the greater amount of time spent near the speaker and, particularly, the greater number of attacks on the speaker during the playback of the stranger call unambiguously indicated the occurrence of a more aggressive response towards the intrusion of a stranger than towards a neighbour. In Corncrakes, a stranger male should clearly represent a more serious threat than a neighbour to a territory holder. It is known that territorial Corncrake males perform visits to the territory of neighbours, especially if the neighbours are close. During such visits, the intruders tend to be silent, apparently to prevent confrontation (Skliba and Fuchs 2004). Moreover, the males are silent if they are paired and call intensely if they are not accompanied by a female (Tyler and Green 1996). Therefore, the appearance of the calling familiar neighbour in the territory of a calling resident is most likely a mistake in location by the intruder, whereas the appearance of an unknown stranger should be a more serious threat.

In birds, vocal communication is very important in territorial interactions (Catchpole and Slater 2008). Changes in the number of trills and whistles (Sprau et al. 2010), number of calls and songs (Lovell and Lein 2005; Hyman and Hughes 2006), soft songs (Ballentine et al. 2008), frequency bandwidth (DuBois et al. 2009) or length of the song (Ríos-Chelén and Garcia 2007) can indicate a more or less aggressive motivation of the sender. The broadcast call of the Corncrake is simple because there is no repertoire variation. However, the male may still modify the intensity and rhythm of the call. We found that Corncrake males increased the rhythm of calling and performed fewer syllables per 1 min during the playback experiment than before the playback. A change to a more intermittent level of rhythm signals a more aggressive motivation of the sender (Ręk and Osiejuk 2010). In contrast, a decrease in the number of calls during aggressive interactions can be explained in two different ways. First, males cannot move and call simultaneously. Therefore, they simply do not call when approaching the speaker. Second, it could be difficult to send and receive such loud signals (even exceeds 95 dB SPL at 1 m; Ręk and Osiejuk 2011a) and simultaneously try to precisely localise the intruder. Corncrake males perform gurgling-mewing calls in addition to cracking calls. We observed gurgling-mewing calls during the playback (26 cases) but never before the playback. Gurgling-mewing calls appear to be the equivalent of quiet or soft songs in passerine birds and are also a signal of the aggressive motivation of males (Ręk and Osiejuk 2011b). Both the cracking and the gurgling-mewing calls indicate an increase in the aggressiveness of the territory holder during the playback experiment. However, the vocal responses towards the intrusion of a neighbour and of a stranger are the same. Our results suggest that the Corncrake call is a first line of defence (Catchpole and Slater 2008). Males signal aggression towards both intruders, but they assume the risk of attack significantly more often towards a stranger. The lack of differences in very simple call structure responses and the presence of clear differences in other behavioural responses (i.e. approaching or attacking the speaker) demonstrates that the absence of a differential response does not imply the absence of discrimination. This finding is very important for the interpretation of negative results in NSD experiments.

The development of NSD in non-learning species with very simple calls is an important topic. Whether or not songs or calls are learned, individually specific call characteristics must exist to make NSD possible (Falls 1982; Stoddard 1996). Individuals should also be able to distinguish and remember the calls of neighbours. Songbirds develop song through learning from conspecifics, and this process undoubtedly affects NSD by shaping repertoire sizes, the level of song sharing and other outcomes of cultural transmission (Stoddard 1996). However, in the Corncrake, as in the other Gruiformes, song learning is absent (Brenowitz 1991). Thus, any of the call characteristics that contribute to an individual vocal signature must arise independently of learning processes. Nevertheless, Corncrake males must also learn, in the sense that learning and remembering the calls of other males is a necessary condition for distinguishing between the calls at a later time. The time window during which males learn and memorise the calls of their neighbours, as well as the call characteristics that are used for individual discrimination, are still unknown. It is probable that Corncrake males use between-individual variation in PPD characteristics for individual discrimination. This call characteristic is individually specific, does not vary during the life of the bird and is potentially very useful for individual discrimination (Peake et al. 1998). Moreover, information contained in PPD is not lost during transmission in the natural habitat (Ręk and Osiejuk 2011a). However, no direct proof exists that Corncrakes actually use PPD for individual discrimination. It is noteworthy that pulses are, on average, 3–5 ms in length and are separated by intervals of 4–8 ms (Peake et al. 1998). Therefore, the intervals between following pulses are very small (ca. 7–13 ms), and it could be difficult for males to distinguish such small differences (Dooling 2004). Alternatively, Fitch and Kelley (2000) showed that, in Gruiformes, formants provide acoustic cues to individuality and body size. Moreover, Suthers (1994) suggested that formant frequencies are a potential basis of acoustic recognition in the oilbird, Steatornis caripensis, a colonial bird species that inhabits dark caves. Unfortunately, formant dispersion in the Corncrake call has not yet been studied. Therefore, there is a need for further research to determine the call characteristics responsible for coding individual identity in the Corncrake.

Our results show that Corncrake males can use calls to discriminate between neighbours and strangers. The acoustic channel appears to be the most important sensory channel for this species because it allows communication at night, over long distances and in dense vegetation. In these circumstances, visual contact is virtually impossible. Therefore, we suppose that NSD by calls in the Corncrake may substantially reduce the costs of territory defence by avoiding unnecessary conflicts (Fisher 1954). However, the call characteristics used by males for NSD are still unknown. Whether females also use information about male identity (and, if so, how) remains to be determined.