Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Reading comprehension of monolingual and bilingual children in primary school: the role of linguistic abilities and phonological processing skills

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Reading comprehension in bilingual children depends on the extent to which each language is used in daily life. To date, most bilingual studies have focused on children who learn the majority language as their second language (L2 bilingual children). In contrast, bilingual children learning the majority language as their first language (L1 bilingual children) have rarely been addressed. To bridge this gap, this study explored (a) mean differences in reading comprehension and its preschool predictors as well as (b) differential associations between these variables for children from different language groups. The study included 1,842 monolingual, 269 L1 bilingual, and 237 L2 bilingual children from the German National Educational Panel Study who were assessed on their reading comprehension in grade 4 of elementary school. Preschool predictors of reading included linguistic abilities and phonological processing skills that were obtained in kindergarten. The results indicate that after accounting for the children’s socioeconomic background, L2 bilingual children exhibited lower reading comprehension and linguistic skills than L1 bilingual children, who in turn were outperformed by monolingual children. In contrast, phonological processing skills were comparable across groups. Furthermore, the three language groups presented similar relationships between reading comprehension and its preschool predictors.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aaron, P. G., Joshi, R. M., Gooden, R., & Bentum, K. E. (2008). Diagnosis and treatment of reading disabilities based on the component model of reading: An alternative to the discrepancy model of LD. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(1), 67–84. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219407310838

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. S. (2002). Reading, syntactiv, orthographic, and working memory skills of bilingual Arabic-English speaking Canadian children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(6), 661–678. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021221206119

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aguiar, C., Silva, C. S., Guerra, R., Rodrigues, R. B., Ribeiro, L. A., Pastori, G., & Leseman, P. (2020). Early interventions tackling inequalities experienced by immigrant, low-income, and Roma children in 8 European countries: A critical overview. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 28(1), 58–76. https://doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2020.1707363

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohm, T., Krohmer, K. & Gnambs, T. (2017). NEPS technical report for reading: Scaling results of starting cohort 2 for grade 4 (NEPS Survey Paper No. 30). Bamberg, Germany: Leibniz Institute for Educational Trajectories, National Educational Panel Study

  • Babayiğit, S. (2014). The role of oral language skills in reading and listening comprehension of text: A comparison of monolingual (L1) and bilingual (L2) speakers of English language. Journal of Research in Reading, 37(S1), S22–S47. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2012.01538.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 4(10), 829–839. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 47–89). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1

  • Bajo, M. T., Padilla, F., & Padilla, P. (2000). Comprehension processes in simultaneous interpreting. In A. Chesterman, N. G. S. Salvador, & Y. Gambier (Eds.), Translation in context (pp. 127–142). John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bellocchi, S., Tobia, V., & Bonifacci, P. (2017). Predictors of reading and comprehension abilities in bilingual and monolingual children: A longitudinal study on a transparent language. Reading and Writing, 30(6), 1311–1334. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9725-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berendes, K., Weinert, S., Zimmermann, S., & Artelt, C. (2013). Assessing language indicators across the lifespan within the German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS). Journal for Educational Research Online, 5(2), 15–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystock, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystock, E. (2013). The impact of bilingualism on language and literacy development. In T. K. Bhatia & W. C. Ritchie (Eds.), The handbook of bilingualism and multilingualism (pp. 624–648).

  • Bialystock, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(4), 859–873.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4). https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859

  • Birnbaum, A. L. (1968). Some latent trait models and their use in inferring an examinee’s ability. In Lord, F. M. & Novick, M. R. (eds.), Statistical theories of mental test scores (pp. 397–479). Reading: Addison-Wesley.

  • Blom, E., Küntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 105–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.06.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blossfeld, H. P., Roßbach, H.-G., & Maurice, J. von (Eds.) (2011). Education as a lifelong process – The German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS) [Special issue]. Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft. (Sonderheft 14).

  • Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 371–399. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Six theories of child development: Revised formulations and current issues (pp. 187–249). Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E., MacLean, M., Bradley, L. L., & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection, and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 429–438. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.3.429

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R. P. (1994). Percentage of unknown vocabulary words in text as a function of the relative difficulty of the text: Implications for instruction. Journal of Reading Behavior, 26(4), 413–437. https://doi.org/10.1080/10862969409547861

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., & Adlof, S. M. (2005). Developmental changes in reading and reading disabilities. In H. W. Catts & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), The connections between language and reading disabilities (pp. 23–36). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chiappe, P., Siegel, L. S., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2002). Linguistic diversity and the development of reading skills: A longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6(4), 369–400. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0604_04

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coddington, C. H., Mistry, R. S., & Bailey, A. L. (2014). Socioeconomic status and receptive vocabulary development: Replication of the parental investment model with Chilean preschoolers and their families. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 538–549. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.06.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., Bunting, M. F., Therriault, D. J., & Minkoff, S. R. B. (2002). A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30(2), 163–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00096-4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Da Fontoura, H. A., & Siegel, L. S. (1995). Reading, syntactic, and working memory skills of bilingual Portuguese-English Canadian children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 139–153. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01026951

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, B., Proctor, C. P., Uccelli, P., Mo, E., & Snow, C. E. (2011). Designing for diversity: The role of reading strategies and interactive vocabulary in a digital reading environment for fifth-grade monolingual English and bilingual students. Journal of Literacy Research, 43(1), 68–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/1086296X10397872

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Davier, M., Gonzalez, E., & Mislevy, R. (2009). What are plausible values and why are they useful? In M. von Davier & D. Hastedt (Eds.), IERI Monograph Series: Issues and Methodologies in Large Scale Assessments 2 (pp. 90–36). IER Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doyle, O., McEntee, L., & McNamara, K. A. (2012). Skills, capabilities and inequalities at school entry in a disadvantaged community. European Journal of Psychology Education, 27, 133–154. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-011-0072-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Droop, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2003). Language proficiency and reading ability in first- and second-language learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 38(1), 78–103. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.38.1.4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dubowy, M., Duzy, D., Pröscholdt, M. V., Schneider, W., Souvignier, E., & Gold, A. (2011). Was macht den “Migrationshintergrund” bei Vorschulkindern aus? Ein Vergleich alternativer Klassifikationskriterien und ihr Zusammenhang mit deutschen Sprachkompetenzen. Schweizerische Zeitschrift Für Bildungswissenschaften, 33, 355–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dubowy, M., Ebert, S., von Maurice, J. , & Weinert, S. (2008). Sprachlich-kognitive Kompetenzen beim Eintritt in den Kindergarten. Ein Vergleich von Kindern mit und ohne Migrationshintergrund. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 40(3), 124–134.

  • Dufva, M., Niemi, P., & Voeten, M. J. M. (2001). The role of phonological memory, word recognition, and comprehension skills in reading development: From preschool to grade 2. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 91–117. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008186801932

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M. (2004). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (German version). Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duzy, D., Gold, A., Schneider, W., & Souvignier, E. (2013). Die Prädiktion von Leseleistungen bei türkisch-deutschsprachigen Kindern: Die Rolle der phonologischen Bewusstheit. Zeitschrift Für Pädagogische Psychologie, 27(1–2), 41–50. https://doi.org/10.1024/1010-0652/a000087

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ebert, S., & Weinert, S. (2013). Predicting reading literacy in primary school: The contribution of various language indicators in preschool. In M. Pfost, C. Artelt, & S. Weinert (Eds.), The development of reading literacy from early childhood to adolescence. Empirical findings from the Bamberg BiKS longitudinal studies (pp. 93–149). Bamberg: University of Bamberg Press.

  • Engel de Abreu, P. M. J., Gathercole, S. E., & Martin, R. (2011). Disentangling the relationship between working memory and language: The roles of short-term storage and cognitive control. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 569–574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.06.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.128.3.309

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eviatar, Z., & Ibrahim, R. (2000). Bilingual is as bilingual does: Metalinguistic abilities of Arabic-speaking children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(4), 451–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Federal Statistical Office. (2010). Bevölkerung und Erwerbstätigkeit. Bevölkerung mit Migrationshintergrund. Ergebnisse des Mikrozensus 2009. (Population and employment. The population with immigrant back- ground. Results of the Microcensus 2009) [Press release]. Wiesbaden. Retrieved from http://www.intercultus.de/pdf-daten/destatis_2009.pdf

  • Fox, A. V. (2006). TROG-D Test zur Überprüfung des Grammatikverständnisses. Schulz-Kirchner Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D. A. (2006). On the so-called “Huber sandwich estimator” and “robust standard errors.” American Statistician, 60,. https://doi.org/10.1198/000313006X152207

  • Fricke, S., & Schäfer, B. (2008). Test für Phonologische Bewusstheitsfähigkeiten (TPB). Schulz-Kirchner Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganzeboom, H. B. G. (2010). A new international socio-economic index (ISEI) of occupational status for the international standard classification of occupation 2008 (ISCO-08) constructed with data from the ISSP 2002–2007. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of International Social Survey Programme, Lisbon.

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90044-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gehrer, K., Zimmermann, S., Artelt, C., & Weinert, S. (2013). NEPS framework for assessing reading competence and results from adult pilot study. Journal for Educational Research Online, 5(2), 50–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haman, E., Wodniecka, Z., Marecka, M., Szewczyk, J., Białecka-Pikul, M., Otwinowska, A., & Foryś-Nogala, M. (2017). How does L1 and L2 exposure impact L1 performance in bilingual children? Evidence from Polish-English migrants to the United Kingdom. Frontiers in Psychology, 8,. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01444

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H Brookes Publishing.

  • Hesse, H.-G., Göbel, K., & Hartig, J. (2008). Sprachliche Kompetenzen von mehrsprachigen Jugendlichen und Jugendlichen nicht-deutscher Erstsprache. In E. Klieme (Ed.), Unterricht und Kompetenzerwerb in Deutsch und Englisch. Ergebnisse der DESI-Studie (pp. 208–230). Weinheim: Beltz.

  • Hoff, E. (2015). Language development in bilingual children. In E. Bavin & L. Naigles (Eds.), The Cambridge Book of Child Language (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2(2), 127–160. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00401799

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jared, D. (2015). Literacy and literacy development in bilinguals. In A. Pollatsek & R. Treiman (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Reading (pp. 165–184). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199324576.013.28

  • de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 450–476. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.450

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kambel, E.-R. (2014). Language support for migrant children in early school years: Mapping European policies. Amsterdam.

  • Kieffer, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status, English proficiency, and late-emerging reading difficulties. Educational Researcher, 39(6), 484–486. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X10378400

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kovelman, I., Baker, S. A., & Petitto, L. A. (2008). Age of first bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development. Bilingualism, 11(2), 203–223. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728908003386

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lervåg, A., & Aukrust, V. G. (2010). Vocabulary knowledge is a critical determinant of the difference in reading comprehension growth between first and second language learners. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(5), 612–620. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02185.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Limbird, C. K., & Stanat, P. (2006). Prädiktoren von Leseverständnis bei Kindern deutscher und türkischer Herkunftssprache: Ergebnisse eine Längsschnittstudie. In A. Ittel (Ed.), Veränderungsmessung und Längsschnittstudien in der empirischen Erziehungswissenschaft (1st ed., pp. 91–123). VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften/GWV Fachverlage GmbH Wiesbaden.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. J., Anthony, J. L., Phillips, B. M., Purpura, D. J., Wilson, S. B., & McQueen, J. D. (2009). The nature of preschool phonological processing abilities and their relations to vocabulary, general cognitive abilities, and print knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2), 345–358. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013837

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, C., Berendes, K., & Weinert, S. (2017). Measuring receptive grammar in kindergarten and elementary school children in the National Educational Panel Study (NEPS Survey Paper No. 24). Bamberg.

  • Marx, A., Stanat, P., Roick, T., Segerer, R., Marx, P., & Schneider, W. (2015). Components of reading comprehension in adolescent first-language and second-language students from low-track schools. Reading and Writing, 28(6), 891–914. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9554-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McElvany, N., Becker, M., & Lüdtke, O. (2009). Die Bedeutung familiärer Merkmale für Lesekompetenz, Wortschatz, Lesemotivation und Leseverhalten. Zeitschrift Für Entwicklungspsychologie Und Pädagogische Psychologie, 41(3), 121–131. https://doi.org/10.1026/0049-8637.41.3.121

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melchers, P., & Preuß, U. (2009). Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC). Pearson Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 187–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., & Diethelm, K. (2001). The contribution of phonological skills and letter knowledge to early reading development in a multilingual population. Language Learning, 51(2), 187–219. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9922.00153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 665–681. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.665

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., & Herman, P. A. (1985). Incidental vs. instructional approaches to increasing reading vocabulary. Educational Perspectives, 23, 16–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., García, G. E., Durgunoğlu, A. Y., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. (1993). Spanish-English bilingual students’ use of cognates in English reading. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25(3), 241–259. https://doi.org/10.1080/10862969009547816

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Namazi, M., & Thordardottir, E. (2010). A working memory, not bilingual advantage, in controlled attention. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13(5), 597–616. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2010.488288

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naumann, J., Cordula, A., Schneider, W., & Stanat, P. (2010). Lesekompetenz von PISA 2000 bis PISA 2009. In E. Klieme, C. Artelt, J. Hartig, N. Jude, O. Köller, M. Prenzel,. . . P. Stanat (Eds.), PISA 2009: Bilanz nach einem Jahrzehnt (pp. 23–71). Münster: Waxmann.

  • OECD (2011). PISA 2009 results: Overcoming social background: Equity in learning opportunities and outcomes (volume II) (1. Aufl.). PISA 2009 Results. Paris: OECD Publishing. Retrieved from http://gbv.eblib.com/patron/FullRecord.aspx?p=655760

  • OECD (2019). Germany Country Note PISA 2018 Results. Paris: OECD Publishing. Retrieved from https://www.oecd.org/pisa/publications/PISA2018_CN_DEU.pdf

  • Ordóñez, C. L., Carlo, M. S., Snow, C. E., & McLaughlin, B. (2002). Depth and breadth of vocabulary in two languages: Which vocabulary skills transfer? Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(4), 719–728. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.94.4.719

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18(1), 41–58. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400009863

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, C. P., Carlo, M., August, D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Native Spanish-speaking children reading in English: Toward a model of comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(2), 246–256. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.246

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, C. P., Silverman, R. D., Harring, J. R., & Montecillo, C. (2012). The role of vocabulary depth in predicting reading comprehension among English monolingual and Spanish-English bilingual children in elementary school. Reading and Writing, 25(7), 1635–1664. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9336-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, C. P., Uccelli, P., Dalton, B., & Snow, C. E. (2009). Understanding depth of vocabulary online with bilingual and monolingual children. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 25(4), 311–333. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573560903123502

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/

  • Rasch, G. (1960). Studies in mathematical psychology: I. Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. Studies in mathematical psychology: I. Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. Oxford, England: Nielsen & Lydiche.

  • Raudszus, H., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2018). Lexical quality and executive control predict children’s first and second language reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 31(2), 405–424. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9791-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, J., Jergens, J., & Burchinal, M. (2005). The role of home literacy practices in preschool children’s language and emergent literacy skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(2), 345–359. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/024)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roßbach, H. G., Tietze, W., & Weinert, S. (2005). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Revised. Deutsche Forschungsversion des Tests von L.M. Dunn & L.M. Dunn von 1981. Universität Bamberg, FU Berlin.

  • Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 1–36. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02

  • Rubin, H., & Turner, A. (1989). Linguistic awareness skills in grade one children in a French immersion setting. Reading and Writing, 1(1), 73–86. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00178839

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schleicher, A. (2019). PISA 2018: Insights and interpretations.

  • Schnepf, S. V. (2007). Immigrants’ educational disadvantage: An examination across ten countries and three surveys. Journal of Population Economics, 20(3), 527–545. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-006-0102-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segerer, R., Marx, A., Stanat, P., Schneider, W., Roick, T., & Marx, P. (2013). Determinanten der Lesekompetenz bei Jugendlichen nicht deutscher Herkunftssprache. Zur Bedeutung der Spracherwerbsreihenfolge im Falle von Mehrsprachigkeit. In N. Jude & E. Klieme (Eds.), PISA 2009 - Impulse für die Schul- und Unterrichtforschung (pp. 111–131). Weinhem: Beltz.

  • Sonnenschein, S., & Sun, S. (2016). Racial/ethnic differences in kindergartners’ reading and math skills: Parents’ knowledge of children’s development and home-based activities as mediators. Infant and Child Development, 26(5), e2010. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2010

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanat, P., Schipolowski, S., Rjosk, C., Weirich, S., & Haag, N. (Eds.) (2016). IQB trends in student achievement 2016: The second national assessment of German and Mathematics proficiencies at the end of fourth grade. Münster: Waxmann.

  • Stanovich, K. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.21.4.1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tewes, U., Rossmann, P., & Schallberger, U. (1999). Hamburg-Wehcsler-Intelligenztest für Kinder (HAWIK-III). Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tilstra, J., McMaster, K., van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., & Rapp, D. (2009). Simple but complex: Components of the simple view of reading across grade levels. Journal of Research in Reading, 32(4), 383–401. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2009.01401.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, R., & Gawlitzek-Maiwald, I. (2000). Bilingualismus in der frühen Kindheit. In H. Grimm (Ed.), Sprachentwicklung: Enzyklopädie der Psychologie. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

  • Verhoeven, L. (1994). Linguistic diversity and literacy development. In L. Verhoeven (Ed.), Functional literacy. Theoretical issues and educational implications (pp. 199–220). Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

  • Verhoeven, L. (2000). Components in early second language reading and spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 313–330. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0404_4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., & Garon, T. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33(3), 468–479. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.33.3.468

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warm, T. A. (1986). Weighted likelihood estimation of ability in item response theory. Psychometrika, 54, 427–450. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02294627

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, M. (2005). The role of plausible values in large-scale surveys. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 31, 114–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.stueduc.2005.05.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Würbach, A. (2018). Samples, weights and nonresponse: The Kindergarten Cohort of the National Educational Panel Study (Waves 1 to 6). Technical Report. Bamberg.

  • Yelland, G., Pollard, J., & Mercuri, A. (1993). The metalinguistic benefits of limited contact with a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14(4), 423–444. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400010687

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuan, K.-H., & Bentler, P. M. (2000). 5. Three likelihood-based methods for mean and covariance structure analysis with nonnormal missing data. Sociological Methodology, 30(1), 165–200. https://doi.org/10.1111/0081-1750.00078

  • Zhao, X., & Li, P. (2010). Bilingual lexical interactions in an unsupervised neural network model. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13(5), 505–524. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2010.488284

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This paper uses data from the National Educational Panel Study (NEPS): Starting Cohort Kindergarten, https://doi.org/10.5157/NEPS:SC2:8.0.1. From 2008 to 2013, NEPS data was collected as part of the Framework Program for the Promotion of Empirical Educational Research funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF). As of 2014, NEPS is carried out by the Leibniz Institute for Educational Trajectories (LIfBi) at the University of Bamberg in cooperation with a nationwide network.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shally Novita.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Shally Novita. Padjadjaran University, Jl. Raya Sumedang Jatinangor KM 21, 45,363 West Java, Indonesia. E-mail: shally.novita@hotmail.de.

Current themes of research

Shally Novita: Development of reading. Reading disabilities. Home learning environment. Parents’ and teachers’ judgments of cognitive competences.

Kathrin Lockl: Development of metacognition. Self-regulated learning. Relationship between language and metacognitive development. Competence development.

Timo Gnambs: Methods of large-scale assessment and survey research. Longitudinal measurement models. Meta-analysis. Technology-based psychological assessment.

Most relevant publications in the field of Psychology of Education

1. Shally Novita

Novita, S., Uyun, Q., Witruk, E. Siregar, J. (2019). Children with dyslexia in different cultures: Investigation of anxiety and coping strategies of children with dyslexia in Indonesia and Germany. Annals of Dyslexia 69, 204–218. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-019-00179-5

2. Kathrin Lockl

Freund, M.-J., Wolter, I., Lockl, K., & Gnambs, T. (2021). Determinants of profiles of competence development in mathematics and reading in upper secondary education in Germany. PLoS ONE, 16(10): e0258152. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258152

Weinert, S., Artelt, C., Prenzel, M., Senkbeil, M., Ehmke, T., Carstensen, C. H. & Lockl, K. (2019). Development of competencies across the life course. In H.-P. Blossfeld & H.-G. Roßbach (Eds.), Education as a Lifelong Process (pp. 57-81). Wiesbaden: Springer VS.

3. Timo Gnambs

Freund, M.-J., Wolter, I., Lockl, K., & Gnambs, T. (2021). Determinants of profiles of competence development in mathematics and reading in upper secondary education in Germany. PLoS ONE, 16(10): e0258152. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258152

Gnambs, T. (2021). The development of gender differences in information and communication technology (ICT) literacy in middle adolescence. Computers in Human Behavior, 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106533

Thums, K., Gnambs, T., & Wolter, I. (2020). The impact of gender-stereotypical text contents on reading competence in women and men. Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft, 23, 1283-1301. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11618-020-00980-8

Heyne, N., Artelt, C., Gnambs, T., Gehrer, K., & Schoor, C. (2020). Instructed highlighting of text passages – Indicator of reading or strategic performance?. Lingua, 236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2020.102803

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 24 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Novita, S., Lockl, K. & Gnambs, T. Reading comprehension of monolingual and bilingual children in primary school: the role of linguistic abilities and phonological processing skills. Eur J Psychol Educ 37, 993–1013 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-021-00587-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-021-00587-5

Keywords

Navigation