Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

How gender fair are German schoolbooks in the twenty-first century? An analysis of language and illustrations in schoolbooks for mathematics and German

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Schoolbooks convey not only school-relevant knowledge; they also influence the development of stereotypes about different social groups. Particularly during the 1970s and 1980s, many studies analysed schoolbooks and criticised the overall predominance of male persons and of traditional role allocations. Since that time, women’s and men’s occupations and social functions have changed considerably. The present research investigated gender portrayals in schoolbooks for German and mathematics that were recently published in Germany. We examined the proportions of female and male persons in pictures and texts and categorized their activities, occupational and parental roles. Going beyond previous studies, we added two criteria: the use of gender-fair language and the spatial arrangements of persons in pictures. Our results show that schoolbooks for German contained almost balanced depictions of girls and boys, whereas women were less frequently shown than men. In mathematics books, males outnumbered females in general. Across both types of books, female and male persons were engaged in many different activities, not only gender-typed ones; however, male persons were more often described via their profession than females. Use of gender-fair language has found its way into schoolbooks but is not used consistently. Books for German were more gender fair in terms of linguistic forms than books for mathematics. For spatial arrangements, we found no indication for gender biases. The results are discussed with a focus on how schoolbooks can be optimized to contribute to gender equality.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. http://www.bildungsserver.de/Zugelassene-Lernmittel-und-Schulbuecher-522.html

  2. http://www.gesetze-im-internet.de/bgleig/

  3. www.random.org

  4. Overall chi2 tests comparing frequencies of the genders across the two school subjects were insignificant for human characters (since three out of the four single comparisons were in the direction of males predominating over females: see Fig. 1); χ 2 (1, N = 1,193) = 0.360, p = 0.55 and significant for non-human characters: χ 2 (1, N = 82) = 4.94, p = 0.03, φ = 0.25.

References

  • Abele, A. (2003). The dynamics of masculine-agentic and feminine-communal traits: findings from a prospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 768–776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L., & Bem, D. J. (1973). Does sex-biased job advertising “aid and abet” sex discrimination? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 3, 6–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, J. E. O., Berenbaum, S. A., & Liben, L. S. (2009). Gender development. New York: Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers' conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braun. F. (2000). Mehr Frauen in die Sprache. Leitfaden zur geschlechtergerechten Formulierung. [More women into language. Guideline for gender-fair formulation]. Ministerium für Justiz, Frauen, Jugend und Familie des Landes Schleswig-Holstein. http://www.berlin.de/imperia/md/content/sen-frauen/sprache.pdf?start&ts=1188881015&file=sprache.pdf. Accessed 4 December 2011.

  • Braun, F., Sczesny, S., & Stahlberg, D. (2005). Cognitive effects of masculine generics in German: an overview of empirical findings. Communications, 30, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bühlmann, M. (2009). Geschlechterrollenstereotype in Lesebüchern. Eine quantitative Inhaltsanalyse von Schulbuchtexten aus drei Generationen von Schweizer Lesebüchern. [Gender stereotypes in textbooks. A quantitative content analysis of three generations of Swiss schoolbooks]. Swiss Journal of Sociology, 35, 593–619.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bussmann, H., & Hellinger, M. (2003). Engendering female visibility in German. In M. Hellinger & H. Bussmann (Eds.), Gender across languages: the linguistic representation of women and men (Vol. 3, pp. 141–174). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chatterjee, A. (2002). Portrait profiles and the notion of agency. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 20, 33–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colley, A., & Comber, C. (2003). Age and gender differences in computer use and attitudes among secondary school students: What has changed? Educational Research, 45(2), 155–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demes, B. (1984). Geschlechtsrollendarstellung in Biologieschulbüchern der Sekundarstufen I & II. [Portrayal of gender roles in biology books for secondary class levels I & II. In I. Brehmer & U. Enders-Dragässer (Eds.), Die Schule lebt—Frauen bewegen die Schule [School is alive—women set school in motion (pp. 213–224). Munich: DJI Deutsches Jugendinstitut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: women and men of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social Psychological Bulletin, 26, 1171–1188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diekman, A. B., & Murnen, S. K. (2004). Learning to be little women and little men: the inequitable gender equality of nonsexist children’s literature. Sex Roles, 50, 373–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dougherty, W. H., & Engel, R. E. (1987). An 80s look for sex equality in Caldecott winners and Honor Books. The Reading Teacher, 40, 394–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbracht, M., & Mosler, I. (1972). Femina non est homo? Zum Frauenbild in Grund- und Hauptschullesebüchern. [Femina non est homo? Images of women in elementary schoolbooks]. In P. Braun (Ed.), Neue Lesebücher—Analyse und Kritik. [New readers—analysis and critique] (pp. 82–96). Düsseldorf: Verlagsgruppe Bertelsmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, L., & Davies, K. (2000). No sissy boys here: a content analyses of the representation of masculinity in elementary school reading books. Sex Roles, 41, 255–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fichera, U. (1996). Die Schulbuchdiskussion in der BRD—Beiträge zur Neugestaltung des Geschlechterverhältnisses. Bestandsaufnahme und Sekundäranalyse. [The discussion on schoolbooks in the FRG—contributions on the rearrangement of gender relations. State of the art and secondary analysis]. Frankfurt: Lang.

  • Finsterwald, M., & Ziegler, A. (2007). Geschlechtsrollenstereotype in Schulbuchabbildungen der Grundschule. [Gender role stereotypes in schoolbook illustrations for elementary school]. In P. Ludwig & H. Ludwig (Eds.), Erwartungen in Himmelblau und rosarot. Effekte, Determinanten und Konsequenzen von Geschlechterdifferenzen in der Schule [Expectations in baby-blue and pink. Effects, determinants, and consequences of gender differences at school] (pp. 117–141). Weinheim: Juventa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisk, W. R. (1985). Responses to “neutral” pronoun presentations and the development of sex-biased responding. Developmental Psychology, 21, 481–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gabriel, U., & Gygax, P. (2008). Can language amendments change gender representation? The case of Norway. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49, 451–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glötzner, J. (1982). Ist ein mathematisches Weib wider der Natur? Heidi häkelt Quadrate, Thomas erklärt die Multiplikation. Rollenklischees in neuen Mathematikbüchern. [Does a mathematical female contradict nature? Heidi crochets squares and Thomas explains multiplication. Clichés in recent mathematics books]. In I. Brehmer (Ed.), Sexismus in der Schule. Der heimliche Lehrplan der Frauendiskriminierung [Sexism at school. The hidden curriculum of women’s discrimination] (pp. 150–158). Weinheim: Beltz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grauel, G. (1973). Rollenbilder in Fibeln—Barrieren für den politischen Unterricht. [Role models in early readers—barriers to political education]. In G. Beck & W. Hilligen (Eds.), Politische Bildung ohne Fundament. Untersuchungen zu Richtlinien, Fibeln, Lehrerhandbücher für den Unterricht in der Grundschule. [Political education without a foundation. Examination of guidelines, early readers, teachers’ manuals for teaching in elementary school] (pp. 109–147). Neuwied: Luchterhand.

  • Hannover, B., & Kessels, U. (2002). Challenge the science-stereotype! Der Einfluss von Technikfreizeitkursen auf das Naturwissenschaften-Stereotyp von Schülerinnen und Schülern. [Challenge the science stereotype! The influence of extracurricular technical courses on pupils’ stereotype of natural science]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, Beiheft, 45, 341–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannover, B., & Kessels, U. (2004). Why German school students don’t like math and sciences. A self-to-prototype matching approach. Learning and Instruction, 14, 51–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hellinger, M. (1980). For men must work and women must weep: sexism in English language textbooks used in German schools. Women‘s Studies International Quarterly, 3, 267–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunze, A. (2003). Geschlechtertypisierung in Schulbüchern [Gender stereotyping in schoolbooks]. In M. Stürzer, H. Roisch, W. Cornelissen, & A. Hunze (Eds.), Geschlechterverhältnisse in der Schule [Gender relations at school] (pp. 49–81). Opladen: Leske & Budrich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irmen, L., & Rossberg, N. (2004). Gender markedness of language. The impact of grammatical and nonlinguistic information on the mental representation of person information. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 23, 272–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karsten, G. (1977). Mariechens Weg ins Glück? Die Diskriminierung von Mädchen in Grundschullesebüchern. [Little Mary’s way to happiness? The discrimination of girls in readers for elementary school]. Berlin: Frauenselbstverlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessels, U., & Hannover, B. (2007). How the image of math and science affects the development of academic interests. In M. Prenzel (Ed.), Studies on the educational quality of schools (pp. 283–297). Münster: Waxmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindner, V., & Lukesch, H. (1994). Geschlechtsrollenstereotype im deutschen Schulbuch. Eine inhaltsanalytische Untersuchung von Schulbüchern für Grund-, Haupt- und Realschulen der Fächer Deutsch, Mathematik, Heimat- und Sachkunde sowie Religionslehre in Bayern, Nordrhein-Westfalen und der ehemaligen DDR zugelassen im Zeitraum von 1970 bis 1992. [Gender stereotypes in German schoolbooks. A content analysis of schoolbooks for elementary, lower secondary, and middle school for German, mathematics, general studies and religion in Bavaria, North-Rhine Westphalia and the former German Democratic Republic approved between 1970 and 1992]. Regensburg: Roderer.

  • Maass, A., Suitner, C., Favaretto, X., & Cignacchi, M. (2009). Groups in space: stereotypes and the spatial agency bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 496–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationships. A developmental account. American Psychologist, 45, 513–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markom, C., & Weinhäupl, H. (2007). Die anderen im Schulbuch. Rassismen, Exotismen, Sexismen und Antisemitismus in österreichischen Schulbüchern. [The others in schoolbooks. Racism, exocitism, sexism, and anti-semitism in Austrian schoolbooks]. Vienna: Braumüller.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues: cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 67–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MAXQDA (2011). Software für qualitative Datenanalyse, 19892013 [Software for qualitative analyses of data], VERBI Software. Berlin: Consult, Sozialforschung GmbH.

  • McCabe, J., Fairchild, E., Grauerholz, L., Pescosolido, B. A., & Tope, D. (2011). Gender in twentieth-century children’s books: patterns of disparity in titles and central characters. Gender & Society, 25, 197–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B., & Jordan, E. (1984). Gutachten über die Darstellung von Mädchen und Frauen in Hamburger Schulbüchern [Expertise on the portrayal of girls and women in schoolbooks for Hamburg]. Hamburg: Leitstelle Gleichstellung der Frau.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mucchi-Faina, A. (2005). Visible or influential? Language reforms and gender (in)equality. Social Science Information, 44, 189–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohlms, U. (1984). Und drinnen waltet die züchtige Hausfrau…” Das Mädchen- und Frauenbild in Grundschulbüchern. [“And inside rules the modest housewife…” The portrayal of girls and women in elementary schoolbooks]. In I. Brehmer & U. Enders-Dragässer (Eds.), Die Schule lebt - Frauen bewegen die Schule [School is alive—women set school in motion (pp. 131–161). Munich: DJI Deutsches Jugendinstitut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preinsberger, A., & Weisskircher, E. (1997). Mathematikschulbücher - eine aktuelle Untersuchung [Mathematics books – a recent examination]. In L. Lassnigg & A. Paseka (Eds.), Schule weiblich—Schule männlich. Zum Geschlechterverhältnis im Bildungswesen [Female school—male school. On gender relations in the educational system] (pp. 132–143). Innsbruck: Studien.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silbermann, A., & Krüger, U. M. (1971). Abseits der Wirklichkeit. Das Frauenbild in deutschen Lesebüchern. Eine soziologische Untersuchung. [Beyond reality. The image of women in German readers. A sociological investigation]. Cologne: Verlag Wissenschaft und Politik.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schröter, H. (2002). Geschichte ohne Frauen? Das Frauenbild in den Schulgeschichtsbüchern der BRD und DDR von 1949 bis 1989. [History without women? The image of women in history books from the FRG and the GDR 1949 to 1989]. Frankfurt: Verlag der deutschen Hochschulschriften.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweizerische Bundeskanzlei, (2009). Geschlechtergerechte Sprache. Leitfaden zum geschlechtergerechten Formulieren im Deutschen. [Gender-fair language. Guideline for gender-fair formulation in German]. http://www.bk.admin.ch/dokumentation/sprachen/04915/05313/index.html. Accessed 5 June 2010

  • Stahlberg, D., Braun, F., Irmen, L., & Sczesny, S. (2007). Representation of the sexes in language. In K. Fiedler (Ed.), Social communication. A volume in the series frontiers of social psychology (pp. 163–187). New York: Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stout, J. G., & Dasgupta, N. (2011). When he doesn’t mean you: gender-exclusive language as ostracism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 3, 757–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suitner, C., & Maass, A. (2011). Writing direction, agency and gender stereotyping: an embodied connection. In T. W. Schubert & A. Maass (Eds.), Spatial dimensions of social thought (pp. 303–323). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Switzer, J. Y. (1990). The impact of generic word choices: an empirical investigation of age- and sex-related differences. Sex Roles, 22, 69–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trautner, H. M., Ruble, D. N., Cyphers, L., Kirsten, B., Behrendt, R., & Hartmann, P. (2005). Rigidity and flexibility of gender stereotypes in childhood development: developmental or differential? Infant and Child Development, 14, 365–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • UNDP (2011). Gender inequality index. http://hdr.undp.org/en/statistics/gii/. Accessed 6 January 2013.

  • Vervecken, D., Hannover, B., & Wolter, I. (2013). Changing (s)expectations: how gender-fair job descriptions impact children’s perceptions and interest regarding traditionally male occupations. Journal of Vocational Behavior. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2013.01.008.

    Google Scholar 

  • World Economic Forum (2012). The global gender gap report. http://www3.weforum.org/docs/WEF_GenderGap_Report_2012.pdf. Accessed 6 January 2013.

  • Weitzman, L. J., Eifler, D., Hokada, E., & Ross, C. (1972). Sex role socialization in picture books for preschool children. The American Journal of Sociology, 77, 1125–1150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. E., & Best, D. L. (1982). Measuring sex stereotypes: a thirty nation study. Berkeley: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Thanks go to Michael Schulte, Katharina Marquardt, Mario Datts, Sara Matthies and Joanna Schiweck for their help in conducting this research and to Friederike Braun for her comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. This research was conducted within the Marie Curie Initial Training Network: Language, Cognition, & Gender, ITN LCG, funded by the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under grant agreement no. 237907 (www.itn-lcg.eu).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Franziska Moser.

Additional information

Franziska Moser. Freie Universität Berlin, Department of Educational Science and Psychology, Habelschwerdter Allee 45, 14195 Berlin, Germany. E-mail: franziska.moser@fu-berlin.de; Web site: http://www.ewi-psy.fu-berlin.de/en/einrichtungen/arbeitsbereiche/ewi-psy/mitarbeiter_innen/Moser_F/index.html. She was a research fellow of the Marie Curie Initial Training Network—Language, Cognition, and Gender (ITN LCG) at the Freie Universität Berlin.

Current themes of research:

Gender equality. Gender-fair language. Development of gender roles. Attitude–behaviour relations with respect to language use.

Prof. Bettina Hannover. Freie Universität Berlin, Department of Educational Science and Psychology, Habelschwerdter Allee 45, 14195 Berlin, Germany. E-mail: bettina.hannover@fu-berlin.de; Web site: http://www.ewi-psy.fu-berlin.de/en/einrichtungen/arbeitsbereiche/ewi-psy/mitarbeiter_innen/Hannover_B/index.html. She is heading a research group at the Freie Universität Berlin, whose program focuses on the mechanisms underlying the development of gender differences in interests, skills and occupational careers and on measures aiming at their attenuation. She has combined the pursuit of basic and applied issues from social and educational psychology, in order to gain a better understanding of the self, as it relates to gender identity and gender-typed behaviours.

Current themes of research:

Aspects of education (e.g. sex differences in social beliefs; gender-fair instruction). Gender stereotypes. Training and teaching techniques. Observational methods. Behavioural measures. The conduct of experimental field trials. The development and evaluation of trainings.

Most relevant publications in the field of Psychology of Education:

Vervecken, D. & Hannover, B. (2012). Ambassadors of gender equality? How use of pair forms versus masculines as generics impacts perception of the speaker. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 754–762.

Kessels, U. & Hannover, B. (2008). When being a girl matters less. Accessibility of gender-related self-knowledge in single-sex and coeducational classes and its impact on students' physics related self-concept of ability. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 273–289.

Hannover, B. & Kessels, U. (2004). Why German school students don’t like math and sciences. A self-to-prototype matching approach. Learning and Instruction, 14, 51–67.

Hannover, B. (2000). Development of the self in gendered contexts. In Th. Eckes & H.M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 177–206). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Appendix

Appendix

Gender composition of groups and dyads

To test whether the critique of earlier studies that female and male persons are mostly shown in interactions with same-gender others (e.g. Lindner and Lukesch 1994) still applies, we examined the gender composition of groups and dyads. We differentiated three codes: A person was part of either a group of three or more, of a dyad (e.g. girl–boy, boy–man) or was alone.

Cohen's Kappas for the codings of gender compositions were high (books for German: Cohen’s Kappatext = .88, Kappapicture = 0.97; books for mathematics: Kappatext = 0.96, Kappapicture = 0.89). Table 2 shows that in books for German, girls and boys were typically depicted in groups or dyads and rarely alone. Overall, we found mostly mixed-gender group and dyadic constellations. Girls and boys were significantly more often depicted in mixed-gender groups or dyads than in same-gender groups or dyads; girls: χ 2 (1, N = 99) = 16.98, p < 0.001; boys: χ 2 (1, N = 101) = 8.33, p = 0.005. Similarly, women and men were more often depicted in mixed-gender than in same-gender constellations; this difference was, however, not significant, χ 2 (1, N = 24) = 1.50, p = 0.31.

Table 3 shows the depictions of female and male persons in mathematics books. Girls and boys were mostly shown in groups or dyads and seldom alone. Again girls and boys appeared more often in mixed-gender groups/dyads than in same-gender groups/dyads; girls: χ 2 (1, N = 80) = 54.45, p < 0.001; boys: χ 2 (1, N = 79) = 38.29, p < 0.001. In contrast, women and men were shown equally often in mixed-gender and same-gender constellations.

To summarize, our analyses revealed that children/adolescents (but not adults) were significantly more often depicted in mixed-gender than in same-gender compositions, in both books for German and for mathematics. This clearly contradicts the situation in the real world. As studies on interpersonal relations, friendships and peer choices showed, children aged 3 up to about 11 years have a strong preference for same-gender others (cf. Blakemore et al. 2009; Maccoby 1990). An explanation might be that schoolbook producers try to make illustrations and texts equally appealing to both male and female students, by disregarding real word differences in the frequencies of same-gender and mixed-gender groups.

Locations

Locations were criticised by earlier studies, showing that girls are mainly depicted indoors and boys mainly outdoors (e.g. Silbermann and Krüger 1971). We therefore differentiated whether the characters were performing their activities indoors or outdoors. These codes could be combined with codes for specific locations such as school, work or houses/rooms.

Cohen's Kappas for the codings of locations were good (books for German: Kappatext = 0.77, Kappapicture = 0.86; books for mathematics: Kappatext = 0.87, Kappapicture = 0.83). The coding resulted in a very high percentage of uncodable information: Many of the backgrounds in pictures which might have contained location information were blank (e.g. white background), and many texts did not contain any reference to location (books for German: 209 (47 %) locations out of 445 not codable; books for mathematics: 368 (70.4 %) locations out of 523 not codable). The remaining locations were analysed for indoor/outdoor information. In general, in books for German, the characters were significantly more often depicted in an outdoor than in an indoor location, χ 2 (1, N = 353) = 16.50, p < .001. In both school subjects, gender was independent of whether the person was depicted in an indoor or an outdoor location (books for German: χ 2 (1, N = 214) = 0.14, p = 0.71; books for mathematics: χ 2 (1, N = 141) = 0.62, p = 0.43).

To summarize, our hypothesis that gender might be systematically related to the locations in which characters are shown was not confirmed. Possibly, blank backgrounds and unidentifiable locations are chosen intentionally by illustrators and authors of schoolbooks, trying to avoid the gender biases found in earlier studies where females were often shown inside a house and males outdoors (cf. Lindner and Lukesch 1994).

Secondary texts

Secondary texts were analysed to see whether schoolbook producers choose female and male authors to the same extent and whether texts of these authors include a balanced number of male and female main characters. Only three secondary texts were found in mathematics books, and in each of them, animals were the main characters. We subdivided the secondary texts into older (produced before the 1940s) and recent (from 1940s upwards) ones and coded persons, if present, for their gender and the authors of these texts.

We found a preponderance of male authors for older texts (19 male compared to two female authors and two not specified; χ 2 (1, N = 21) = 13.76, p < 0.001). For recent secondary texts, we found a balanced number of female and male authors (18 female and 18 male authors). With respect to the persons the secondary texts were about, we found a preponderance of male protagonists for all texts, χ 2 (1, N = 68) = 5.88, p = 0.02.

Two things are noticeable about these results. First, in older secondary texts, male authors clearly dominated. It seems that female authors are rarely considered when selecting secondary texts for schoolbooks, a finding which is in line with the underrepresentation of female historic personalities in history schoolbooks (cf. Schröter 2002). Second, although the number of female and male authors was balanced for recent secondary texts, characters appearing in these texts were still predominantly male. Although the majority of contributors (authors, illustrators, editors) to both books for German and mathematics were female, they were more inclined to choose secondary texts of male authors and with male protagonists. With respect to gender fairness, it would be preferable to include more secondary literature written by women in former times and with female protagonists.

Table 2 Frequencies (and percentages) of groups varying in their gender composition in books for German
Table 3 Frequencies (and percentages) of groups varying in their gender composition in mathematics books
Table 4 Frequencies of the ten most frequent activities in books for German
Table 5 Frequencies of the ten most frequent activities in mathematics books
Table 6 Gender comparisons of frequencies of main activities in books for German and mathematics
Table 7 Frequencies of language forms (percentages) split by school subject, school year and reference to groups vs. individuals

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Moser, F., Hannover, B. How gender fair are German schoolbooks in the twenty-first century? An analysis of language and illustrations in schoolbooks for mathematics and German. Eur J Psychol Educ 29, 387–407 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-013-0204-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-013-0204-3

Keywords

Navigation